Next Article in Journal
Determinants of Patient Delay in Diagnosis of Pulmonary Tuberculosis in Somali Pastoralist Setting of Ethiopia: A Matched Case-Control Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Prevalence and Factors Associated with Psychological Distress among Older Adults Admitted to Hospitals After Fall Injuries in Vietnam
Previous Article in Journal
The Influence of Transactive Memory System on Individual Career Resilience: The Role of Taking Charge and Self-Promotion
Previous Article in Special Issue
Polysubstance Use among Patients Enrolling in Methadone Maintenance Treatment Program in a Vietnam Province with Drug-Driven HIV Epidemic
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Discussion

Variations in the Visual Probe Paradigms for Attention Bias Modification for Substance Use Disorders

1
National Addiction Management Service, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore 539747, Singapore
2
Family Medicine & Primary Care, Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, Nanyang Technological University Singapore, Singapore 308232, Singapore
3
Institute of Mental Health, Department of Developmental Psychiatry, Singapore 539747, Singapore
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16(18), 3389; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph16183389
Submission received: 7 August 2019 / Revised: 28 August 2019 / Accepted: 9 September 2019 / Published: 12 September 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Adult Psychiatry)

Abstract

:
Advances in experimental psychology have provided evidence for the presence of attentional and approach biases in individuals with substance use disorders. Traditionally, reaction time tasks, such as the Stroop or the Visual Probe Task, are commonly used in the assessment of attention biases. The Visual Probe Task has been criticized for its poor reliability, and other research has highlighted that variations remain in the paradigms adopted. However, a gap remains in the published literature, as there have not been any prior studies that have reviewed stimulus timings for different substance use disorders. Such a review is pertinent, as the nature of the task might affect its effectiveness. The aim of this paper was in comparing the different methods used in the Visual Probe Task, by focusing on tasks that have been used for the most highly prevalent substance disorders—that of opiate use, cannabis use and stimulant use disorders. A total of eight published articles were identified for opioid use disorders, three for cannabis use disorders and four for stimulant use disorders. As evident from the synthesis, there is great variability in the paradigm adopted, with most articles including only information about the nature of the stimulus, the number of trials, the timings for the fixation cross and the timings for the stimulus set. Future research examining attentional biases among individuals with substance use disorders should take into consideration the paradigms that are commonly used and evaluate the optimal stimulus and stimulus-onset asynchrony timings.

1. Overview of Attention Bias Assessment and Modification

Advances in experimental psychology have provided evidence for the presence of attentional and approach biases in individuals with substance use disorders. Attentional biases result in individuals having a preferential allocation of their attentional processes to substance-related stimuli [1], while approach biases result in individuals having automatic action tendencies in reaching out for substance-related cues [2]. Various theories have provided explanations for the presence of these biases, including that of the incentive-sensitization theory, the classical conditioning theory and that of the dual-process theory [3]. The dual-process theory is most commonly used in the justification of the presence of attentional biases. It postulates that the repeated use of a substance would result in increased automatic processing and increased automatic tendencies to approach substance-specific cues, with the inhibition of normal cognitive control processes [3]. The discovery and the understanding of these unconscious, automatic biases are of importance clinically, as they help to account for the lapses and relapses among individuals with substance use disorders [4]. Recent neuroimaging studies have highlighted that attentional biases are associated with increased activation in several neuroanatomical regions, including that of the anterior cingulate cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, insula, nucleus accumbens and amygdala [5,6].
Traditionally, reaction time tasks, such as the Stroop or the Visual Probe Task, are commonly used in the assessment of attention biases [1]. In the Stroop task, individuals are required to name the colours of both the neutral and the drug-related words. In the Visual Probe Task, participants are required to respond readily to a probe that would replace either the neutral or the drug image. Attentional biases are deemed to be present if individuals respond more readily to a probe or words that replace substance-related images, as compared to the neutral word or images [1]. Ataya et al. (2012) [7] previously reported that both these tasks (the Stroop and Visual Probe Task) are associated with poor internal reliability. Field et al. (2012) [8] have postulated that one of the factors that could account for the poor reliability of the Visual Probe Task pertains to the type of stimulus used. Thus, the authors proposed the use of a personalised stimulus and images that participants could readily identify with. In turn, this could then result in a more demonstrable change in biases [8]. In a review by Lopes et al. (2015) [9], they reported that the Visual Probe Task was effective—in 88% of the studies involving individuals with substance use disorders, there was successful retraining of attentional biases. Jones et al. (2018) [10], in their recent study, have explored methods to improve the internal reliability of the Visual Probe Task. The authors examined the nature of the stimulus included, adopting the previous suggestion of having a personalised stimulus and reported that the inclusion of a personalised stimulus did help to improve the internal consistency of the Visual Probe Task.
Nevertheless, the authors still report the Visual Probe Task to be unreliable and that reliabilities were acceptable if the stimulus cues were presented for short intervals. Jones et al. (2018) [10], in their review, highlighted that there is great variation in the timings of the stimulus intervals used in published studies involving addiction. However, a gap remains in the published literature, as there have not been any prior studies that have reviewed the stimulus timings for the different substance use disorders. Such a review is pertinent, as the nature of the task might affect its effectiveness. Given this, our aim was to compare the different task paradigms and methods for the Visual Probe Tasks used for the most highly prevalent substance disorders—that of opiate use, cannabis use and stimulant use disorders.

2. Visual Probe Trask Paradigms in Published Studies

Two recent reviews have synthesised the evidence for attentional biases among substance users. Maclean et al. (2018) [11] identified 21 studies that have previously examined attentional biases in opioid using individuals. Zhang et al. (2018) [4] identified 11 articles involving participants with opioid use disorder, 16 articles with participants with stimulant use disorders and nine articles involving participants with cannabis use disorders. In order to fulfil our aim, we will describe the Visual Probe Task paradigms (the methods of the Visual Probe Tasks) that have been used in each of the published studies.
Table 1 provides an overview of the characteristics of the Visual Probe Task that were utilized in previous studies involving individuals with opioid use disorders. From both Maclean et al. (2018)’s [11] and Zhang et al. (2018)’s [4] review, we managed to identify a total of eight articles that specified the use of the Visual Probe Task for attention bias assessment or modification. We were unable to access the full text of one of the journals as it was published in a Chinese Journal. In the identified articles, there was great variability in the number of stimulus included, ranging from 12 to 44 picture pairs. Some studies included as few as 64 trials [12,13], while others included as many as 512 trials [14]. Across the studies, there was great variation in the Visual Probe Task. Most of the studies presented the fixation cross for 500 ms, except Frankland et al. (2016) [15] and Zhao et al. (2017) [16], who presented the fixation cross for 1000 ms. Several studies have presented the stimulus and neutral image set for both a short and long duration [12,13,14,15,17,18]. The short stimulus timing was commonly that of 200 ms, and the long stimulus timing was that of 2000 ms, though, in Frankland et al. (2016)’s [15] study, they presented the images for 500 and 1500 ms as well.
While all the studies explicitly stated that they were based on the Visual Probe Task, there were variations in the nature of the task. Some studies [12,13,18] have included an interstimulus interval, before the presentation of the probe. Also, in some studies, the probe remained on the screen until the participant made a response [14,16,17], while in other studies, the probe only appeared for 100 ms, before disappearing [12,13,18]. Some studies also included an inter-trial interval, but there was variation in the timing of this interval (from 250 to 2000 ms). Some of the studies [15,16,19] have included practice trials. Figure 1 provides a graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for opioid use disorder.
Table 2 provides an overview of the characteristics of the Visual Probe Task that was utilized in previous studies involving individuals with cannabis use disorders. A total of three articles from Zhang et al. (2018)’s [4] prior review were included, as they have reported the use of the Visual Probe Task. For the stimulus, Field et al. (2004) [21] included words instead of pictorial stimuli. In terms of the Visual Probe Task, two studies [21,22] presented the fixation cross for 500 ms, whereas Field et al. (2006) [23] presented it for 1000 ms. There was again variation in the timings for the stimuli cues, with two studies [21,22] presenting the stimulus cue for 500 ms, whereas that of Field et al. (2006) [23] presented it for 2000 ms. In terms of probe presentation, two studies presented the probe [21,22] until a response was made. Vujanovic et al. (2016) [22], presented the probe for 125 or 250 ms. Across all the studies, they have included an inter-trial interval, which ranged from 1000 to 1500 ms. In terms of the number of trials, it ranged between 72 and 96. All the studies included practice trials for participants. Figure 2 provides a graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for cannabis use disorder.
Table 3 provides an overview of the characteristics of the Visual Probe Task that were utilized in the previous studies involving individuals with stimulant use disorders—that of cocaine use disorders. A total of four articles from Zhang et al. (2018)’s [4] prior review was included. While all the studies have their basis in the Visual Probe Task, there was variability in the paradigms. Some studies included 10 sets of images [24], while others [25,26] included up to 20 sets of images. There was variation in the number of trials individuals had to undertake, ranging from 80 to 240 trials. Two studies [25,26] reported the inclusion of practice trials. Three out of the four studies reported that they presented a fixation cross for 500 ms. In terms of stimulus timings, they were presented for 500 ms in three studies [24,25,26] and for a short (200 ms) and long (500 ms) interval in Mayer et al. (2016)’s study [27]. In all of the studies, the probe appeared up until a response was made. Only two of the four identified studies allowed for an inter-trial interval [24,27]. There was variation in the inter-trial interval, as it ranged from 500 to 1500 ms. Figure 3 provides a graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for stimulant use disorder.

3. Implications for Future Research

It is apparent that there is great variability in the paradigm of the Visual Probe Task. In addition, there is also a varied amount of information shared about the nature of the paradigm. Most of the articles included information about the nature of the stimulus, the number of trials, the timings for the fixation cross and the timings for the stimulus set. However, information is missing in some studies, with regards to the inter-stimulus interval, the time that the probe appears for, the inter-trial interview and the time allocated for the individual to response. The absence of this information limits the reproducibility of the Visual Probe Task by others. For future research, it is essential that the intervention is described in full, carefully specifying the full methodology of the Visual Probe Task used, in order to allow for the replication of studies.
While there were clear variations in the paradigms, there were some common elements across all the studies. For studies involving participants with opioid use disorders, most of the studies presented the fixation cross for 500 ms and presented the set of stimulus images for both a short and long stimulus duration. A shorter stimulus duration would allow for the evaluation of the initial, automatic detection attentional processes, while a longer stimulus duration would allow for the evaluation of the engagement stages of attention [9]. In contrast, for studies involving participants with cannabis use or stimulant use disorders, the stimulus pair was most commonly presented for 500 ms. Most of the studies utilizing these timings have provided positive findings for attentional biases, except for Charles et al. (2015) [14] and Mayer et al. (2016) [27].
This evidence synthesis has direct implications for future research. We propose that future studies assessing and modifying attentional biases among individuals with opioid use disorders should consider the use of both a short and long stimulus timing, whereas studies evaluating attentional processes among people using cannabis or with stimulant disorders could use a single stimulus interval. To date, there is only a single study (Mayer et al., 2016) [27] that has examined a varying timing stimulus for individuals with stimulant use disorder. Future research should also examine whether the presence of a varying stimulus timing interval will enhance the detection and modification of attentional biases among individuals with cannabis and stimulant use disorders.
From the studies that we have included, there were a limited number of studies that have reported the stimulus-onset asynchrony timings. Lopes et al. (2015) [9], in their prior review exploring the Visual Probe Task for various disorders, have reported that there was variation in the timings for the different psychiatric disorders. For substance use disorders, it ranged from 50 to 500 ms; for depressive disorders, it ranged from 500 to 2000 ms; and for anxiety disorders, it ranged from 200 to 1500 ms. Lopes et al. (2015) [9] have previously highlighted that a relatively longer stimulus duration is advantageous as it allows for participants to fully process the nature of the stimuli. However, as evident from this evidence synthesis, there are few studies that report on this timing, and this is indeed an area that future research should evaluate to determine the optimal interval for the different substance use disorders.
Our article is perhaps the first article that has reviewed the task paradigms that have been adopted in previously published studies, involving individuals with substance use disorders. We managed to systematically extract information, primarily from the methods section of each manuscript, to ascertain the details of the visual probe paradigms that were utilized. However, there were limitations in our current study. We were unable to access the full text of one of the published articles, as it was published in a Chinese Journal. We have also attempted to contact each of the authors for further details about the Visual Probe Task they have previously used but, to date, we have only managed to receive replies from a single author, who stated that all the details have already been cited in the methods section of the published manuscript.

4. Conclusions

Our article has reviewed all the visual probe paradigms that have been applied previously for addiction and substance research. While there are variations in the underlying paradigms, there are some commonalities as well. Future research examining attentional biases among individuals with substance use disorders should take into consideration the paradigms that are commonly used and evaluate the optimal stimulus and stimulus-onset asynchrony timings.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.Z., D.S.S.F., and H.S.; data collation, M.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, M.Z.; writing—review and editing, D.S.S.F. and H.S. All authors read and approved of the manuscript prior to submission.

Funding

M.Z. is supported by a grant under the Singapore Ministry of Health’s National Medical Research Council (grant number NMRC/Fellowship/0048/2017) for PhD training. The funding source was not involved in any part of this project.

Conflicts of Interest

None of the authors have any competing interests.

References

  1. Field, M.; Cox, W.M. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008, 97, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Eberl, C.; Wiers, R.W.; Pawelczack, S.; Rinck, M.; Becker, E.S.; Lindenmeyer, J. Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 4, 38–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Stacy, A.W.; Wiers, R.W. Implicit cognition and addiction: A tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 6, 551–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Melvyn, Z.W.; Ying, J.; Wing, T.; Guo, S.; Fung, D.S.; Smith, H. Cognitive Biases in Cannabis, Opioid, and Stimulant Disorders: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychiatry 2018, 9, 376. [Google Scholar] [Green Version]
  5. Vollstädt-Klein, S.; Loeber, S.; Richter, A.; Kirsch, M.; Bach, P.; von der Goltz, C.; Hermann, D.; Mann, K.; Kiefer, F. Validating incentive salience with functional magnetic resonance imaging: Association between mesolimbic cue reactivity and attentional bias in alcohol-dependent patients. Addict. Biol. 2012, 17, 807–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Hester, R.; Garavan, H. Neural mechanisms underlying drug-related cue distraction in active cocaine users. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2009, 93, 270–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Ataya, A.F.; Adams, S.; Mullings, E.; Cooper, R.M.; Attwood, A.S.; Munafo, M.R. Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012, 121, 148–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Field, M.; Christiansen, P. Commentary on, ‘Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias’. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012, 124, 189–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Lopes, F.M.; Viacava, K.R.; Bizarro, L. Attentional bias modification based on visual probe task: Methodological issues, results and clinical relevance. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2015, 37, 183–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Jones, A.; Christiansen, P.; Field, M. Failed attempts to improve the reliability of the alcohol visual probe task following empirical recommendations. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2018, 32, 922–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. MacLean, R.R.; Sofuoglu, M.; Brede, E.; Robinson, C.; Waters, A.J. Attentional bias in opioid users: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018, 191, 270–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Garland, E.L.; Howard, M.O. Opioid attentional bias and cue-elicited craving predict future risk of prescription opioid misuse among chronic pain patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014, 144, 283–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Garland, E.L.; Froeliger, B.E.; Passik, S.D.; Howard, M.O. Attentional bias for prescription opioid cues among opioid dependent chronic pain patients. J. Behav. Med. 2013, 36, 611–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Charles, M.; Wellington, C.E.; Mokrysz, C.; Freeman, T.P.; O’Ryan, D.; Curran, H.V. Attentional bias and treatment adherence in substitute-prescribed opiate users. Addict. Behav. 2015, 46, 100–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Frankland, L.; Bradley, B.P.; Mogg, K. Time course of attentional bias to drug cues in opioid dependence. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2016, 30, 601–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Zhao, H.; Yang, B.; Zhu, Q.; Zhang, G.; Xiao, Y.; Guo, X.; Huang, X.; Zhang, Z. Eye Movement Evidence of Attentional Bias for Substance-Related Cues in Heroin Dependents on Methadone Maintenance Therapy. Subst. Use Misuse 2017, 52, 527–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Constantinou, N.; Morgan, C.J.; Battistella, S.; O’Ryan, D.; Davis, P.; Curran, H.V. Attentional bias, inhibitory control and acute stress in current and former opiate addicts. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010, 109, 220–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Garland, E.L.; Froeliger, B.; Howard, M.O. Allostatic dysregulation of natural reward processing in prescription opioid misuse: Autonomic and attentional evidence. Biol. Psychol. 2015, 105, 124–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Lubman, D.I.; Peters, L.A.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P.; Deakin, J.F.W. Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence. Psychol. Med. 2000, 30, 169–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Tull, M.T.; McDermott, M.J.; Gratz, K.L.; Coffey, S.F.; Lejuez, C.W. Cocaine-related attentional bias following trauma cue exposure among cocaine dependent in-patients with and without post-traumatic stress disorder. Addiction 2011, 106, 1810–1818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Field, M.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P. Cognitive bias and drug craving in recreational cannabis users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004, 74, 105–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Vujanovic, A.A.; Wardle, M.C.; Liu, S.; Dias, N.R.; Lane, S.D. Attentional bias in adults with cannabis use disorders. J. Addict. Dis. 2016, 35, 144–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Field, M. Cannabis ‘dependence’ and attentional bias for cannabis-related words. Behav. Pharmacol. 2005, 16, 473–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Montgomery, C.; Field, M.; Atkinson, A.M.; Cole, J.C.; Goudie, A.J.; Sumnall, H.R. Effects of alcohol preload on attentional bias towards cocaine-related cues. Psychopharmacology 2010, 210, 365–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Bardeen, J.R.; Dixon-Gordon, L.K.; Tull, M.T.; Lyons, J.A.; Gratz, K.L. An investigation of the relationship between borderline personality disorder and cocaine-related attentional bias following trauma cue exposure: The moderating role of gender. Compr. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 113–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Mayer, A.R.; Wilcox, C.E.; Dodd, A.B.; Klimaj, S.D.; Dekonenko, C.J.; Claus, E.D.; Bogenschutz, M. The efficacy of attention bias modification therapy in cocaine use disorders. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 2016, 42, 459–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Wei, Z.; Cai, H.; Zhao, Q.; Han, Q.; Ma, E.; Zhao, G.; Peng, H. An ERP Study of Attentional Bias to Drug Cues in Heroin Dependence by Using Dot-Probe Task. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Human Centered Computing, Colombo, Sri Lanka, 7–9 January 2016; Springer: Cham, Switzerland; pp. 794–799. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. A graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for opioid use disorder (n = 8). Green highlights: reported in study; red highlights: not reported in study.
Figure 1. A graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for opioid use disorder (n = 8). Green highlights: reported in study; red highlights: not reported in study.
Ijerph 16 03389 g001
Figure 2. A graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for cannabis use disorder (n = 3). Green highlights: reported in study; red highlights: not reported in study.
Figure 2. A graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for cannabis use disorder (n = 3). Green highlights: reported in study; red highlights: not reported in study.
Ijerph 16 03389 g002
Figure 3. A graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for stimulant use disorder (n = 4). Green highlights: reported in study; red highlights: not reported in study.
Figure 3. A graphical representation of the details of the Visual Probe Task that were reported in each of the identified studies for stimulant use disorder (n = 4). Green highlights: reported in study; red highlights: not reported in study.
Ijerph 16 03389 g003
Table 1. Characteristics of the Visual Probe Task used in previous studies involving individuals with opioid dependence (n = 8).
Table 1. Characteristics of the Visual Probe Task used in previous studies involving individuals with opioid dependence (n = 8).
Study ParticipantsIntervention DetailsNature of Stimulus IncludedDetails of Assessment TaskOutcomes
Charles et al. (2015) [14]In total, 23 opiate users and 21 healthy controlsDot Probe Task for attention retraining in the lab.
Participants maintained on Buprenorphine and Methadone Maintenance Therapy (MMT)
In total, 44 picture pairs
In total, 40 pairs matched for visual complexity and composition, contained opiate-related items (spoons, needles, lighters, heroin-like substance)
The other four pairs were neutral images only
Fixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: Short (200 ms) or Long (500 ms)
Probe: Arrow pointing upwards or downwards, remained on screen until participant responded
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: 250–500 ms
Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA): Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 64 (assessment), 512 (Intervention)
Total number of neutral trials: 16
Total number of practice trials: Not mentioned
No baseline difference in attention bias between patients and controls
No significant effect of Attention Bias Modification (ABM) on Attention Bias (AB) or substance cravings
Constantinou et al. (2010) [17]In total, 16 opiate users in treatment, 16 ex-users, and 16 healthy controlsDot Probe Task for attention retraining in the lab.
Participants maintained on methadone maintenance therapy
In total, 40 picture pairs
Of which, 20 pairs matched for visual complexity and composition, and contained one opiate related picture and one non opiate related picture
The other 20 pairs were neutral pairs
Fixation Cross Timing: Not mentioned
Stimulus Pair Timing: Short (200 ms) or Long (2000 ms)
Probe: Probe appeared on the side of the screen where one of the pictures have been previously
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: Not mentioned
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 160
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: Not mentioned
Greater attentional biases towards drug-related stimuli for current users, as compared to ex-users.
Ex-users showed a bias away from drug-related stimuli in the stress condition and this correlated positively with their length of abstinence
Frankland et al. (2016) [15]In total, 19 were opioid dependent and 20 healthy controlsDot Probe Task for attention retraining in the lab.
Participants maintained on partial MMT
In total, 14 drug-related images (images of drug paraphernalia and an unidentified addict appearing to cook up and inject heroin),
Another 14 pairs matched for control pictures (items from a children’s building game, and a person building a model railway)
Fixation Cross Timing: 1000 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 200, 500 and 1000 ms
Probe: Probe replaced the images. Participants to press one of the buttons as quickly as quickly as possible, without making mistakes, to indicate whether the probe appeared on the left- or right-hand side.
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: Not mentioned
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 164
Total number of neutral trials: 84
Total number of practice trials: 10
Opioid dependent group had a significant attentional bias for opioid related information presented at 200 ms and 500 ms.
No attentional biases at 150 0ms.
Garland et al. (2013) [13]In total, 32 were opioid dependent and
33 non opioid-dependent
Dot Probe Task for attentional retraining in the lab
Participants maintained on partial buprenorphine and MMT
In total, 12 opioid images, including photos of pill (Oxycontin, Vicodin), pill bottles, crushed and powdered opioids for insufflation, and a syringe next to a vial of injectable morphine
Neutral images included 12 photos from the International Affective Picture System, depicting household items
Fixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 200 or 2000 ms
Probe: Target probe will replace the images after 50 ms inter-stimulus interval
Probe Timing: 100 ms
Inter-trial interval: Not mentioned
SOA: 50 ms
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 64
Total number of neutral trials: 12
Total number of practice trials: Not mentioned
Opioid-dependent individuals had significant attention bias towards opioids cues presented for 200 ms but not for cues presented for 2000 ms
Garland and Howard et al. (2014) [12]In total, 28 with a high risk for misuse and
19 with a low risk for misuse
Dot Probe Task for attentional retraining in the lab
In total, 12 opioid images, including photos of pill (Oxycontin, Vicodin), pill bottles, crushed and powdered opioids for insufflation
Neutral images included 12 photos from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS), depicting household items
Fixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 200 or 2000 ms
Probe: Target probe will replace the images after 50ms inter-stimulus interval
Probe Timing: 100 ms
Inter-trial interval: Not mentioned
SOA: 50 ms
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 64
Total number of neutral trials: 12
Total number of practice trials: Not mentioned
Biased initial attentional orienting to prescription opioid cues
Garland et al. (2015) [18]In total, 72 opioid misusers and 26 opioids non misusersDot Probe Task for attentional retraining in the labPair of photos containing one emotionally salient image and one neutral image
Three blocks of cues (opioid-related, pain-related, and pleasure-related cues)
Opioid-related cues included images of pills and bottles
Pain-related cues included images of severe injuries, painful medical procedures, and human faces grimacing in pain
Natural reward cues included images of romantic couples and food
In total, 36 neutral images selected from International Affective Picture System (IAPS)
Fixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 200 or 2000 ms
Half of the trials presented at 200ms, half 2000 ms.
Probe: Target probe will replace the images after 50 ms inter-stimulus interval
Probe Timing: 100 ms
Inter-trial interval: Not mentioned
SOA: 50 ms
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 64
Total number of neutral trials: 12
Total number of practice trials: Not mentioned
Opioid misusers exhibit attentional deficits during reward processing
Lubman et al. (2000) [19]In total, 16 methadone-maintained addicts and 16 age-matched controlsPictorial Probe Detection taskDrug-related photographs (drug paraphernalia, for example, needles, spoons and heroin wraps)
Scenes of unidentified addict cooking up and injecting heroin.
Control photographs included items from children building a game and scenes showing an unidentified person building a model railway using various components from the set
Fixation Cross Timing: 1000 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 500 ms
Probe: After each pair of images, a dot probe will appear in the position of one of the pictures
Participants are to press as quickly as possible, one of the two response buttons.
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: Not mentioned
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 160
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 12
Attentional biases in opiate addicts to probe that replaced the drug pictures, rather than neutral images
Wei et al. (2016) [20]In total, 22 heroin addicts and 22 healthy controlsVisual Probe TaskIn total, 10 heroin images
The other 10 were natural scenery pictures
Fixation Cross Timing: 1000 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 500 ms
Probe: Appear in the location where one of the images disappear
Probe Timing: 200 ms
Inter-trial interval: 1350 ms
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 80 and 240
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 20
Heroin addicts have more rapid response when the dot located on the heroin-related picture as compared to the neutral picture
Zhao et al. (2017) [16]In total, 30 methadone-maintained outpatients and
38 healthy controls
Visual Probe TaskDrug neutral picture pairs and neutral–neutral picture pairs.
In total, 20 pictures of substance-related scenes and 20 similar pictures matched with same layout but lacking substance-related cues
Another 20 pairs of pictures matched with similar neutral scenes of daily life
Fixation Cross Timing: 1000 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 2000 ms
Probe: Probe then displayed in one of the positions of the pictures
Probe remained until the participant responded by clicking the left or right button of mouse
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: 200 ms
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 120
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 16
Heroin group reacted faster to probes associated with substance-related pictures than neutral pictures
More initial fixations
Maintained longer initial fixation durations towards substance-related pictures
Table 2. Characteristics of the Visual Probe Task used in previous studies involving individuals with cannabis dependence.
Table 2. Characteristics of the Visual Probe Task used in previous studies involving individuals with cannabis dependence.
Study ParticipantsIntervention DetailsNature of Stimulus IncludedDetails of Assessment TaskOutcomes
Field et al. (2004) [21]In total, 17 regular cannabis users and 16 non-usersVisual Probe TaskCannabis-related words, environment-related words, pleasant words, and unpleasant wordsFixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 500 ms
Probe: Immediately after the offset of the words, a small dot probe was presented in the position of one of the words, until the participant gave a manual response.
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: 1000 ms
Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA): Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 64
Total number of neutral trials: 32
Total number of practice trials: 12
High levels of craving associated with significant attention bias for cannabis-related words
Field et al. (2006) [23]In total, 23 regular cannabis users and 23 non-user controlsVisual Probe Task with concurrent eye movement monitoringIn total, 18 cannabis-related photographs
Depicting a scene relating to cannabis use
Control photograph (did not contain cannabis related content)
Fixation Cross Timing: 1000 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 2000 ms
Probe: After picture offset, small visual probe (an arrow which is pointed up or down) was presented on the left or right of the screen
Respond to the probe as quickly as possible
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: 500 ms
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 72
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 10
Regular users had biases to maintain gaze on cannabis cues and faster approach responses to cannabis cues
Vujanovic et al. (2016) [22]In total, 12 adults with cannabis use disorder and 13 controlsVisual Probe TaskIn total, six cannabis pictures—cannabis-related stimuli— and six neutral pictures matched in size, colour and contextFixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 500 ms
Probe: Probe stimulus removed, picture (cue) remained on screen 1500 ms
Probe Timing: 125 or 250 ms
Inter-trial interval: 1500 ms
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: 1500 ms
Total number of critical trials: 96
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 12
Cannabis use group showed greater attentional biases to cannabis cues at the 125 ms probe time
Table 3. Characteristics of the Visual Probe Task used in previous studies involving individuals with stimulant dependence.
Table 3. Characteristics of the Visual Probe Task used in previous studies involving individuals with stimulant dependence.
StudyParticipantsIntervention DetailsNature of Stimulus IncludedDetails of Assessment TaskOutcomes
Montgomery et al. (2010) [24]In total, 32 regular cocaine users and 40 nonusersVisual Probe Task and Modified Stroop TaskIn total, 10 pairs of images, with one cocaine-related image depicting cocaine, cocaine paraphernalia, or close up of an individual using cocaine
Matched with a neutral image, perceptually similar to cocaine image as possible, but did not have cocaine content
Fixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 500 ms
Probe: Visual probe (an arrow pointing up or down) presented in the location previously occupied by one of the pictures
Required to rapidly identify the orientation of the arrow probe by pressing the appropriate arrow on the keyboard
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: 500 ms
Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA): Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 80
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 10
Cocaine participants who consumed alcohol had increased attentional biases for cocaine pictures
Tull et al. (2011) [26]In total, 30 cocaine-dependent patients with Post-traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and 30 cocaine-dependent patients without PTSDVisual Probe TaskIn total, 20 cocaine-related images (crack rocks, powder cocaine, crack pipes, etc.) and 40 images of furnitureFixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 500 ms
Probe: Dot probe appeared in the left or right position, remaining until the participant responded
Indicate where the dot appeared by pressing one of the two response keys.
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: 250 ms
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 240
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 5
PTSD participants have had greater attentional biases towards the location of cocaine imagery than non-PTSD participants
Bardeen et al. (2013) [25]In total, 22 cocaine-dependent patients with borderline personality disorder and 36 cocaine-dependent patients without borderline personality disorderVisual Probe TaskIn total, 20 cocaine-related pictures (crack pipes, crack rocks, etc.) and 40 pictures of furnitureFixation Cross Timing: 500 ms
Stimulus Pair Timing: 500 ms
Probe: Dot appeared on the screen replacing one of the two pictures.
Press a button on the computer keyboard that corresponded to the relative position of the dot on the screen
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: Not mentioned
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: Not mentioned
Total number of critical trials: 240
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 5
Greater bias for attending to cocaine stimuli among male cocaine-dependent patients with or without borderline personality disorder, when presented with a trauma script intervention
Mayer et al. (2016) [27]In total, 37 participants randomly assigned to Attention Bias Modification Therapt (ABMT) or control therapyVisual Probe TaskCocaine and neutral stimuli were equivalent in size and visual angle, generally matched for colour and contentFixation Cross Timing: Not mentioned
Stimulus Pair Timing: Stimulus pairs presented for 200 ms (speed detection trials) or 500 (difficulty to disengage trials).
Probe: Stimulus replaced by a probe (arrow)
Probe Timing: Not mentioned
Inter-trial interval: 1000 or 1500 ms
SOA: Not mentioned
Response Timing: 1500 ms
Total number of critical trials: 240
Total number of neutral trials: Not mentioned
Total number of practice trials: 5
Attention bias modification was not more effective than control at reducing attentional biases

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhang, M.; Fung, D.S.S.; Smith, H. Variations in the Visual Probe Paradigms for Attention Bias Modification for Substance Use Disorders. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3389. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph16183389

AMA Style

Zhang M, Fung DSS, Smith H. Variations in the Visual Probe Paradigms for Attention Bias Modification for Substance Use Disorders. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019; 16(18):3389. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph16183389

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhang, Melvyn, Daniel S. S. Fung, and Helen Smith. 2019. "Variations in the Visual Probe Paradigms for Attention Bias Modification for Substance Use Disorders" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16, no. 18: 3389. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph16183389

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop