Next Article in Journal
When Control Exacerbates Distress: A Qualitative Study Exploring the Experiences of Hong Kong Chinese Parents in Caring for a Child with Asthma
Previous Article in Journal
Physical Activity and Physical Fitness of Adults with Intellectual Disabilities in Group Homes in Hong Kong
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Is Nature Relatedness Associated with Better Mental and Physical Health?

1
Faculty of Medicine, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4006, Australia
2
School of Biological Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia
3
Zealandia, 31 Waiapu Road, Karori, WLG 6012, New Zealand
4
Environment & Sustainability Institute, University of Exeter, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9EZ, UK
5
CSIRO Land & Water Flagship, PMB 1, 107-121 Station Street, Aspendale, VIC 3195, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15(7), 1371; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15071371
Submission received: 7 June 2018 / Revised: 21 June 2018 / Accepted: 26 June 2018 / Published: 29 June 2018
(This article belongs to the Section Health Behavior, Chronic Disease and Health Promotion)

Abstract

:
Nature relatedness is a psychological characteristic with the potential to drive interaction with nature and influence well-being. We surveyed 1538 people in Brisbane, Australia to investigate how nature relatedness varies among socio-demographic groups. We determined whether people with higher nature relatedness reported fewer symptoms of depression, anxiety, stress and better overall health, controlling for potentially confounding socio-demographic and health-related variables. Overall nature relatedness was higher in older people, females, those without children living at home, not working, and people speaking English at home. Aspects of nature relatedness reflecting enjoyment of nature were consistently associated with reduced ill health, consistent with widespread evidence of the health and well-being benefits of experiencing nature. In contrast, aspects of nature relatedness reflecting self-identification with nature, and a conservation worldview, were associated with increased depression, anxiety or stress, after accounting for potential confounding factors. Detailed investigation of causal pathways among nature relatedness, socio-demographic factors and health is warranted, with particular focus on the relationship between stress and nature orientation.

1. Introduction

Human society has become increasingly urbanized over the past century with more than half of the world’s population now living in towns and cities [1]. As a result, many people now have fewer opportunities to interact with nature, defined as landscapes with living organisms, including plants and non-human animals [2]. There is mounting concern that this ‘extinction of experience’ of nature [3] has led to nature becoming less relevant in people’s lives as well as having negative impacts on human health and well-being [3,4,5,6,7,8]. The concept of nature relatedness—individual differences in the way people view the natural world and their connection with nature—has become the focus of increasing attention as an individual-level characteristic that may drive interaction with nature and influence well-being [2,9,10,11]. Yet, there is limited empirical data on the connection between nature relatedness and mental and physical ill health. This paper contributes to knowledge about the psychological construct of nature relatedness through: (1) examining differences in nature relatedness in people from different socio-demographic backgrounds; (2) investigating the relationships between nature relatedness and mental and physical ill health when accounting for socio-demographic characteristics and other key health-related variables; and (3) systematically considering these relationships across multiple dimensions of nature relatedness reflecting affective, cognitive and experiential aspects.

1.1. Benefits from Contact with Nature

There is well documented evidence that when humans interact with nature they tend to experience benefits, including physiological and psychological stress reduction [12,13,14,15], increases in well-being [16,17,18]; vitality [19], restored capacity to direct attention to relevant tasks in daily life [20], ability to reflect on a life problem [18] and decreased rumination and neural activity in an area of the brain associated with behavioral withdrawal [21]. Places with natural features can provide a setting for behaviors that enhance health and well-being, such as physical activity [22,23] and social contact [24,25,26]. Indeed, a number of these pathways may be simultaneously activated when a person has contact with a natural setting [8,27].
There is immense potential to develop effective public health interventions that harness people’s interactions with, and orientations to, urban nature as a pathway to promote health [28]. Globally, the leading cause of years of life lost is chronic disease associated with urbanization and sedentary lifestyles [29]. The leading cause of non-fatal burden of disease is mental ill health [30] and there is high co-morbidity for chronic disease and mental ill health [31]. The health benefits of interacting with urban nature—including promoting physical activity, psychological restoration, reduced stress and enhanced social connection—have the potential to reduce the burden of the major non-communicable diseases, such as cardiovascular diseases and some forms of diabetes, that are prevalent in high and middle income countries [32]. Further, careful design and provision of green space holds promise as one approach to reducing ill health at the population level [33].

1.2. The Role of Nature Relatedness in Enhancing Health

Across centuries and societies the connections between nature and human well-being have formed part of the cultural fabric in stories, art, poetry and other traditional practices [34]. Recent theorizing has sought to explain why people experience positive emotions when in contact with natural features. The two main explanatory frameworks are stress reduction theory and attention restoration theory. Stress reduction theory, located within psycho-evolutionary theory, posits that when individuals are in contact with natural elements that support survival (e.g., water, food etc.) they experience a psychophysiological response involving a decrease in physiological arousal, reduced negative affect and increased positive affect [35]. Attention restoration theory proposes that contact with nature helps replenish ‘directed attention’—a form of focused concentration that can become depleted in urban environments—through unconscious cognitive processes [36]. Several related explanations have emerged from these frameworks, for example positive responses to nature contact being related to ‘processing fluency’ whereby the visual structure of nature settings requires fewer cognitive resources than for urban settings [37] and the ‘topophilia hypothesis’ in which the co-evolution of innate behavior and cultural learning fosters connection with nature through attachment to place [38]. Psychological theories of identity have posited the connection between well-being and nature through the concept of ‘ecological identity’ or sense of ‘ecological self’, experienced when one’s sense of self is expanded to include not just other humans but other living species and ecosystems [39,40]. Ecological philosopher Arne Naess proposed that an expanded sense of oneself as in a reciprocal relationship with other living species and ecological systems, rather than a more independent constrained sense of self, can provide an enhanced sense of self-realization and fulfillment of personal potential [40].
Over the previous decade there has been growing empirical research on individual differences in such connections to nature, with the development of a number of scales sharing a common construct [41]. Measures of nature relatedness assess various dimensions of this construct including emotional affiliation (e.g., feelings of oneness with nature), cognitive processes (e.g., views about how nature overlaps with one’s sense of self), relationship commitment (e.g., feelings of attachment to nature), as well as personal experience and behavior (e.g., time spent in nature) [41]. Connection to nature is typically considered as trait-like, or relatively stable over time for an individual, although it can fluctuate over time in response to changed contexts [42,43,44]. In this paper, we use the term ‘nature relatedness’ to refer to the wider psychological construct encompassing connectedness and relationships with nature.

1.3. Is Strong Nature Relatedness Protective Against Ill Health?

Research on nature relatedness has typically focused on relationships with indicators of positive psychological health, rather than psychological or physical ill health. In general, people scoring higher on a measure of connection to nature are more likely to report higher levels of subjective well-being (concerning optimizing pleasure and minimizing pain [41,42,43,44,45]) and psychological well-being (relating to self-actualization and fulfilling one’s potential [46]), although occasionally no association is found [47]. Zelenski and Nisbet (2014) found that nature relatedness was more likely to correlate with indicators of positive well-being than indicators of negative mental health (specifically negative affect and symptoms of depression), proposing that nature relatedness may have a more important role in promoting well-being than buffering negative psychological health [48]. There is inconsistent evidence of relationships between nature relatedness and mental ill health. Several studies have reported correlations between some measures of nature relatedness and reduced anxiety and depression [49,50,51], and a measure of nature relatedness concerning familiarity with and experiences in nature predicted lower somatic anxiety [50]. In contrast, non-significant relationships between nature relatedness and some mental ill-health measures have been reported when accounting for other participant characteristics [49,50]. Here we focus on indicators of psychological and physical ill health to explore the potential role of nature relatedness as a protective factor against ill health.

1.4. Differences in Nature Relatedness Among Social Groups

Understanding differences in nature relatedness across socio-demographic groups can help shed light on potential influencing factors underpinning the development of this orientation. While the personal and social factors influencing environmental concern and behavior have been a focus for investigation [52], to our knowledge socio-demographic factors have not been systematically investigated for the construct of nature relatedness. Additionally, accounting for the effects of socio-demographic variables when considering the relationship between nature relatedness and ill health is critical given the extensive literature on the influence of social characteristics on health. Social characteristics influence health and well-being through complex causal chains of mediating factors, where social, economic and political processes create social hierarchies, which in turn shape the conditions of daily life that determine health status [53]. Key social characteristics associated with mental and physical health include socio-economic disadvantage, gender, cultural background, age and life stage [30,54]. Several studies have reported that health benefits of green space vary according to these kinds of socio-demographic factors, with greater benefits variously reported for young people, older people, those who are socioeconomically disadvantaged, and men [55,56]. Associations between one or several socio-demographic characteristics and nature relatedness have been reported, for example women and older people have scored higher than men and younger age groups [47,57,58]. However, in studies accounting for the influence of several characteristics such as gender and age, these variables were not significant moderators of relationships between nature relatedness and various indicators of well-being [18,45,58]. If higher nature relatedness is associated with reduced psychological and physical ill health after accounting for the effects of a range of socio-demographic variables, this suggests a potential role for enhancing nature relatedness in public health initiatives.
Here we address a critical gap in knowledge about the role of nature relatedness in fostering health. The first step in this paper is to consider the associations between nature relatedness and a broad range of socio-demographic factors. We then consider the relationships between nature relatedness and four variables related to ill health—depression, anxiety, stress and self-reported health—while accounting for a broad range of potentially confounding socio-demographic indicators and other health-related variables in a large community sample. The findings will shed light on the potential for nature relatedness to be considered as a psychological orientation relevant to the development, implementation and evaluation of public health initiatives.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Location

This study was undertaken in Brisbane, Queensland, a city located in subtropical eastern Australia. The local government authority area covers 1380 km2, with a human population of approximately 1,090,000 in 2011. A substantial per capita area of land is designated as public green space within the city (over 200 m2 per person, with these areas including, for example, manicured parks, natural bushland, and children’s play areas). This green space is relatively evenly distributed across the built up part of the city [11].

2.2. Participants and Procedure

An online survey was delivered to 1538 respondents across Brisbane in November 2012 (late spring) via Q&A Market Research Ltd (Brisbane, Australia). Survey respondents were part of a market research panel through which they receive “points” for completing surveys they have been invited to fill-in. This approach allowed stratification of the survey population, which was completed by people between 18 and 70 years of age stratified such that the sample spanned variation among the population, being: (a) equally spread across male and females; (b) equally spread above and below 45 years of age; (c) equally sampling income quartiles representative of the total Brisbane population (2011 Australian Census data); and (d) equally sampling respondents residing across four spatial zones reflecting quartiles of different levels of tree cover across the city. Participants were invited to complete a 15–20 min survey considering how city spaces influence lifestyles and well-being (for full scale see [59]).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Nature Relatedness

The Nature Relatedness (NR) Scale measures a person’s affective, cognitive and experiential relationships with the natural world on a 21 item scale (for complete scale see [43]). We used this scale due to its sound psychometric properties and multidimensional nature, comprising three subscales assessing different aspects of a person’s relationship with nature: NR Self—assessing personal identification and connection with nature (8 items; e.g., “My relationship with nature is an important part of who I am”; Cronbach’s alpa = 0.85); NR Perspective—assessing a person’s pro-environmental conservation attitudes, world view and sense of agency relating to nature including the impacts of humans on all living things (7 items; e.g., “Conservation is unnecessary because nature is strong enough to recover from any human impact” (reversed; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.51); humans have the right to use natural resources any way we want (reversed; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.52); and NR Experience—assessing a person’s attraction to and physical familiarity with nature (6 items; e.g., “I enjoy being outdoors, even in unpleasant weather”; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.76). Tam (2013) found that the Nature Relatedness Scale tended to be more highly correlated with criterion variables such as personality traits, subjective well-being and environmental behavior than other unidimensional measures, as well as having an additional ability to predict some criterion variables [41], although Capaldi et al. (2014) found the converse, where the single item inclusion of nature in self measure had the strongest relationship with indicators of happiness in comparison to other measures [45]. Each item on the NR scale is presented on a five point Likert scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly), with higher scores indicating stronger connection to nature (8 items were reverse scored). The NR Score is the mean of the responses to the 21 statements, with subscale scores being the mean of the responses to the items in each subscale. Nature relatedness can therefore vary between 1 and 5.

2.3.2. Depression, Anxiety and Stress

Respondents completed the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21 [60]). Each subscale consisted of seven items, with lower scores indicative of lower levels of depression, anxiety or stress. This scale was developed and validated with a range of population samples across different nations and in both clinical and nonclinical settings [61,62,63]. Respondents rated each symptom regarding its severity during the previous week on a four point response scale from 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me very much or most of the time). The total score for each of the subscales was used for analysis purposes in this study (Cronbach’s alpha for depression subscale = 0.93, anxiety subscale = 0.90, and stress subscale = 0.88).

2.3.3. Self-Reported Health

Respondents were asked to rate their own health on a five point rating with five response options (from very poor to very good). There is extensive evidence that this single item measure designed to reduce burden for respondents is significantly associated with a wide range of health-related outcomes including mortality, changes in health status, health service usage, and experience of and recovery from specific health problems, thus providing a robust measure of current health status [64].

2.3.4. Socio-Demographic Indicators

Respondents provided information on key socio-demographic characteristics (outlined in Table S1) including age, gender, educational attainment, personal annual income, how many hours a week they usually worked and primary language spoken at home. Respondents also indicated whether any children under 16 lived at home.

2.3.5. Other Health-Related Variables

Respondents indicated the number of days they exercised for 30 min or more during the previous week, with the final score being between 0 and 7. Body Mass Index (BMI) was determined from respondent self-reports of height and weight, calculating BMI as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters [65]. Perceptions of social cohesion were assessed using 3 scales (17 items) measuring trust, reciprocal exchange within communities, and general community cohesion. The items included five from Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls’ (1997) social cohesion and trust scale (e.g., “People in this community are willing to help their neighbors” with response options ‘Don’t know’, ‘Disagree strongly’, ‘Disagree’, ‘Agree’, ‘Agree strongly’; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.82) [66], six items adapted from the reciprocated exchange scale developed by Sampson, Morenoff, and Earls (1999; e.g., “About how often do you and people in your community do favors for each other?” with response options ‘Don’t know’, ‘Never’, ‘Rarely’, ‘Sometimes’, ‘Often’; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.89) [67] and six items from a general measure of social capital using components from Bullen and Onyx (1998; e.g., “Do you think multiculturalism makes life in your area better?” with response options ‘Don’t know’, ‘Not at all’, ‘Not often’, ‘Sometimes’, or ‘Yes, definitely’; Cronbach’s alpha = 0.68) [68]. ‘Don’t know’ responses were excluded from the analysis. An average score was generated for each of the three scales, with higher scores indicating greater social cohesion (Cronbach’s alpha across all 17 items was 0.81). The average of scores from the three scales provided an overall estimate of social cohesion resulting in a continuous score from highest (4) to the lowest (0) perceived social cohesion.

2.4. Ethical Clearance

This research was conducted in accordance with Institutional Human Research Ethics Approval (Behavioural & Social Sciences Ethical Review Committee, University of Queensland), ethical code project number 2012000869 and approved by the Behavioural & Social Sciences Ethical Review Committee, University of Queensland.

2.5. Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out using the software package R [69]. Relationships between the socio-demographic indicators and each of the four measures of NR (NR Score, NR Self, NR Perspective, NR Experience; as four separate response variables) were tested using independent-samples t tests and analyses of variance, with effect sizes indicated by Cohen’s d and partial η-squared respectively. Highest formal qualification, work status, income, gender, language, presence of children in the home and age were treated as ordinal factors. Ordinal regression models (with a cumulative link function) were then used to test the association between the NR measures as predictors of depression, anxiety, stress and self-reported health. We first tested the association ignoring all possible covariates, and then included the other above-mentioned socio-demographic indicators and health-related variables as predictors. We checked for multicollinearity between predictors using the variance inflation factor (VIF). All variables had VIF < 2, which was sufficiently low to include them in the same model, except for the NR variables. Consequently, we included each as a predictor in its own separate model for each health variable because of the high level of correlation between the NR subscales (e.g., for depression we constructed four models, which included all covariates, and one each of NR Score, NR Self, NR Perspective, and NR Experience). We tested for zero-order correlations between the NR measures and the other predictors, between the NR measures and health variables, and between key significant predictor variables and health measures.

3. Results

Nature relatedness differed significantly across a number of socio-demographic characteristics (Table 1). People over 45 years had significantly higher scores across all NR measures in comparison to those less than 45 years. Women had higher scores than did men for all NR measures aside from NR Experience, where men had significantly higher scores. Those in the middle income quartiles had higher NR Self scores compared to those in the highest income quartile, but with very low effect sizes. People who did not have children living at home had higher NR scores for all scales. Those who completed a trade/diploma had higher NR Score, NR Self and NR Experience in comparison to those with those who had completed secondary school, again with very small effect sizes. Those speaking English at home had higher scores across all NR measures. People who did not work tended to have higher scores for the overall NR scale and NR Self compared with those who worked full time, with part-time workers reporting higher scores than full-time workers for NR Self (Table 1). See Supplementary Material Tables S2 and S3 for regression models assessing the relationship between socio-demographic variables and NR measures.
People with higher NR Score and NR Experience scores were more likely to report better self-reported health, and those with higher NR Experience were also more likely to report fewer symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress. Perhaps surprisingly, people with higher NR Self were more marginally likely to report poorer self-reported health, although the effect was rather weak (Table 2).
Accounting for the range of covariates, the association between NR Experience and better self-reported health remained significant, although NR Experience was no longer associated with fewer symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress. People with higher NR Score, NR Self and NR Perspective reported increased symptoms of stress, and those with higher NR Self also reported higher levels of depression and anxiety (Table 3). Significant associations were found between all nature relatedness measures and social cohesion, between three of the four nature relatedness measures and exercise, but there were no significant associations between any of the nature relatedness measures and BMI (Supplementary Materials Tables S2–S8).

4. Discussion

Using a community sample from Brisbane, Queensland we assessed differences in nature relatedness scores among different socio-demographic groups. We then investigated whether nature relatedness was associated with self-reported mental and physical ill health, including controlling for socio-demographic and other health-related characteristics that are potentially important covariates. People with higher scores on the Nature Relatedness scale were more likely to be older, female, not have children living at home, speak English at home and not work. In testing associations between NR measures and health variables, three of the four NR measures were associated with at least one measure of reduced ill health. Strikingly, people with higher NR Experience were significantly less likely to experience symptoms of depression, anxiety, stress and self-reported ill health. When potentially confounding variables were accounted for, NR Experience remained significantly associated with better self-reported health, but the associations with negative mental health were no longer significant. NR Score, NR Self and NR Perspective were associated with increased stress when socio-demographic and health-related variables were accounted for.
There are several possible explanations for these findings, and they suggest some fruitful directions for future research. Our finding that older people and females tend to report higher nature relatedness is consistent with some other studies of connectedness to nature [47,57,58]. The increase in nature relatedness for older people may reflect a cohort effect in which older people have had distinctly different experiences with nature throughout their lives compared with those born after the mid-1960s. It was during the 1960s that television and car ownership became more common in households, potentially leading to reduced time outdoors during leisure and recreation time [70]. Over the previous decades there has also been an escalation in the use of personal computers and engagement in social media associated with decreased time in natural settings [71]. It is also possible that a person’s orientation to nature changes across the life course; for example, the opportunities and motivations associated with spending time in natural settings for caregivers of young children are likely to be very different than those with adult children or in retirement [9,72]. These life changes may help explain why having children at home was associated with lower nature relatedness, with other caregiving tasks possibly taking a higher priority than engagement with and appreciation of nature. However, longitudinal research to consider causal pathways is needed to disentangle whether other explanations may be at play.
Our finding that females reported higher nature relatedness is consistent with most other studies, in which females tend to report greater environmental concern, behavior and attitudes than males, although notably this does not hold for specific subgroups, for example women in China appear to show lower environmental concern than do men [73,74]. In our study, while females had higher overall nature relatedness scores, males had higher scores on NR Experience, reflecting comfort and enjoyment of being in nature. Again, these differences are consistent with findings that women experience and use nature settings differently than men, particularly in relation to perceptions of safety [56].
It is unclear why people who do not speak English at home report lower nature relatedness. The construct of ‘nature relatedness’ is likely to differ across cultures and settings, given evidence of cross-cultural differences in landscape preferences, type of use and motivations to visit outdoor settings [75,76]. Although there is consistent evidence of high levels of concern about the environment and support for protection of the environment in cross-cultural studies over the previous two decades [77], there is substantial evidence of different types of environmental concern across countries and ethnic groups, with the ‘structure’ or focus of the concern markedly different across these diverse contexts [52]. To date, there has been little specific focus on the extent and nature of differences in nature relatedness between people from different cultural backgrounds [45]; most studies have recruited participants from western, high income countries, although recent research has also confirmed the positive association between nature relatedness and well-being in people from Japanese and Russian cultures [78].
We surmise there may be a particularly important role for NR Experience—the dimension of nature relatedness that concerns physical familiarity, comfort with and enjoyment in natural settings—given its association with reduced ill health across each of the health measures. NR Experience was also the only measure of nature relatedness that remained significantly associated with reduced physical ill health when potentially confounding demographic and health-related variables were included in the analysis. NR Experience is a more behaviorally focused measure of nature relatedness than the more psychological orientation of NR Self and NR Perspective, with items referring to experiential engagement in nature activities such as enjoying ‘being outdoors’ and ‘digging in the earth’. Given that evidence to date does not provide strong support for the premise that nature relatedness moderates the effect of nature contact on well-being [18,79], a key pathway to better health for people with higher NR Experience may be through their time spent in natural settings, which in turn provides people with the opportunity to obtain diverse health-related benefits. Indeed, higher scores on NR Experience may be associated with ‘exposure’ to nature across the life course [27].
The marked changes in the associations between nature relatedness measures and health variables when the broad range of socio-demographic and health-related variables were included in the analysis points to the need for additional understanding of causal pathways and complex interactions between a person’s orientation to nature and his/her life stage, health, psychological characteristics and social context. While our analysis accounts for health-related variables that could provide an intermediary pathway between nature relatedness and health (e.g., exercise, social cohesion), the potential complexity of underlying mechanistic pathways means we remain some way from a robust understanding of the impact of nature relatedness on health. For example, people may engage in high levels of exercise in built settings or experience high levels of social cohesion unrelated to activities connected to green space or natural settings.
Our finding that overall nature relatedness, as well as the subscales NR Self and NR Perspective, were all associated with increased stress (involving chronic non-specific arousal such as feeling agitated or having difficulty relaxing) was unexpected. Additional research is needed to investigate whether this finding can be replicated. One hypothesis is that people with greater environmental concern may experience increased psychological stress or distress related to the escalation of environmental degradation locally and globally. During the year prior to data collection there had been a major flood in Brisbane, as well as widespread dismantling of environmental regulation by the Queensland state government [80], both of which plausibly could contribute to additional stress related to environmental concern [81]. There is a small but growing literature regarding the emotional distress related to awareness of climate change, and consequences of environmental degradation more generally [81,82,83,84,85].
That higher scores on the NR Self subscale were associated with increased stress when the covariates were not included in the analysis, and also increased symptoms of depression and anxiety when the covariates were accounted for, further highlights the need for additional understanding of potentially negative aspects to dimensions of nature relatedness. Several questions in the NR Self subscale concern awareness of how one’s actions affect the environment, awareness of environmental issues, and concerns around the suffering of animals, all of which might be associated with increased negative emotions. Several studies have found negative or non-significant relationships between nature relatedness and the personality trait of neuroticism [41,43], although other research has found associations between neuroticism and environmental values [86] and environmental concern [87]. These inconsistent findings suggest that the specific psychological processes or dimensions inherent in different types of psychological orientation to nature should be carefully considered in future research investigating relationships between positive or negative emotions and nature relatedness, alongside careful consideration of the socio-demographic characteristics of sub-groups of people with these different psychological orientations.

4.1. Strengths and Limitations

This study provides a unique examination of the relationships between dimensions of nature relatedness and four health-related variables, using data from a large stratified sample of respondents. The study is of course somewhat limited by its cross-sectional design, which precludes drawing conclusions about the direction or pathways of causation; indeed it is possible that those who have better mental or physical health differ in their psychological capacity to connect not just with humans but also landscapes and living organisms.
Recent research has considered ways in which the construct of nature relatedness may also be linked with other unmeasured variables influencing the relationships with health measures; suggesting that linkages between nature relatedness and psychological well-being may be mediated by individual characteristics such as meaning in life [88] and spirituality [89]. Prospective longitudinal studies are required that carefully consider the causal pathways between nature-related orientations and behaviors (e.g., psychological connection to nature and exposure to nature—including frequency, duration, type of nature activity, quality of the nature setting) and a range of psychological and physical health outcomes across both the short and longer term. Including psychological orientation to nature in the systematic evaluation of health behavior interventions in natural settings (e.g., green exercise or ‘mindfulness in nature’ programs) will help shed light on the role of nature relatedness in moderating and mediating health outcomes.
Stratification of our sample enhanced representativeness of the population, although recruitment through a market research company may have introduced an unknown form of sampling bias. The sample was recruited from one Australian city, and findings may not be generalizable to other populations. Specifically, the participants were living within a Western high income context, a warm climate with substantial nearby green space conducive to outdoor activities, and relatively high environmental concern [90]. The use of self-report measures risks recall or response bias that could influence participant responses. The measure of depression, anxiety and stress was not a clinical diagnostic tool, rather it aimed to provide a measure of emotional experiences, including different characteristics of anxiety and depression, over the previous week [62]. The additional use of other measurement approaches such as implicit measures of nature relatedness [91], clinical diagnoses of anxiety or depression [25] or biomarkers of variation in stress levels [14] might enhance the validity of our findings. The use of single-item measures of health assisted to reduce response burden and enabled the data collection across a wide range of variables. Nonetheless, single item self-report measures such as exercise and BMI inevitably risk oversimplification.
A key consideration for interpreting the results laid out in this paper is the potential for over-adjustment bias. This issue arises where covariates are included in the models that are on the causal pathway from an influencing factor to an effect [92]. For example, this could be the case in our study if nature relatedness affected social cohesion, which in turn affected a health variable. While there is likely some small amount of bias present in this study, we expect that these effects are relatively minor. This is supported by the finding that we see similar directional results in the zero-order correlations to those outlined in the full models.

4.2. Future Research

The findings highlight the need for a more fine-grained understanding of the different dimensions of nature relatedness, and their associations with physical and mental health for diverse subpopulations. The mixed findings suggest that some dimensions of this psychological orientation—particularly a strong emotional connection to nature and awareness of environmental degradation—may be associated with negative emotions with the potential to contribute to ill health for some individuals. Further understanding is needed of the nature of any disbenefits, for whom, and in what circumstances, along with exploration of the potential for other societal benefits. For example, increasing environmental connection and concern may provide a critical catalyst for environmental behavior, given escalating ecosystem degradation globally and the urgent need to take action to promote socially and ecologically sustainable societies [93,94,95].
Careful attention to differences in nature relatedness across social groups, including people from different socioeconomic backgrounds, gender, life stage and cultural groups, is imperative in future research. Cross cultural consideration of nature relatedness is needed to understand if and how the construct of nature relatedness holds meaning for people from different ethnic and socio-cultural backgrounds, including the development of scales that are appropriate for cross-cultural research. In the Australian context, Indigenous Australian conceptualizations of nature and its inextricable linkages with health hold promise as a pathway to addressing health inequities as well as environmental issues [96].
Investigation of individuals’ connections to different forms of nature is also needed. Urban nature exists in many disparate forms—from private gardens to natural features on verges, parking lots and railway embankments—all of which have the potential to be valued and have salutogenic effects [97,98]. To date, research on nature relatedness has typically focused on ‘nature’ in general; understanding the ‘lived experience’ of connectedness to nature or ‘place attachment’ and considering how individuals relate to specific ecological features or places important in their lives will aid in developing more context-specific understanding of this construct, with implications for its potential to foster health and well-being [47,99,100].
The findings suggest that programs to foster the dimension of nature relatedness involving enjoyment of and comfort in natural settings may offer the most promising way forward as potential public health initiatives, although knowledge about how to foster nature relatedness is not yet well-developed. Childhood experiences in nature are frequently suggested as important in cultivating nature relatedness, especially engagement with ‘expansive’ or ‘wild’ natural settings [41,101]. However, little is known about the extent to which nature relatedness changes over the longer term in the context of formative experiences for children, or indeed across different life stages. Longitudinal studies showing the predictive value of nature relatedness to child health and development, as well as experimental studies of approaches to increase nature relatedness via families, childcare, schools and other contexts, are needed. There is promising research suggesting that school-based education programs involving direct engagement with nature may result in increased connection to nature, as well as findings that child-led immersion in nature settings may also promote increased parental engagement in nature activities with their children [102]. There is also potential to promote nature relatedness through immersive environmental education programs and wilderness expeditions [18,103,104] and psychological approaches involving self-reflective practices such as curiosity about emotions, attitudes, values and thoughts while being exposed to natural elements [105]. Consideration of intervention elements beyond engagement in the nature setting itself is also worthy of consideration, such as key role models (parents, grandparents, teachers, community leaders) communicating and inspiring connection to nature [106] or creative arts that involve engaging with natural elements [107]. Of critical importance is understanding how to foster nature relatedness among the population as a whole—particularly subgroups experiencing social disadvantage—to avoid perpetuating and amplifying health inequities.
Finally, there may be new ways forward in considering ‘nature relatedness’ in deeper or expanded ways that move beyond relatively constrained conceptions of humans in a relationship with ‘nature’ confined to landscapes and living organisms. This could encompass a broader relational understanding of self as part of a planetary ecosystem that involves not just living systems but built, material environments and social relationships. There is burgeoning theoretical work on the need for cultural change in which our sense of self is embedded within wider global systems in order to respond to critical challenges, such as the growing gaps between advantaged and disadvantaged groups within and across nations, and ecological imperatives such as biodiversity loss, pollution and climate change [108].

5. Conclusions

This paper has systematically considered the relationships between a broad range of socio-demographic factors and dimensions of nature relatedness, and presented some possible explanations of the development of higher nature relatedness in specific social groups. Additionally, the study investigated relationships between affective, cognitive and experiential dimensions of nature relatedness and mental and physical ill health. The nature relatedness orientation in which people feel pleasure in spending time in natural settings showed particular promise as a potential protective factor against high levels of depression, anxiety and poor self-reported health. The associations between negative mental health and the dimension of nature relatedness involving an internalized sense of identification with nature highlights the need to further understand the potentially negative aspects of nature relatedness for mental health. A critical next step is systematic longitudinal and experimental research that disentangles causal pathways between different dimensions of nature relatedness and the health outcomes experienced, including careful consideration of diversity between subpopulations and the contexts in which they live.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/1660-4601/15/7/1371/s1. Table S1: Socio-demographic variables, Table S2: Results from ordinal regression models (Cumulative link) assessing the relationship between socio-demographic predictor variables and nature relatedness score and nature relatedness self (Zero Order Correlations), Table S3: Results from ordinal regression models (Cumulative link) assessing the relationship between socio-demographic predictor variables and nature relatedness perspective and nature relatedness experience (Zero Order Correlations), Table S4: Results from ordinal regression models (Cumulative link) assessing the relationship between socio-demographic predictor variables and symptoms of depression, Table S5: Results from ordinal regression models (Cumulative link) assessing the relationship between socio-demographic variables and symptoms of anxiety, Table S6: Results from ordinal regression models (Cumulative link) assessing the relationship between socio-demographic predictor variables and symptoms of stress, Table S7: Results from ordinal regression models (Cumulative link) assessing the relationship between socio-demographic predictor variables and respondent’s assessment of their own health, Table S8: Results from ordinal regression models (Cumulative link) assessing the relationship between predictor variables and respondent’s assessment of their own health. These models consider each predictor separately without other variables (i.e., zero order correlations). The three predictors shown are important predictors of the health variables measured here.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.H.D., R.B., K.J.G., B.B.L., E.B., L.F. and R.A.F.; Data curation, D.F.S.; Formal analysis, D.F.S.; Funding acquisition, R.B., K.J.G., B.B.L. and R.A.F.; Investigation, D.F.S., R.B., K.J.G., B.B.L., E.B., L.F. and R.A.F.; Methodology, J.H.D., D.F.S., R.B., K.J.G., B.B.L., E.B., L.F. and R.A.F.; Project administration, D.F.S., R.B., K.J.G., B.B.L. and R.A.F.; Writing-original draft, J.H.D. and D.F.S.; Writing-review & editing, J.H.D., D.F.S., R.B., K.J.G., B.B.L., E.B., L.F. and R.A.F.

Funding

This research was funded by Australian Research Council Discovery Grant DP120102857.

Acknowledgments

We thank Brisbane City Council and the Queensland Government for providing access to the park data used in this research. Danielle Shanahan is supported through Australian Research Council Discovery grant DP120102857. Kevin Gaston is supported by Natural Environment Research Council grant NE/J015237/1. Brenda Lin is supported through the Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation Climate Adaptation Flagship. Elizabeth Barber is supported by a University of Queensland Research Scholarship and Lara Franco is supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award. Richard Fuller holds an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs. World Urbanization Prospects: The 2014 Revision, Highlights (ST/ESA/SER.A/352); United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bratman, G.N.; Hamilton, J.P.; Daily, G.C. The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. In Year in Ecology and Conservation Biology; Ostfeld, R.S., Schlesinger, W.H., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; Volume 1249, pp. 118–136. [Google Scholar]
  3. Pyle, R.M. The extinction of experience. Horticulture 1978, 56, 64–67. [Google Scholar]
  4. Gascon, M.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Martinez, D.; Dadvand, P.; Rojas-Rueda, D.; Plasencia, A.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Residential green spaces and mortality: A systematic review. Environ. Int. 2016, 86, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Louv, R. Last Child in the Woods: Saving Our Children from Nature-Deficit Disorder; Algonquin Books: Chapel Hill, NC, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  6. Miller, J.R. Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2005, 20, 430–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Soga, M.; Gaston, K.J. Extinction of experience: The loss of human-nature interactions. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2016, 14, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Van den Berg, A.E.; Hartig, T.; Staats, H. Preference for nature in urbanized societies: Stress, restoration, and the pursuit of sustainability. J. Soc. Issues 2007, 63, 79–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bell, S.L.; Phoenix, C.; Lovell, R.; Wheeler, B.W. Green space, health and wellbeing: Making space for individual agency. Health Place 2014, 30, 287–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Lin, B.B.; Fuller, R.A.; Bush, R.; Gaston, K.J.; Shanahan, D.F. Opportunity or orientation? Who uses urban parks and why. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e87422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Shanahan, D.F.; Lin, B.B.; Gaston, K.J.; Bush, R.; Fuller, R.A. Socio-economic inequalities in access to nature on public and private lands: A case study from Brisbane, Australia. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 130, 14–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Fuller, R.A.; Irvine, K.N.; Devine-Wright, P.; Warren, P.H.; Gaston, K.J. Psychological benefits of greenspace increase with biodiversity. Biol. Lett. 2007, 3, 390–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Hartig, T.; Evans, G.W.; Jamner, L.D.; Davis, D.S.; Garling, T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Roe, J.J.; Thompson, C.W.; Aspinall, P.A.; Brewer, M.J.; Duff, E.I.; Miller, D.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A. Green space and stress: Evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 4086–4103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Stigsdotter, U.K.; Ekholm, O.; Schipperijn, J.; Toftager, M.; Kamper-Jorgensen, F.; Randrup, T.B. Health promoting outdoor environments—Associations between green space, and health, health-related quality of life and stress based on a Danish national representative survey. Scand. J. Public Health 2010, 38, 411–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Berman, M.G.; Kross, E.; Krpan, K.M.; Askren, M.K.; Burson, A.; Deldin, P.J.; Kaplan, S.; Sherdell, L.; Gotlib, I.H.; Jonides, J. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 140, 300–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. MacKerron, G.; Mourato, S. Happiness is greater in natural environments. Global Environ. Chang.-Hum. Policy Dimens. 2013, 23, 992–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Mayer, F.S.; Frantz, C.M.; Bruehlman-Senecal, E.; Dolliver, K. Why is nature beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 607–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ryan, R.M.; Weinstein, N.; Bernstein, J.; Brown, K.W.; Mistretta, L.; Gagne, M. Vitalizing effects of being outdoors and in nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Berman, M.G.; Jonides, J.; Kaplan, S. The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 1207–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Bratman, G.N.; Hamilton, J.P.; Hahn, K.S.; Daily, G.C.; Gross, J.J. Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 8567–8572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Giles-Corti, B.; Bull, F.; Knuiman, M.; McCormack, G.; Van Niel, K.; Timperio, A.; Christian, H.; Foster, S.; Divitini, M.; Middleton, N.; et al. The influence of urban design on neighbourhood walking following residential relocation: Longitudinal results from the RESIDE study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 77, 20–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Toftager, M.; Ekholm, O.; Schipperijn, J.; Stigsdotter, U.; Bentsen, P.; Gronbaek, M.; Randrup, T.B.; Kamper-Jorgensen, F. Distance to Green Space and Physical Activity: A Danish National Representative Survey. J. Phys. Act. Health 2011, 8, 741–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Kim, J.; Kaplan, R. Physical and psychological factors in sense of community—New urbanist Kentlands and nearby orchard village. Environ. Behav. 2004, 36, 313–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A.; de Vries, S.; Spreeuwenberg, P.; Schellevis, F.G.; Groenewegen, P.P. Morbidity is related to a green living environment. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2009, 63, 967–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Sugiyama, T.; Leslie, E.; Giles-Corti, B.; Owen, N. Associations of neighbourhood greenness with physical and mental health: Do walking, social coherence and local social interaction explain the relationships? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2008, 62, e9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Shanahan, D.F.; Fuller, R.A.; Bush, R.; Lin, B.B.; Gaston, K.J. The health benefits of urban nature: How much do we need? Bioscience 2015, 65, 476–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Shanahan, D.F.; Lin, B.B.; Bush, R.; Gaston, K.J.; Dean, J.H.; Barber, E.; Fuller, R.A. Toward improved public health outcomes from urban nature. Am. J. Public Health 2015, 105, 470–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Vos, T.; Barber, R.M.; Bell, B.; Bertozzi-Villa, A.; Biryukov, S.; Bolliger, I.; Charlson, F.; Davis, A.; Degenhardt, L.; Dicker, D.; et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 301 acute and chronic diseases and injuries in 188 countries, 1990–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet 2015, 386, 743–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Whiteford, H.A.; Degenhardt, L.; Rehm, J.; Baxter, A.J.; Ferrari, A.J.; Erskine, H.E.; Charlson, F.J.; Norman, R.E.; Flaxman, A.D.; Johns, N.; et al. Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: Findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 2013, 382, 1575–1586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Prados-Torres, A.; Calderon-Larranaga, A.; Hancco-Saavedra, J.; Poblador-Plou, B.; van den Akker, M. Multimorbidity patterns: A systematic review. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2014, 67, 254–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Bauer, U.E.; Briss, P.A.; Goodman, R.A.; Bowman, B.A. Prevention of chronic disease in the 21st century: Elimination of the leading preventable causes of premature death and disability in the USA. Lancet 2014, 384, 45–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Wolch, J.R.; Byrne, J.; Newell, J.P. Urban green space, public health, and environmental justice: The challenge of making cities ‘just green enough’. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 125, 234–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Thompson, C.W. Linking landscape and health: The recurring theme. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2011, 99, 187–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature—Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15, 169–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Joye, Y.; van den Berg, A. Is love for green in our genes? A critical analysis of evolutionary assumptions in restorative environments research. Urban For. Urban Green. 2011, 10, 261–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Beery, T.; Jonsson, K.I.; Elmberg, J. From environmental connectedness to sustainable futures: Topophilia and human affiliation with nature. Sustainability 2015, 7, 8837–8854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bragg, E.A. Towards ecological self: Deep ecology meets constructionist self-theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 1996, 16, 93–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Naess, A. Self-realization: An ecological approach to being in the world. Trumpeter 1987, 4, 35–42. [Google Scholar]
  41. Tam, K.P. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: Similarities and differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 34, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mayer, F.S.; Frantz, C.M. The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 503–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M.; Murphy, S.A. The nature relatedness scale linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 715–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M. Underestimating nearby nature: Affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 1101–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Capaldi, C.A.; Dopko, R.L.; Zelenski, J.M. The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Howell, A.J.; Dopko, R.L.; Passmore, H.A.; Buro, K. Nature connectedness: Associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 51, 166–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Cervinka, R.; Roderer, K.; Hefler, E. Are nature lovers happy? On various indicators of well-being and connectedness with nature. J. Health Psychol. 2012, 17, 379–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Zelenski, J.M.; Nisbet, E.K. Happiness and feeling connected: The distinct role of nature relatedness. Environ. Behav. 2014, 46, 3–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ginting, H.; Naring, G.; Kwakkenbos, L.; Becker, E.S. Spirituality and Negative Emotions in Individuals with Coronary Heart Disease. J. Cardiovasc. Nurs. 2015, 30, 537–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Lawton, E.; Brymer, E.; Clough, P.; Denovan, A. The relationship between the physical activity environment, nature relatedness, anxiety, and the psychological well-being benefits of regular exercisers. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Martyn, P.; Brymer, E. The relationship between nature relatedness and anxiety. J. Health Psychol. 2016, 21, 1436–1445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Gifford, R.; Nilsson, A. Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: A review. Int. J. Psychol. 2014, 49, 141–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Solar, O.; Irwin, A. A Conceptual Framework for Action on the Social Determinants of Health. Social Determinants of Health Discussion Paper 2 (Policy and Practice); World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  54. Allen, J.; Balfour, R.; Bell, R.; Marmot, M. Social determinants of mental health. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2014, 26, 392–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A.; Groenewegen, P.P.; de Vries, S.; Spreeuwenberg, P. Green space, urbanity, and health: How strong is the relation? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2006, 60, 587–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Richardson, E.A.; Mitchell, R. Gender differences in relationships between urban green space and health in the United Kingdom. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010, 71, 568–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Haluza, D.; Simic, S.; Holtge, J.; Cervinka, R.; Moshammer, H. Connectedness to Nature and Public (Skin) Health Perspectives: Results of a Representative, Population-Based Survey among Austrian Residents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 1176–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Zhang, J.W.; Howell, R.T.; Iyer, R. Engagement with natural beauty moderates the positive relation between connectedness with nature and psychological well-being. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Shanahan, D.F.; Bush, R.; Gaston, K.J.; Lin, B.B.; Dean, J.; Barber, E.; Fuller, R.A. Health benefits from nature experiences depend on dose. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 28551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Lovibond, S.H.; Lovibond, P.F. Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, 2nd ed.; University of New South Wales: Sydney, NSW, Australia, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  61. Antony, M.M.; Bieling, P.J.; Cox, B.J.; Enns, M.W.; Swinson, R.P. Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol. Assess. 1998, 10, 176–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Davies, G.; Caputi, P.; Skarvelis, M.; Ronan, N. The Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales: Reference data from a large psychiatric outpatient sample. Aust. J. Psychol. 2015, 67, 97–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Henry, J.D.; Crawford, J.R. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 44, 227–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Bowling, A. Just one question: If one question works, why ask several? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2005, 59, 342–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. World Health Organization. Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic Report of a Who Consultation. Who Technical Report Series 894; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  66. Sampson, R.J.; Raudenbush, S.W.; Earls, F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 1997, 277, 918–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Sampson, R.J.; Morenoff, J.D.; Earls, F. Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1999, 64, 633–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Bullen, P.; Onyx, J. Measuring Social Capital in Five Communities in NSW—A Practitioner’s Guide; Management Alternatives Pty Ltd.: Coogee, New South Wales, Australian, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  69. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  70. Karsten, L. It all used to be better? Different generations on continuity and change in urban children’s daily use of space. Child. Geogr. 2005, 3, 275–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Pergams, O.R.W.; Zaradic, P.A. Is love of nature in the US becoming love of electronic media? 16-year downtrend in national park visits explained by watching movies, playing video games, internet use, and oil prices. J. Environ. Manag. 2006, 80, 387–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Astell-Burt, T.; Mitchell, R.; Hartig, T. The association between green space and mental health varies across the lifecourse. A longitudinal study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2014, 68, 578–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Xiao, C.Y.; Hong, D.Y. Gender differences in environmental behaviors in China. Popul. Environ. 2010, 32, 88–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Zelezny, L.C.; Chua, P.P.; Aldrich, C. Elaborating on gender differences in environmentalism. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 443–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Buijs, A.E.; Elands, B.H.M.; Langers, F. No wilderness for immigrants: Cultural differences in images of nature and landscape preferences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2009, 91, 113–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Gentin, S. Outdoor recreation and ethnicity in Europe-A review. Urban For. Urban Green. 2011, 10, 153–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Milfont, T.L.; Schultz, P.W. Culture and the natural environment. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2016, 8, 194–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Capaldi, C.A.; Passmore, H.-A.; Ishii, R.; Chistopolskaya, K.A.; Vowinckel, J.; Nikolaev, E.L.; Semikin, G.I. Engaging with natural beauty may be related to well-being because it connects people to nature: Evidence from three cultures. Ecopsychology 2017, 9, 199–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Passmore, H.-A.; Howell, A.J. Nature involvement increases hedonic and eudemonic well-being: A two week experimental study. Ecopsychology 2014, 6, 148–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Lowe, I. Queensland’s Big Step Back from Environmental Assessment 2012. Available online: https://theconversation.com/queenslands-big-step-back-from-environmental-assessment-9238 (accessed on 12 February 2018).
  81. Reser, J.P.; Swim, J.K. Adapting to and coping with the threat and impacts of climate change. Am. Psychol. 2011, 66, 277–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Albrecht, G. Chronic environmental change: Emerging ‘psychoterratic’ syndromes. In Climate Change and Human Well-Being: Global Challenges Opportunities; Weissbecker, I., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 43–56. [Google Scholar]
  83. Fritze, J.G.; Blashki, G.A.; Burke, S.; Wiseman, H. Hope, despair and transformation: Climate change and the promotion of mental health and wellbeing. Int. J. Ment. Health Syst. 2008, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Gifford, E.; Gifford, R. The largely unacknowledged impact of climate change on mental health. Bull. At. Sci. 2016, 72, 292–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Searle, K.; Gow, K. Do concerns about climate change lead to distress? Int. J. Clim. Chang. Strateg. Manag. 2010, 2, 362–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Wiseman, M.; Bogner, F.X. A higher-order model of ecological values and its relationship to personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003, 34, 783–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Hirsh, J.B. Personality and environmental concern. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 245–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Howell, A.J.; Passmore, H.A.; Buro, K. Meaning in nature: Meaning in life as a mediator of the relationship between nature connectedness and well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 2013, 14, 1681–1696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Kamitsis, I.; Francis, A.J.P. Spirituality mediates the relationship between engagement with nature and psychological wellbeing. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 136–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Chapman, J.; Parvazian, S.; Skinner, N. How do Australians rate as environmental citizens? An international comparative analysis of environmental concern and action. Australas. J. Environ. Manag. 2017, 24, 117–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Schultz, P.W.; Tabanico, J. Self, identity, and the natural environment: Exploring implicit connections with nature. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 37, 1219–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Schisterman, E.F.; Cole, S.R.; Platt, R.W. Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 2009, 20, 488–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Hao, F. A panel regression study on multiple predictors of environmental concern for 82 countries across seven years. Soc. Sci. Q. 2016, 97, 991–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Homburg, A.; Stolberg, A. Explaining pro-environmental behavior with a cognitive theory of stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Rees, J.H.; Bamberg, S. Climate protection needs societal change: Determinants of intention to participate in collective climate action. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 44, 466–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Kingsley, J.; Townsend, M.; Henderson-Wilson, C.; Bolam, B. Developing an exploratory framework linking australian aboriginal peoples’ connection to country and concepts of wellbeing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 678–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Clayton, S. Domesticated nature: Motivations for gardening and perceptions of environmental impact. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 215–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Lorimer, J. Living roofs and brownfield wildlife: Towards a fluid biogeography of UK nature conservation. Environ. Plan. A 2008, 40, 2042–2060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Beery, T.H.; Wolf-Watz, D. Nature to place: Rethinking the environmental connectedness perspective. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 198–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Scannell, L.; Gifford, R. Defining place attachment: A tripartite organizing framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Windhorse, E.; Williams, A. Growing up, naturally: The mental health legacy of early nature affiliation. Ecopsychology 2015, 7, 115–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Lieflander, A.K.; Frohlich, G.; Bogner, F.X.; Schultz, P.W. Promoting connectedness with nature through environmental education. Environ. Educ. Res. 2013, 19, 370–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Barton, J.; Bragg, R.; Pretty, J.; Roberts, J.; Wood, C. The wilderness expedition: An effective life course intervention to improve young people’s well-being and connectedness to nature. J. Exp. Educ. 2016, 39, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Ernst, J.; Theimer, S. Evaluating the effects of environmental education programming on connectedness to nature. Environ. Educ. Res. 2011, 17, 577–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Richardson, M.; Sheffield, D. Reflective self-attention: A more stable predictor of connection to nature than mindful attention. Ecopsychology 2015, 7, 166–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Schelly, C.; Cross, J.E.; Franzen, W.; Hall, P.; Reeve, S. How to go green: Creating a conservation culture in a public high school through education, modeling, and communication. J. Environ. Educ. 2012, 43, 143–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Bruni, C.M.; Winter, P.L.; Schultz, P.W.; Omoto, A.M.; Tabanico, J.J. Getting to know nature: Evaluating the effects of the Get to Know Program on children’s connectedness with nature. Environ. Educ. Res. 2017, 23, 43–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Rayner, G.; Lang, T. Ecological Public Health. Reshaping the Conditions for Good Health; Routledge: London, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Comparing Nature Relatedness across Socio-Demographic Groups.
Table 1. Comparing Nature Relatedness across Socio-Demographic Groups.
VariableNumber in GroupNR Score aNR Self aNR Perspective aNR Experience a
AverageStd DevAverageStd DevAverageStd DevAverageStd Dev
Age
<459523.380.593.290.723.600.703.240.83
≥455863.640.593.670.713.770.723.450.74
Cohen’s d 0.441 0.538 0.232 0.269
Gender
Male8093.430.603.350.713.530.733.430.77
Female7293.520.613.510.753.810.663.190.82
Cohen’s d 0.150 0.220 0.414 −0.290
Income quartile
First3423.460.593.440.753.680.723.240.79
Second3633.500.603.490.753.690.673.290.82
Third3513.540.593.480.723.710.713.400.80
Fourth4823.420.623.340.72 *,a3.600.733.330.80
Effect size (ANOVA) N.S. 0.007 N.S. N.S.
Presence of children <16 years in home
Not present11293.510.623.470.763.690.713.360.82
Present4093.380.543.340.663.570.693.200.76
Cohen’s d 0.223 0.174 0.177 0.191
Educational attainment
Secondary school not completed1453.440.573.430.673.650.713.220.82
Secondary school completed3333.390.583.330.723.630.673.210.82
Trade/Diploma or equivalent4053.540.61 *,b3.500.77 *b3.680.703.420.73 **,b
University degree4723.490.643.430.763.690.753.340.86
Post-graduate degree1833.480.563.490.673.620.713.290.78
Effect size (ANOVA) 0.002 0.002 N.S. 0.002
Primary language spoken at home
English13253.500.613.440.743.680.713.370.80
Other language2133.320.543.370.723.550.672.980.77
Cohen’s d 6.756 5.700 6.308 5.123
Work status in survey week
No work3923.540.583.530.713.720.673.320.78
Part-time7383.470.603.440.733.640.703.300.80
Full-time4083.430.63 **c3.320.76 ***c, **d3.630.763.330.83
Effect size (ANOVA) 0.004 0.011 N.S. N.S.
Note. NR scale/subscales range from 1–5. Effect size for ANOVA analyses are indicated by partial eta-squared. a Compared with 2nd & 3rd income quartiles. b Compared with secondary school completed. c Compared with no work. d Compared with part-time work. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Relationships between Nature Relatedness and Four Health Variables (From Ordinal Regression Models, Cumulative Link), Without Accounting for Covariates (Zero Order Correlations).
Table 2. Relationships between Nature Relatedness and Four Health Variables (From Ordinal Regression Models, Cumulative Link), Without Accounting for Covariates (Zero Order Correlations).
Health VariableCoefficient (Standard Error)
NR ScoreNR SelfNR PerspectiveNR Experience
Depression−0.11(0.07)−0.02(0.06)−0.04(0.06)−0.17(0.06) **
Anxiety−0.08(0.08)0.05(0.06)−0.04(0.07)−0.17(0.06) **
Stress−0.03(0.07)0.00(0.00)0.07(0.06)−0.12(0.06) *
Self-reported health0.26(0.08) ***−0.15(0.06) *−0.03(0.07)0.38(0.06) ***
Note. Co-efficients are unstandardized. * p <.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Relationships between Nature Relatedness and Four Health Variables (From Ordinal Regression Models, Cumulative Link), Accounting for Covariates.
Table 3. Relationships between Nature Relatedness and Four Health Variables (From Ordinal Regression Models, Cumulative Link), Accounting for Covariates.
Health VariableCoefficient (Standard Error)
NR ScoreNR SelfNR PerspectiveNR Experience
Depression0.07(0.08)0.15(0.06) *0.05(0.06)−0.07(0.06)
Anxiety0.13(0.08)0.24(0.06) ***0.05(0.06)−0.04(0.06)
Stress0.17(0.08) *0.19(0.06) **0.15(0.06) *−0.02(0.06)
Self-reported health0.13(0.08)0.08(0.07)−0.08(0.07)0.24(0.07) ***
Note. Covariates were included in all models and full results are shown in the Supplementary Material. For depression, anxiety and stress, the coefficients were derived from ordinal regression models with a cumulative link function. Coefficients are unstandardized. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dean, J.H.; Shanahan, D.F.; Bush, R.; Gaston, K.J.; Lin, B.B.; Barber, E.; Franco, L.; Fuller, R.A. Is Nature Relatedness Associated with Better Mental and Physical Health? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 1371. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15071371

AMA Style

Dean JH, Shanahan DF, Bush R, Gaston KJ, Lin BB, Barber E, Franco L, Fuller RA. Is Nature Relatedness Associated with Better Mental and Physical Health? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(7):1371. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15071371

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dean, Julie H., Danielle F. Shanahan, Robert Bush, Kevin J. Gaston, Brenda B. Lin, Elizabeth Barber, Lara Franco, and Richard A. Fuller. 2018. "Is Nature Relatedness Associated with Better Mental and Physical Health?" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 15, no. 7: 1371. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15071371

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop