Next Article in Journal
Perfluoroalkyl Chemicals and Male Reproductive Health: Do PFOA and PFOS Increase Risk for Male Infertility?
Next Article in Special Issue
Students’ Perceptions of Instructional Rubrics in Neurological Physical Therapy and Their Effects on Students’ Engagement and Course Satisfaction
Previous Article in Journal
A Model for Determining Predictors of the MUAC in Acute Malnutrition in Ghana
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Role of Gender and Age in the Emotional Well-Being Outcomes of Young Adults
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Regulation/Non-Regulation/Dys-Regulation of Health Behavior, Psychological Reactance, and Health of University Undergraduate Students

by
Mónica Pachón-Basallo
1,
Jesús de la Fuente
1,2,* and
María Carmen Gonzáles-Torres
1
1
Department of Theory and Methods of Evaluation and Educational and Psychological Intervention, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, 31009 Pamplona, Navarra, Spain
2
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Almeria, 04120 La Cañada, Almería, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(7), 3793; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18073793
Submission received: 11 February 2021 / Revised: 25 March 2021 / Accepted: 1 April 2021 / Published: 5 April 2021

Abstract

:
The Self-Regulation vs. External-Regulation Theory (2017) has postulated a continuum of regulation/non-regulation/dys-regulation that is present both in the individual and in the individual’s context. This gives rise to a behavioral heuristic that can predict and explain other health-related variables, such as psychological reactance and student health. On a voluntary basis, 269 university students completed validated questionnaires on variables of regulation, reactance and health. Using an ex post facto design, we performed correlational analysis and structural linear regression to build a structural equations model (SEM) with acceptable statistical values. The results showed various predicted relationships: self-regulation was associated with and positively predicted self-regulated health behavior; external health-regulating contexts were associated with and positively predicted self-regulated health behavior; non-regulatory and dysregulatory contexts negatively predicted self-regulated health behavior and students’ health itself, as well as positively predicting psychological reactance behavior. Implications are established for explaining variability in general and health-related self-regulation, as well as for intervening in these variables in health programs.

1. Introduction

University students’ health has become the object of research. Prior studies have revealed certain factors that affect student health, such as academic or money-related worries [1]. Other factors that show statistically significant association with students’ well-being are frequency of physical activity, current tobacco use, sleep quality, depression and the availability of mental health services [2,3]. The World Health Organization (WHO) [3] adds that development of skills to maintain interpersonal relationships, to face problematic situations, and to manage one’s emotions are essential for the health of this group of young people.
In this context, self-regulation (SR) behavior has been found to positively predict flourishing and student health, as well as to negatively predict procrastination [4]. Similarly, psychological reactance has been positively correlated with greater perceived loneliness and behavioral problems in adolescents and youths [5]. Therefore, the purpose of this research was to use correlations and a structural equations model (SEM) to establish whether the health-regulatory levels of the individual and of their context were linearly related to the individual’s general self-regulation, psychological reactance and health.

1.1. Self-Regulation Behavior (SR)

Lazarus and Folkman (1984) [6] conceptualized self-regulation as the set of processes and behaviors that support pursuit of personal goals within a changing external environment. Brown et al. (1999) [7] evaluated individuals’ behavioral self-regulation, considering it their ability to plan and manage their own behavior in a flexible way, according to desired outcomes.
Sufficient research evidence has established SR as a meta-ability [8,9] that it is associated with different personality variables, such as coping strategies [10]. It has also been found to positively predict health and flourishing [4]. Complementarily, self-regulation has shown a significant, negative association with poor sleep quality in an average university population, as well as negative predictive value of smartphone addiction [11]. Levels of self-regulation have also been found to correlate positively with degree of well-being [12]. Recent studies have proposed self-regulation as an explanatory cross-diagnostic variable, where different diagnostic categories converge [13,14,15,16].
Research has evolved, however, allowing us to move from Brown’s initial general self-regulation concept [17] toward new conceptions that imply both (1) a gradation in types of self-regulated behavior, and (2) the consideration of both internal and external factors in one’s self-regulation. A heuristic that takes into account this new conception is included in the theory presented below.
Based on this new model and on previous studies by De la Fuente et al. using Brown’s Self-Regulation Questionnaire, specific scales have been developed to examine these different aspects of regulation [18].

1.2. The Theory of Self Regulation vs. External Regulation

The theoretical model of SR versus external regulation (ER) [9] is complementary to Brown’s prior theory [17] of self-regulated behavior. In both theories, self-regulated behavior is a variable that makes possible skill acquisition, that is, it refers to a self-directed process where a subject transforms his or her mental abilities into practical skills in multiple contexts [19].
SR vs. ER theory [9,20,21,22,23,24,25,26], when applied to health, seeks to explain the combination of external and internal conditions that predispose adequate behavior in health situations. SR vs. ER theory emerged in the field of self-regulated learning [27,28], but has evolved toward the study of general self-regulated behavior [4] and toward self-regulated health behavior, of university students in particular [25,29]. It can be described as follows.

1.2.1. Self-Regulation, Non-Regulation and Dys-Regulation of Health Behavior

Self-regulated health behavior is understood as the ability to plan and manage one’s own health-related behavior in a flexible way, in accordance with desired outcomes. The initial theoretical conception of self-regulation, non-regulation and dys-regulation (SR-NR-DR) has been applied to three levels of health regulation. Individuals’ level of competence in the area of health behavior can be classified into three regulatory levels. Levels 1, 2, 3 indicate a descending gradation (high-medium-low) in level of personal health regulation: self-regulated health (SRH), the most proactive self-regulatory level; non-regulation of health (NRH), a middle level that is not proactive; and dys-regulation of health, where negative proactive behaviors may even appear (DRH) [8,21,24,30]:
(1)
Self-Regulated Health behavior (SRH) reflects an individual’s positive proactivity, that is, active and adequate management of their own health-related behavior [28,31]. Previous research has shown that people can have different degrees of self-regulation [8,20,32].
(2)
Non-Regulation of Health behavior (NRH) refers to lack of proactivity and the absence of personal health-regulating behaviors. This is the conceptual equivalent of reactivity [19,25,33]. In this case, the student is fully dependent on whatever external regulation is provided by the context.
(3)
Dys-Regulated Health behavior (DRH) refers to a degree of negative proactivity in one’s behavior, that is, active but inadequate behavior. The individual may find advantages in such dysregulation, which enables them to avoid the effort involved in positive, proactive health regulation. Examples include self-handicapping strategies, procrastination in matters of health or other risky behaviors [34,35,36,37,38,39].
Based on Zimmerman’s model [27,28], Principle 1 of SR vs. ER theory [9,20,40] suggests a graded continuum of health-behavior regulation. In this framework, specific behaviors carried out before, during and after a given health-related behavior have been described. See Table 1.

1.2.2. Contexts of External Regulation, External Non-Regulation, and External Dys-Regulation of Health Behavior

A context that is externally regulatory in health is defined as providing environmental stimuli that predispose, to a greater or lesser degree, self-regulated health behavior. Similar to internal, self-regulation, external contexts can also be classified into three levels of regulation of health behavior. In external regulation of health behavior, the context encourages the individual to make adequate use of the different health-regulating competencies [10,22,23,24,25,26,41]: externally-regulated health behavior (ERH) is the most proactive level of a health-promoting context; external non-regulation of health behavior (ENH) is a context that does not offer external support to the student; finally, a context which is externally dysregulatory toward health behavior (EDH) is characterized by unhealthful factors (stress triggers in the teaching process, prompting negative achievement emotions, etc.):
(1)
Externally-regulated health behavior (ERH). In this context, individuals are encouraged to show positive or adequate proactivity and to practice self-regulation of health [42]. External indicators or promptings promote and increase the likelihood of self-regulated health behaviors. These may take the form of antecedents (patterns, norms, limits, expectations of successful health regulation, value given to health regulation) or of contextual consequences (positive and negative contingencies that favor health regulation, adaptation, etc.). Positive events that result in regulated health are strongly predicted in this context [43]).
(2)
External non-regulation of health behavior (ENH). The context does nothing to encourage one’s personal health regulation, but it does not contain health-dysregulating factors. No external indicators or promptings encourage self-regulated or dysregulated health behaviors, nor do they increase the likelihood of one or the other. A non-regulatory context requires the individual to employ a moderate level of their own health-regulating behavior, given that contextual elements offer no direction. Positive events that result in regulated health are not predicted in this context [44]).
(3)
External dys-regulation of health (EDH), actively promoting non-adaptive health behaviors. Inadequate or negative proactivity is encouraged in this type of context. There are many external indicators that encourage and increase the likelihood of active, dysregulating behavior. This behavior is also stimulated by contextual antecedents (modeling, rules, limits, negative expectations of self-regulation, negative value given to self-regulation, etc.) and by contextual consequences (positive and negative contingencies, molding, etc.) that encourage dysregulation. A great deal of effort is required from the individual who wishes to practice health-regulating behavior in this context. This context is highly predictive of negative events that result in dysregulated health.
In this case, Principle 2 of SR vs. ER Theory [9,20,40], also based on Zimmerman’s model [26,27], establishes the specific external, contextual characteristics that encourage or hinder self-regulation. The behavioral sequence is the same as in Zimmerman’s model: before-during-after. See Table 2.

1.2.3. Combinations of Self/External Regulation, Non-Regulation, and Dys-Regulation of Health

Principle 3 of SR vs. ER [9,20] establishes interactivity between the previous levels. Prior research presented a heuristic of regulation combinations that can explain variability in the academic sphere and is able to explain and predict different academic learning behaviors [9,22,23,24,25,26]. In similar fashion, the student’s level of self-regulated health behavior combines with levels of external regulation offered by the context. It is plausible to assume that behavioral contexts may interact with personal levels of self-regulation in order to explain diversity in student health behaviors. An illustration from the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) situation was recently published (de la Fuente, Kauffman, Dempsey and Kauffman, 2021) [40].

1.3. Psychological Reactance

Psychological reactance is a motivational state that individuals may adopt when they perceive that a freedom is eliminated or threatened. It is an unpleasant provocation that motivates them to act to restore their autonomy [45,46,47]. Laird and Frazer [5] found that high levels of behavior problems can predict high levels of reactance and perceived psychological control. Likewise, high levels of reactance can lead adolescents and youths to interpret positive or ambiguous parenting behaviors as compatible with psychological control. Hence, when individuals experience psychological reactance, this may indicate a perception that the threatened behaviors or thoughts are more attractive, producing a desire to reestablish them [46,48].
Empirically, psychological reactance has been negatively correlated to self-assessments of happiness and indices of self-esteem, and positively correlated to perceived loneliness and fear of failure. Moreover, persons with psychological reactance tend to have more violent relationships [49,50]. Hong and Faeedda [51] indicated that a high level of psychological reactance could explain a person’s motives for resisting social influence, the invasion of their personal space, or influence from psychotherapy. On the other hand, Steindl and collaborators [45] indicated that psychological reactance can be triggered vicariously through indirect reactions; for this reason, reactance is also studied from a contextual and cultural standpoint. Reactance has been investigated from the spheres of neurophysiology, social psychology, clinical psychology, the psychology of motivation, and so on. Psychological reactance shows obvious potential as a mediator in human interaction, clearly warranting more in-depth study as a critically important construct for both scientists and professionals.

1.4. Student Health

Health is a state of social, mental and physical well-being, not merely the absence of disease. It is important to investigate student health, given that half of all mental disorders start by the age of 14; in most cases, however, they are neither detected nor treated [3,52,53]. Physical health ranges across a spectrum from high levels of energy with no chronic conditions or symptoms, to the opposite end of having severe disability. Some of the practices relating to the physical health of youth people are: regularity in eating, regularity of physical activity, and the use of psychoactive substances like alcohol and tobacco [42,54].
Psychologically, we speak of health when the student enjoys a set of characteristics such as positive emotions, enthusiasm, vitality, self-determination, and subjective perception of well-being [55,56]. On the other hand, a significant quantity of research indicates the importance of university students’ self-assessed health; when this assessment is unsatisfactory, it is associated with sedentarism, headaches and insomnia and with lower quality of life [57,58,59]. In the student population, a low level of well-being has been related to low academic achievement and high levels of procrastination [4,60]. Recent research has studied how physical and psychological health mutually affect one another [61], so student health can be approached from the standpoint of well-being, as proposed by the WHO. In relation to the aforementioned variables, a positive correlation has been found between levels of self-regulation and one’s degree of physical and psychological well-being [4].

1.5. Objectives and Hypotheses

Given the prior evidence, the general aim of the current study was to establish possible linear associations and structural prediction relationships between self-regulated health behavior and external regulation of health behavior, with respect to psychological reactance and student health.
The association hypotheses were as follows:
Hypothesis 1. 
Self-regulated behavior (SR) is expected to show a positive association with self-regulation of health (SRH), and a negative association with non-regulation (NRH) and dys-regulation of health (DRH); this directionality will be identical in the case of the external regulatory factors, that is, external regulation of health (ERH), external non-regulation of health (ENH) and external dysregulation of health (EDH).
Hypothesis 2. 
In a complementary way, self-regulation of health (SRH) is expected to have a negative association with non-regulation of health (NRH) and dys-regulation of health (DRH); also, external regulation of health (ERH) will show a negative association with external non-regulation of health (ENH) and external dys-regulation of health (EDH).
Hypothesis 3. 
External non-regulation of health (ENH) will be positively related to personal non-regulation of health (NRH); and external dys-regulation of health (EDH) will be positively associated with personal dys-regulation of health (DRH).
Hypothesis 4. 
External regulation of health (ERH) will be positively associated with student health (SH); dys-regulation of health (DRH) will be positively associated with psychological reactance (REACT) and negatively with student health (SH); and external regulation of health (ERH) will be negatively associated with reactance (REACT) and positively with student health (SH).
Hypothesis 5. 
Psychological reactance (PR) will be negatively associated with student health (SH) and with self-regulation of health (SRH).
The prediction hypotheses were as follows:
Hypothesis 6. 
Each level of external regulation will predict the same level in self-regulation: external regulation of health (ERH) will predict internal, self-regulation of health (SRH); external non-regulation of health (ENH) will predict internal non-regulation of health (NRH); external dysregulation of health (DRH) will predict internal dysregulation of health (DRH).
Hypothesis 7. 
External contexts that are non-regulating or dysregulating toward health, together with personal dysregulation of health behavior, will jointly predict psychological reactance, and the latter will negatively predict general self-regulation and student health (physical and psychological); by contrast, external health-regulating contexts in conjunction with personal self-regulated health will positively predict general self-regulation and also student health.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Participating in the study were 269 undergraduate students from different universities. The percentage of responses from the students invited to participate was 98.5% (n = 265). The sample contained students enrolled in psychology, primary education, and law degrees; 66.9% were women and 18.3% were men. Average age was 22.82 years (sd = 2.29), age range was from 18 to 29. The sample could not be randomized, hence sampling was incidental and non-probabilistic. The students completed self-reports in reference to their health behaviors. All participation was anonymous and on a voluntary basis.

2.2. Instruments

Self-Regulation Behavior. Measured by the Short Self-Regulation Questionnaire (SSRQ) [62]. The Spanish adaptation had been previously validated in Spanish samples, and showed acceptable validity and reliability, with values similar to the English version. The Short SRQ comprises [18,63] four factors: goal setting-planning (I usually keep track of my progress toward my study goals), perseverance (I am able to resist temptation), decision making (I have so many plans that it’s hard for me to focus on any one of them) and learning from mistakes (I usually only have to make a mistake once in order to learn from it). There are 17 items (all of them with saturations >0.40), and a consistent confirmatory factor structure in this sample—Chi-square = 439.699, df = 113, p < 0.001; Ch/df = 3.89; standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) = 0.088; comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.959, GFI = 0.951, AGFI = 0.97, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.076. Internal consistency was acceptable for the questionnaire total (α = 0.87) and for the factors: goal setting-planning (α = 0.79), decision making (α = 0.72), perseverance (a = 0.75), and learning from mistakes (α = 0.72).
Self-Regulation vs. External Regulation of Health. This questionnaire [64] has a structure of 6 subscales with six items each. It assesses health-regulating aspects of both the person and the context. The behaviors assessed may be regulatory [internal: I think consciously about my health needs, external: The social context that I live in (family, environment, friends) helps me plan my health-related behavior by setting goals and objectives]; non-regulatory [internal: It is not necessary to make decisions in order to achieve changes in my health-related behaviors, external: The social context that I live in (family, environment, friends) gives me the idea that you do not need to make specific decisions in order to achieve changes in your health-related behaviors]; or dysregulatory (internal: It does not make sense to change your health-related behavior, if that takes away from your enjoyment and satisfaction, external: The social context that I live in (family, environment, friends) helps me enjoy myself to the fullest, since it does not press me to change my health-related behavior, but rather to do what I feel like, if that makes me happy and live fully]. Factor structure, as analyzed in this sample, is consistent [Chi Square = 1348.005, df = 583, p < 0.001; Ch/df = 2.379; RMSR = 0.035; NFI = 0.967; RFI = 0.954; incremental fit index (IFI) = 0.902; TLI = 0.967; CFI = 0.978; RMSEA = 0.70]. Total reliability values were also acceptable [alpha total = 0.776]. Subscale consistency was also acceptable: SRH = 0.847; NRH = 0.779; DRH = 0.769; ERH = 0.900; ENH = 0.761; EDH = 0.828. See Table 3 for a conceptual listing of the types of subscales and their hypothesized relationships.
Psychological reactance measured by the Escala de Reactancia Psicológica (Scale of psychological reactance) [40]. The scale structure consists of four subscales; however, our exploratory and confirmatory analysis showed three scales in our sample. Construct structural values were adequate in this sample [Chi-square = 100.301, df = 41, p < 0.001; Ch/df = 2.446; RMR = 0.042; CFI = 0.929 GFI = 0.917; IFI = 0.918; TLI = 0.927; CFI = 0.915; RMSEA = 0.073]. Total reliability values were also acceptable [alpha total = 0.734]. Some examples of scale items are: Advice and recommendations usually prompt me to do just the opposite, and, Regulations trigger a sense of resistance in me.
Student Health [4]. Measured by the Cuestionario de Salud Académica (Student Health Inventory). This self-report scale assesses students’ physical health (I have good eating habits/I sleep well) as well as psychological health (I feel depressed by studies, I properly balance homework with recreational activities). The structure consists of two subscales. Construct structural values were adequate in this sample [Chi-square = 41.385, df = 19; p < 0.001; Ch/df = 2.446; RMR = 0.021; CFI = 0.946; GFI = 0.947; IFI = 0.941; TLI = 0.911; CFI = 0.940; RMSEA= 0.073]. Total reliability values were also acceptable [alpha total = 0.751].

2.3. Procedure

Students were invited to participate in the study voluntarily. After reading the informed consent statement, students completed the scales using an online platform that ensured response anonymity. Questionnaires were completed online. The R&D Project was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Navarra (ref. 2018.170). Compliance with the deontological norms of psychology was assured.

2.4. Data Analyses

Using an ex post facto, transversal design [65], three types of analyses were conducted. The usual assumptions of regression analysis were tested beforehand. First, we explored the quality of the data by examining the existence of outliers and missing cases. The method used for detecting univariate outliers consisted of calculating the typical scores of each variable and considering cases with Z scores outside the ±3 range to be potentially atypical cases [65]. On the other hand, the Mahalanobis distance (D2) was used to detect atypical combinations of variables (atypical multivariate cases), a statistical measure of the multidimensional distance of an individual, with respect to the centroid or mean of the observations given [66]. This procedure detects significant distances from the typical combinations or centroids of a set of variables. The literature suggests removing univariate and multivariate outliers, or recodifying them to the nearest extreme score [67]. The procedure was carried out using SPSS (26, IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) which provides a specific routine for missing values analysis that determines the magnitude of missing values and whether they are presented in a systematic or random manner.
Assumptions related to sample size, independence of errors, univariate and multivariate normality, linearity, multicollinearity, recursion, and interval measurement level were also evaluated, presenting acceptable levels of reliability. Regarding the sample size, inclusion of 10–20 cases per parameter is recommended, and at least 200 observations [68]. Independence of errors refers to the fact that the error term of each endogenous variable must not be correlated with other variables. In order to determine univariate normality, we examined the distribution of each observed variable, and its indices of asymmetry and kurtosis. Asymmetry values greater than 3 and kurtosis greater than 10 suggest that the data should be transformed [68]. On the other hand, values less than 70 on the Mardia multivariate index indicate that distance from the multivariate normality is not a critical deterrent to this analysis [69]. Although one of the assumptions is the level of interval measurement, in some cases, variables measured at a nominal or ordinal level are used, as long as the distribution of scores, particularly of the dependent variables, is not markedly asymmetric [67].
The assumption of multicollinearity was analyzed by examining bivariate correlations between variables, since a correlation of 0.85 or higher would indicate difficulty in fulfilling this assumption. The model should be recursive, so causal influences must be one-directional and without retroactive effects. Finally, it is recommended that the measuring instruments show at least moderate reliability properties. This aspect was also fulfilled (see instruments section).
A power value of 0.80 was determined as acceptable. The power of a statistical test is related to: (1) sample size n; (2) level of significance alpha: 5% was assumed, that is, a 95% confidence level (1-alpha); (3) effect size d or r: these measures indicate the relationship between variables (correlation coefficient). Low power might indicate a small sample size, a smaller alpha, or a small effect size, while the opposite can be true of high power.
As a preliminary analysis, we checked for normal sample distribution using the Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test for dependent variables. We also used the Hoelter Index to determine sample size adequacy [65]. In addition, we conducted analyses of linearity and atypical values, missing and influential cases, as well as critical values of multivariate normality; recommended values for the multivariate index of kurtosis, or Mardia coefficient, are less than 70 [70].
For Hypothesis 1, Pearson bivariate correlations were carried out. For Hypothesis 2, we used multiple regression analysis. For Hypothesis 3, we used predictive analyses of structural equations, or SEM models. For this purpose, we followed the recommendations of Hu and Bentler [71], where a model shows adequate fit to the observed data when the ratio of the chi-square statistic to its degrees of freedom is less than five, the values of RMSEA and SRMR are <0.08, and NNFI (non-normed fit index), IFI and CFI are >0.95 [72]. If sample sizes are equal to or less than 250 participants, Hu and Bentler [71] suggest using only the CFI and SRMR fit indices (this was not our case). As an estimation method, the robust maximum likelihood method was used, which allows the use of polychoric correlations; their use is more suitable in variables with the foregoing characteristics of high normality indices and multivariate kurtosis, and a clearly ordinal nature [73]. Reliability of the model dimensions, of the total, and of each of the proposed factor structures was also examined by calculating Cronbach’s alpha. The software programs used to perform these analyses were SPSS 26 [74] for the reliability analyses, and AMOS v. 23 [75] for the confirmatory factor analyses and SEM model.

3. Results

3.1. Previous Analyses

The results were adequate for verifying assumptions of normality in the different indicators analyzed. See Table 4:
The Kolmogorov–Smirnov (p < 0.200) and Shapiro-Wilk (p < 0.208) tests were nonsignificant in all cases. Mardia coefficient or multivariate kurtosis was appropriate (<0.70).

3.2. Linear Association

Correlations between general SR and types of health regulation behavior (internal and external).
A significant, positive correlation appeared between SR and internal (SRH) and external regulation (ERH) of health (r = 0.405, p < 0.001; r = 0.365, p < 0.001 respectively). By contrast, SR showed a significant negative correlation with personal NRH (r = −0. 321, p < 0.001) and ENH (r = −0. 341, p < 0.001), and with personal DRH (r = −0.140, p < 0.05) and EDH (r = −0.231, p < 0.001. See the first column of Table 5.
In complementary fashion, there was a similar trend with SRH, since it showed a significant, negative correlation with NRH (r = −0.418, p < 0.001). ERH, likewise, showed a significant, negative correlation with ENH and dysregulation of health (EDH) (r = −0.336, p < 0.001; r = −0. 211, p < 0.001 respectively). However, the negative correlation between SRH and DRH was not significant.
Other results of interest refer to significant, positive correlations between SRH and ERH (r = 0. 310, p < 0.001), and significant, negative correlations between SRH and internal (NRH) and external non-regulated health (ENH) (r = −0.418, p < 0.001; r = −0.139, p < 0.05, respectively). Similarly, there were positive correlations between internal and external non-regulation of health (r = 0.395, p < 0.001), and between internal and external dys-regulation of health (r = 0.492, p < 0.001). In other words, a significant, positive correlation was found between levels of internal and external regulation, that is: SRH and ERH, NRH and ENH, DRH and EDH. See Table 5.
Regarding psychological reactance, it correlated positively with DRH (r = 0.341, p < 0.001) and negatively with ERH (r = 0.232, p < 0.001) and with student health (SH) (r = −0.151, p < 0.05). By contrast, student health (SH) showed a significant, positive association with internal and external regulation of health (SRH and ERH) (r = 0. 183, p < 0.001; r = 0.252, p < 0.001, respectively) and a negative association with external non-regulation and dysregulation of health (ENH and EDH) (r = −0.201, p < 0.001; r = −0.156, p < 0.001, in that order) and with internal non-regulation (NRH) (r = −0.123, p < 0.05). It is noteworthy that student health (SH) showed a significant, positive association with self- and externally-regulated health behavior (SRH, ERH) and a negative association with internal and external non-regulation and dysregulation of health (NRH, ENH, DRH, EDH). See Table 6.

3.3. Linear and Structural Prediction

3.3.1. Structural Models

Three structural models were tested. Model 1 evaluated the predictive effect of gender on ERH with respect to SRH, reactance and student health (SH). Model 2 evaluated the predictive effect of ERH with respect to SRH, and the predictive value of these two with respect to psychological reactance (REACT) and student health (SH). Model 3 tested the predictive value of the 6 factors of internal and external health regulation with respect to general self-regulated behavior (SR), reactance (REACT), and student health (SH). The third model showed the best statistical standards. See Table 7.

3.3.2. Direct and Indirect Effects

Regarding direct predictive effects from the self- and context-regulation factors, results showed that F1 (SRH) negatively predicted F2 (NRH) (−0.550), which in turn was a predictor of F3 (DRH) (0.203). In complementary manner, F4 (ERH) positively predicted F1 (SRH) (0.316) and negatively predicted F5 (ENH) (−0.409), while F5 (ENH) positively predicted F2 (NRH) (0.433), and F6 (EDH) positively predicted F3 (DRH) (0.584).
Factors F1 (SRH) and F4 (ERH) predicted SR (0.440; 0.226, respectively). Psychological reactance was positively predicted by F3 (EDH) (0.543) and negatively by F4 (ERH) (−0.208). General self-regulation of behavior (SR) strongly predicted student health (SH) (0.790), while psychological reactance (REACT) negatively predicted student health (SH) (−0.322). See Table 8 and Figure 1.
Indirect predictive effects are particularly interesting because they further explain the results above (see Table 9). F1 (SRH) and F4 (ERH) negatively predicted F3 (DRH) (−0.111; −0.216, correspondingly) and psychological reactance (REACT) (−0.060 and −0.117), while positively predicting self-regulation behavior (SR) (0.019; 0.243) and total student health (SH) (0.363; 0.371, respectively). It is noteworthy that F4 (ERH) had greater predictive strength, and was also a negative predictor of F2 (NRH) (−0.351). In complementary manner, F2 (NRH) and F5 (ENH) negatively predicted self-regulation behavior (SR) (−0.035 and −0.077, in that order) and total health (SH) (−0.028; −0.061, correspondingly), and positively predicted psychological reactance (0.110 and 0.239). In this case again, it is the context factor of non-regulation, F5 (ENH), that has more predictive strength; it also positively predicted F3 (DRH) (0.441). In addition, psychological reactance negatively predicted student health (−0.254).
Regarding the indirect predictive effect of the factors on items of other factors, we point to the negative effect of F1 (SRH) on items 7–12 of F2 (NRH) (−0.313 and −0.259, respectively) and on items 13–18 of F3 (DRH) (−0.066 and −0.063). In addition to having these same effects, F4 (ERH) also had a positive effect on items from F1 (SRH). F2 (NRH) had similar effects on F5 (ENH). F2 (NRH) had a positive effect on items 13–18 of F3 (DRH) (0.119; 0.115, in that order), while F5 had additional positive effects on items 7–12 of F2 (NRH) (0.246 and 0.204). Besides the two preceding factors, F3 (DRH) and F6 (EDH) also showed negative predictive effects on factors of general self-regulation (SR) and of health, while their effects were positive for psychological reactance. Finally, self-regulation behavior (SR) indirectly and positively predicted factors of health, while reactance negatively predicted factors of self-regulation (SR) and student health (SH).

4. Discussion

These results allowed us to partially validate the relationships between constructs that are proposed in SRL vs. ERL Theory [9], using specific instruments for this purpose. Some of these relationships between constructs had already been shown using prior instruments. Evidence has shown that the combination of levels of students’ self-regulation and of regulatory teaching account for variability in achievement emotions [22,25], coping with stress [24], learning approaches [26], and achievement [21,76]. Notwithstanding this, it was necessary to demonstrate these relationships with specific measuring instruments in order to corroborate the prior evidence. The new instruments assess degree of self-regulation, non-regulation and dysregulation (internal and external), proposed as specific theoretical constructions pertaining to the aforementioned theory. This study has generated empirical evidence about the convergent validity of the questionnaire on Self-Regulation vs. External Regulation of Health. To this end, we have examined its relations with measures of Self-Regulation behavior, Psychological reactance and Student health.

4.1. Discussion of Hypotheses

Regarding the association hypotheses, the evidence shows that general self-regulation behavior (SR) is positively associated with SRH, as well as negatively associated with NRH and with DRH, whether we speak of personal or contextual regulation (Hypothesis 1). This constitutes an external validation of these new constructions that expand on the prior, classic construct of SR, amply validated in the literature [4,63]. In complementary manner, SRH appeared with the same tendency, that is, negatively associated with personal NRH and with DRH behaviors. In the same way, External Regulation of Health was negatively associated with ENH and EDH.
This result is of particular interest for two reasons: (1) on the one hand, it allows us to theoretically acknowledge the category of dysregulatory behavior, which has not been sufficiently addressed in research on health regulation behaviors, even though it is a concept used in the field of mental health [77,78]; (2) on the other hand, it allows us to theorize the behavioral sequence between self-regulation of health and dysregulation of health. It seems reasonable to assume that a certain self-regulating health behavior may evolve toward a state of non-regulation, when an individual stops making the effort to self-regulate—studies on ego depletion are an example [79,80]. In addition, it seems to suggest that non-regulation is a previous step that is positively associated with dysregulation.
Other interesting associations had to do with the significant negative relationship between psychological reactance and student health. Although reactance had already been associated negatively with self-assessments of happiness and self-esteem, and positively with perceived loneliness and fear of failure [49,50], the present study used a broader model of health, measuring both psychological and physical constructs. Personal dysregulation of health was positively correlated to psychological reactance, while external regulation of health was negatively related. This result is similar to that reported by Laird and Frazer [5], where psychological reactance was related to behavioral problems; the present research adds the complementary aspect of relating it to problems in student health.
For its part, student health had several significant relationships, being positively associated with internal and external health regulation, and negatively with internal and external non-regulation and dysregulation of health. These results underscore not only the importance of students regulating their own behavior in order to attain optimal states of health, but also that one’s state of health is related to how well the context (family, friends, community) encourages self-regulating choices in health behavior.
Regarding linear prediction hypotheses using the structural model, one especially interesting result was the prediction from context to health behavior (SRH, NRH, and DRH) in the three types. Certain prior studies have already documented the importance of context in health behaviors [81,82]. Also, of interest was the negative prediction of self-regulating health behavior on non-regulating health behavior, and the positive prediction of non-regulating health behavior on dysregulating health behavior. This predictive sequence of health behavior could become a powerful explanatory heuristic that addresses both personal and contextual regulation of healthful conduct. Prior evidence has shown a sequential evolution—not always made explicit—between lowering one’s level of regulation, to not regulating oneself, to dysregulation, where these must be considered differentially in the analysis of problematic health behaviors [83]. Contexts can also follow this sequence, from weakening their regulatory aspect, to becoming non-regulatory, and finally, encouraging dysregulation of health. The highest levels in both sequences predict the highest level of student health, while the second and third levels predict successively lower levels of student health. These results, though novel, corroborate certain prior evidence referring to the role of self-regulation in one’s personal health behavior [4,32,84,85,86].
The innovative aspect of the present proposition is that it more precisely defines types of regulatory behaviors as a heuristic for predicting student health: SRH-NRH-DRH. This conceptual and empirical proposition also includes context in the progressive regulatory categorization: ERH-ENH-EDH. Previous evidence has recognized the concept of health dysregulation, alluding to both physical factors [87] and behavioral factors [83]. However, the concept of non-regulation, as an intermediate link between self-regulated and dysregulated health, has not yet been the object of analysis.
Moreover, there has been little analysis of the role of context in promoting or not promoting health behaviors. While context has usually been assumed to play a meaningful role in health, no typology of this role was defined. The present study classifies it along this continuum: ERH-ENH-EDH. Plentiful evidence has suggested the relevance of context in promoting health, though without this analytical breakdown [85,88].

4.2. Limitations

The present study also has limitations to be analyzed. On one hand, the sample is limited; the number of students is insufficient for making high-level inferences to the population, even though a large number of countries are represented. On the other hand, the initial validation of the instruments was conducted using this same sample. For this reason, despite the good statistical fit, the instruments measuring self-regulation of health (SRH-NRH-DRH) and external regulation of health (ERH-ENH-EDH) should continue to be studied and revalidated. Consequently, the results should be taken with caution.

4.3. Goodness and Future Research

This study contributes three highly relevant types of evidence. (1) There is preliminary empirical evidence regarding the hypothesized gradation of types of regulatory behavior and context in the SR vs. ER theoretical model [9], in this case applied to health (regulatory, non-regulatory and dys-regulatory), and positioned alongside the classic construct of self-regulation behavior (SR). We have documented that this gradation is measurable and has empirical consistency. (2) There is evidence of the individual and combined predictive value of the regulatory characteristics of the context and of the subject for predicting students’ psychological reactance and health behaviors. As predicted by the theory and prior evidence, context has an essential effect in inducing each regulation type in subjects. In other words, regulatory contexts predict self-regulated health behavior, non-regulatory contexts produce nonregulated health behavior and dysregulatory contexts predict dysregulatory behavior. (3) Finally, there is evidence that psychological reactance is jointly predicted by a dysregulatory context that induces dysregulation of health behavior, as well as by the subject’s own level of behavioral dysregulation. Psychological reactance should, therefore, be considered a behavioral correlate of dys-regulation, a non-protective variable in university students’ health behaviors. These results as a whole lend empirical support to recent studies that propose self-regulation as an explanatory cross-diagnostic variable, where different diagnostic categories converge [13,14,15,16], taking the combined explanatory view of subject x context.
Future research should validate the factorial invariance of these relationships in other contexts, such as personality variables, education, and other health problems. On the one hand, the adequacy of this regulatory categorization (SRH-NRH-DRH; ERH-ENH-EDH) should be verified for its utility in characterizing different behavior problems. On the other hand, cross-cultural studies should be conducted to provide evidence for the intercultural validity of this categorization. These preliminary results once again lend empirical support to the SR vs. ER Theory [9].

4.4. Professional Implications

There are various implications for intervention in university students’ health and self-regulation. First, it is important to identify the types of health regulation in each case, both internal to the student (SRH-NRH-DRH), and external, from the context (ERH-ENH-EDH). It is essential to take into consideration both sides of this reality in order to assess and intervene in student health programs, using an interactive approach. For too long, student health has been considered almost exclusively determined by the student’s own behaviors, to the detriment of contextual considerations [25]. The time has come to begin designing interventions that also address contextual factors, so that these may likewise promote the health of students [88].

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.d.l.F.; data curation, M.P.-B.; formal analysis, M.C.G.-T.; methodology, J.d.l.F.; project administration, J.d.l.F.; writing—original draft, M.P.-B.; writing—review & editing, J.d.l.F. and M.C.G.-T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by R&D Project PGC2018-094672-B-I00, University of Navarra (Ministry of Science and Education, Spain), and R&D Project UAL18-SEJ-DO31-A-FEDER (University of Almería, Spain), and the European Social Fund.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of University Of Navarra, Spain (REF. 2018.170, 8 February 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Stewart-Brown, S.; Evans, J.; Petersen, S.; Doll, H.; Regis, D. The health of students in institutes of higher education: An important and neglected public health problem? J. Public Health 2000, 22, 492–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Ridner, S.; Newton, K.; Staten, R.; Crawford, T.; Hall, L. Predictors of well-being among college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2016, 64, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Organización Mundial de la Salud. Salud Mental del Adolescente. Available online: https://www.who.int/es/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/adolescent-mental-health (accessed on 20 November 2020).
  4. Garzón-Umerenkova, A.; de la Fuente, J.; Amate, J.; Paolini, P.V.; Fadda, S.; Pérez, J.F. A Linear empirical model of self-regulation on flourishing, health, procrastination, and achievement, among university students. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Laird, D.; Frazer, A. Psychological reactance and negative emotional reactions in the link between psychological control and adolescent adjustment robert. Soc. Dev. 2019, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Lazarus, R.S. Puzzles in the Study of Daily Hassles. In Development as Action in Context; Silbereisen, R.K., Eyferth, K., Rudinger, G., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1986; pp. 39–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Brown, J.M.; Miller, W.R.; Lawendowski, L.A. The Self-Regulation Questionnaire. In Innovations in Clinical Practice: A Source Book.; Vandecreek, L., Jackson, T.L., Eds.; Professional Resources Press: Sarasota, FL, USA, 1999; Volume 17, pp. 281–293. [Google Scholar]
  8. De la Fuente, J.; Zapata, L.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Sander, P.; Cardelle-Elawar, M. The role of personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching to predict motivational-affective variables, achievement, and satisfaction: A structural model. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. De la Fuente, J. Theory of Self- vs. Externally- Regulated Learning TM: Fundamentals, evidence, and applicability. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. De la Fuente, J.; Amate, J.; González-Torres, M.C.; Artuch, R.; García-Torrecillas, J.M.; Fadda, S. Effects of levels of self-regulation and regulatory teaching on strategies for coping with academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Zhang, M.X.; Wu, A.M.S. Effects of smartphone addiction on sleep quality among Chinese university students: The mediating role of self-regulation and bedtime procrastination. Addict. Behav. 2020, 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. McLarnon, M.J.W.; Rothstein, M.G.; King, G.A. Resiliency, self-regulation, and reemployment after job loss. J. Employ. Couns. 2020, 57, 115–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Romer, A.L.; Hariri, A.; Strauman, T.J. Regulatory focus and the p factor: Evidence for self-regulatory dys-function as a transdiagnostic feature of general psychopathology. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2021, 137, 178–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E. All for one and one for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. Am. J. Psychiatry 2018, 175, 831–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Caspi, A.; Houts, R.M.; Belsky, D.W.; Goldman-Mellor, S.J.; Harrington, H.; Israel, S.; Meier, M.H.; Ramrakha, S.; Shalev, I.; Poulton, R.; et al. The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 2, 119–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Caspi, A.; Houts, R.M.; Ambler, A.; Danese, A.; Elliot, M.L.; Hariri, A.; Harrington, H.; Hogan, S.; Poulton, R.; Ramrakha, S.; et al. Longitudinal Assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the dunedin birth cohort study. JAMA Netw. Open 3. JAMA Netw. Open 2020, 3, e203221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Brown, J.M. Self-Regulation and the Addictive Behaviors. In Treating Addictive Behaviors; Miller, W.R., Heather, N., Eds.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 61–73. [Google Scholar]
  18. De la Fuente, J. Self- vs. Externally- Regulation Scale in Health Questionary, SR-ER HEALTH; University of Navarra: Pamplona, Spain, 2020; Manuscrito pendiente de publicar. [Google Scholar]
  19. Zimmerman, B.J.; Labuhn, A.S. Self-Regulation of Learning: Process Approaches to Personal Development. In APA Educational Psychology Handbook; Theories, constructs, and critical issues; American Psychological Association: Worcester, MA, USA, 2012; Volume 1, pp. 399–425. [Google Scholar]
  20. De la Fuente, J.; Berbén, A.B.; Zapata, L. How regulatory teaching impacts university students’ perceptions of the teaching-learning process: The role of teacher training. Infancia Aprendizaje 2013, 36, 375–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. De la Fuente, J.; Sander, P.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Vera, M.M.; Garzón, A.; Fadda, S. Combined effect of levels in personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching on meta-cognitive, on meta-motivational, and on academic achievement variables in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. De la Fuente, J.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Peralta-Sánchez, F.J.; Garzón-Umerenkova, A.; Vera, M.M.; Paolini, P. Applying the SRL vs ERL Theory to the Knowledge of Achievement Emotions in Undergraduate University Students. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. De la Fuente, J.; González-Torres, M.C.; Aznárez-Sanado, M.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Peralta-Sánchez, F.J.; Vera, M.M. Implications of Unconnected Micro, Molecular, and Molar Level Research in Psychology: The Case of Executive Functions, Self-Regulation, and External Regulation. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. De la Fuente, J.; Peralta-Sánchez, F.J.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Sander, P.; Garzón-Umerenkova, A.; Zapata, L. Effects of Self- vs External Regulation on the Factors and Symptoms of Academic Stress in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. De la Fuente, J.; Paolini, P.V.; Vera-Martínez, M.M.; Garzón-Umerenkova, A. Effect of levels of self-regulation and situational Stress on Achievement Emotions in Undergraduate Students: Class, Study and Testing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Zimmerman, B.J.; Moylan, A.R. Self-Regulation: Where Metacognition and Motivation Intersect. In The Educational Psychology Series. Handbook of Metacognition in Education; Hacker, J., Dunlosky, A., Graesser, C., Eds.; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: London, UK, 2009; pp. 299–315. [Google Scholar]
  27. Zimmerman, B.J. Attaining Self-Regulation: A Social Cognitive Perspective. In Handbook of Self-Regulation; Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P., Zeidner, M., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; pp. 13–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Zimmerman, B.J.; Schunk, D.H. Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: Theoretical Perspectives; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  29. De la Fuente, J.; Paoloni, P.; Kauffman, D.; Yilmaz Soylu, M.; Sander, P.; Zapata, L. Big Five, Self-Regulation, and Coping Strategies as Predictors of Achievement Emotions in Undergraduate Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Agostinelli, G.; Brown, J.M.; Miller, W.R. Effects of normative feedback on consumption among heavy drinking college students. J. Drug Educ. 1995, 25, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Hennessy, E.A.; Johnson, B.T.; Acabchuk, R.L.; McCloskey, K.; Stewart-James, J. Self-Regulation Mechanisms in Health Behaviour Change: A Systematic Meta-Review of Meta-Analyses, 2006–2017. Health Psychol. Rev. 2019, 1, 142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Rienties, B.; Tempelaar, D.; Nguyen, Q.; Littlejohn, A. Unpacking the Intertemporal Impact of Self-Regulation in a Blended Mathematics Environment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 100, 345–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Balkis, M.; Duro, E. Gender Differences in the Relationship between Academic Procrastination, Satifaction with Academic Life and Academic Performance. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 15, 105–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Beauchaine, T.P.; Crowell, S.E. (Eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Emotion Dysregulation; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  35. Haghbin, M.; Pychyl, T.A. Measurement of Health-Related Procrastination: Development and Validation of the Exercise and Healthy Diet Procrastination Scales. In Procrastination, Health, and Well-Being; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016; pp. 121–142. [Google Scholar]
  36. Clariana, M. Personalidad, Procrastinación y Conducta Deshonesta En Alumnado de Distintos Grados Universitarios. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 11, 451–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Lew-Starowicz, M.; Lewczuk, K.; Nowakowska, I.; Kraus, S.; Gola, M. Compulsive Sexual Behavior and Dysregulation of Emotion. Sex. Med. Rev. 2020, 8, 191–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Richard, E. Developing Employee Resilience: The Role of Leader-Facilitated Emotion Management. Adv. Dev. Hum. Resour. 2020, 22, 387–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Quinn, P.D.; Fromme, K. Self-Regulation as a Protective Factor against Risky Drinking and Sexual Behavior. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2010, 24, 376–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. De la Fuente, J.; Sander, P.; Kauffman, D.; Yilmaz-Soylu, M. Differential Effects of Self- vs External- Regulation on Learning Approaches, Academic Achievement and Satisfaction in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 543884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Miller, A.L.; Lo, S.L.; Bauer, K.W.; Fredericks, E.M. Developmentally Informed Behaviour Change Techniques to Enhance Self-Regulation in a Health Promotion Context: A Conceptual Review. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 14, 116–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mezuk, B.; Ratliff, S.; Concha, J.B.; Abdou, C.M.; Rafferty, J.; Lee, H.; Jackson, J.S. Stress, Self-Regulation, and Context: Evidence from the Health and Retirement Survey. SSM Popul. Health 2017, 3, 455–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Marquez, S.M. Relational and Contextual Antecedents of Emotion Regulation: Integrating the Easi Model into Existing Emotional Labor Models. Doctoral Dissertation, Michigan State University, Michigan, DT, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  44. Steindl, C.; Jonas, E.; Sittenthaler, S.; Traut-Mattausch, E.; Greenberg, J. Understanding Psychological Reactance. Z. Psychol. 2015, 233, 205–214. [Google Scholar]
  45. Miron, A.M.; Brehm, J.W. Reactance Theory—40 Years Later. Z. Soz. 2006, 37, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Brehm, J.W. A Theory of Psychological Reactance; Academic Press: Oxford, UK, 1966. [Google Scholar]
  47. Brehm, J.W.; Brehm, S.S. Psychological Reactance: A Theory Offeedom and Control; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  48. Dowd, E.T.; Wallbrown, F. Motivational Components of Client Reactance. J. Couns. Dev. 1993, 71, 533–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Joubert, C.E. Relationship among Self-Esteem, Psychological Reactance, and Other Personality Variables. Psychol. Rep. 1990, 66, 147–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hockenberry, S.L.; Billingham, R.E. Psychological Reactance and Violence within Dating Relationships. Psychol. Rep. 1993, 73, 1203–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Hong, S.M.; Faedda, S. Refinement of the Hong Psychological Reactance Scale. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1996, 56, 173–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Organización Mundial de la Salud. Constitución de La Organización Mundial de La Salud, 45th ed.; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  53. Belloc, N.B.; Breslow, L. Relationship of Physical Health Status and Health Practices. Prev. Med. 1972, 1, 409–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Pozo, C.; Garzón, A.; Charry, C.; Bretones, B. Psychometric Properties and Dimensionality of the “Flourishing Scale” in Spanish-Speaking Population. Electr. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2016, 14, 175–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Seligman, M.E.P. Flourishing: A New Understanding of Happiness and Well-Being–and How to Achieve Them; Nicholas Brealey Publishing: Boston, MA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  56. Dallo, F. Predictors of Self-Rated Health among Undergraduate College Students in the United States. Coll. Stud. J. 2018, 52, 431–440. [Google Scholar]
  57. De Mendonca Freire, L.; Dalamaria, T.; de Aguino Cunha, M.; Florencio de Souza, O. Self-Rated Health in University Students from Rio Branco in the Estern Brazilian Amazon. Health 2014, 6, 2245–2249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Vaez, M.; Kristenson, M.; Laflamme, L. Perceived Quality of Life and Self-Rated Health among First-Year University Students. Soc. Indic. Res. 2004, 68, 221–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Howell, A.J. Flourishing: Achievement-Related Correlates of Students’ Well-Being. J. Posit. Psychol. 2009, 4, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Barra, E. Social Support, Stress and Health. Psicología Salud 2004, 14, 237–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Miller, W.R.; Brown, J.M. Self-Regulation as a Conceptual Basis for the Prevention and Treatment of Addictive Behaviours. In Self-Control. and the Addictive Behaviours; Heather, W.R., Miller, W.R., Greeley, J., Eds.; Maxwell Macmillan Publishing Australia: Sydney, NSW, Australia, 1991; pp. 3–79. [Google Scholar]
  62. Pichardo, C.; Justicia, F.; de la Fuente, J.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Berbén, A.B. Factor Structure of the Self-Regulation Questionnaire (SRQ) at Spanish Universities. Span. J. Psychol. 2014, 17, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Pichardo, M.C.; Cano, F.; Garzón, A.; de la Fuente, J.; Peralta, F.J.; Amate, J. Self-Regulation Questionnaire (SRQ) in Spanish Adolescents: Factor Structure and Rasch Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Ato, M.; López, J.; Benavente, A. Un Sistema de Clasificación de Los Diseños de Investigación En Psicología. An. Psicol. 2013, 29, 1038–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Principal Components and Factor Analysis. Using Multivar. Stat. 2001, 4, 582–633. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lohr, S. Sampling: Design and Analysis; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  67. Weston, R.; Gore, P.A. A Brief Guide to Structural Equation Modeling. Couns. Psychol. 2006, 34, 719–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  69. Rodríguez, M.N. Indicadores de Rendimiento de Estudiantes Universitarios: Calificaciones versus Créditos Acumulados. Rev. Educ. 2011, 355, 467–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Mardia, K.V. Measures of Multivariate Skewness and Kurtosis with Applications. Biometrika 1970, 57, 519–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Jöreskog, K.G.; Sörbom, D. LISREL 8: Structural Equation Modeling with the SIMPLIS Command Language; Scientific Software International: Lincolnwood, IL, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  73. Flora, D.B.; Curran, P.J. An Empirical Evaluation of Alternative Methods of Estimation for Confirmatory Factor Analysis with Ordinal Data. Psychol. Methods 2004, 9, 466–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. IBM. SPSS Statistics for Windows; IBM Corp.: Armonk, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  75. Arbuckle, J.L. Amos (Version 23.0); IBM SPSS: Chicago, IL, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  76. De la Fuente, J.; Amate, J.; Sander, P. Relationships between Cognitive Strategies, Motivational Strategies and Academic Stress in Professional Examination Candidates. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Avarzamani, L.; Hosein Mozafarian, M.; Shahmiri Barzoki, H.; Mohsen Rahnejat, A.; Ebrahimi, M. Comparison of Emotional Intelligence in Young Male Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder or Traits and Healthy Controls. In Professional Psychology: Research and Practice; American Psychological Association: Worcester, MA, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wegmann, E.; Brand, M. Affective and cognitive processes involved in behavioral addictions. Addict. Behav. 2021, 118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Freeman, N.; Muraven, M. Self-control depletion leads to increased risk taking. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2010, 1, 175–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Friese, M.; Engeler, M.; Florack, A. Self-perceived successful weight regulators are less affected by self-regulatory depletion in the domain of eating behavior. Eat. Behav. 2015, 16, 5–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Kedia, S.; Ahuja, N.; Wyant, D.K.; Dillon, P.J.; Akkus, C.; Relyea, G. Compositional and contextual factors associated with drug overdose deaths in the United States. J. Addict. Dis. 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Weiss, N.H.; Sullivan, T.P.; Tull, M.T. Explicating the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors: A review and synthesis of the literature with directions for future research and clinical practice. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015, 3, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. O’Carroll, R.E. Self-Regulation Interventions—What Do We Know and Where Should We Go? Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 14, 159–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Van den Toren, S.J.; van Grieken, A.; de Kroon, M.L.; Mulder, W.C.; Vanneste, Y.T.; Raat, H. Young Adults’ Self-Sufficiency in Daily Life: The Relationship with Contextual Factors and Health Indicators. BMC Psychol. 2020, 8, 89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Powers, J.P.; Moshontz, H.; Hoyle, R.H. Self-Control and Affect Regulation Styles Predict Anxiety Longitudinally in University Students. Collabra Psychol. 2020, 6, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Carbone, J.T. Allostatic Load and Mental Health: A Latent Class Analysis of Physiological Dysregulation. Stress Int. J. Biol. Stress 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Wiss, D.A.; Avena, N.; Gold, M. Food addiction and psychosocial adversity: Biological embedding, contextual factors, and public health implications. Nutrients 2020, 12, 3521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Liu, Y.; Kim, H.; Carney, J.V.; Chung, K.S.; Hazler, R.J. Individual and Contextual Factors Associated with School Connectedness in the Context of Counseling in Schools. J. Couns. Dev. 2020, 98, 391–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Structural Equations Model of prediction. F1 = Self-Regulation of Health; F2 = Self Non-Regulation of Health; F3 = Self Dys-Regulation of Health; F4 = External Regulation of Health; F5 = External Nonregulation of Health; F6 = External Dysregulation of Health; SR = Self-regulation of general behavior; REACT = Reactance; ACHE = Student Health; ARHR = items of SRH vs. ERH questionnaire; METAS = Goals; PERSEV = Perseverance; DECISIO = Decisions; ERROR = Learning from mistakes; REACT = Factors of reactivity: SALUDFIS = Physical health; SALUDPSI = Psychological health.
Figure 1. Structural Equations Model of prediction. F1 = Self-Regulation of Health; F2 = Self Non-Regulation of Health; F3 = Self Dys-Regulation of Health; F4 = External Regulation of Health; F5 = External Nonregulation of Health; F6 = External Dysregulation of Health; SR = Self-regulation of general behavior; REACT = Reactance; ACHE = Student Health; ARHR = items of SRH vs. ERH questionnaire; METAS = Goals; PERSEV = Perseverance; DECISIO = Decisions; ERROR = Learning from mistakes; REACT = Factors of reactivity: SALUDFIS = Physical health; SALUDPSI = Psychological health.
Ijerph 18 03793 g001
Table 1. Conceptual continuum and typologies of personal health regulation.
Table 1. Conceptual continuum and typologies of personal health regulation.
Characteristics of Health Behavior Regulation(1) Self-Regulated Health
(SRH)
POSITIVE PRO-ACTIVITY (+1)
(2) Non-Regulated Health (NRH)
RE-ACTIVITY (0)
(3) Dys-Regulated Health
(DRH)
NEGATIVE PRO-ACTIVITY (−1)
Before (planning)
Self-analysis
Self-defined goals
Self-motivation
Before (no planning)
No self-analysis
No goals
No motivation
Before (planning amiss)
Erroneous self-analysis
Erroneous goals
Self-demotivation
During (self-monitoring)
Self-observation
Self-analysis
Self-correction
During (no monitoring)
No self-observation
No self-analysis
No self-correction
During (misguided monitoring)
Self-distraction
Cognitive self-avoidance
Self-handicapping, procrastination
After (self-assessment)
Personal reflection
Self-attributions
Positive self-affect
After (no assessment)
No reflection
No attributions
Indifferent in affect
After (misguided assessment)
Erroneous self-assessment
Erroneous self-attributions
Negative self-affect
Type of ActivitySelf-Regulated Health (SRH)Non-Regulated Health (NRH)Dys-Regulated Health (DRH)
Health BehaviorSR HealthNo norms/limitsSelf-induced excesses/deficits
Table 2. Conceptual continuum of external health regulation.
Table 2. Conceptual continuum of external health regulation.
Characteristics of the External Regulatory Context1. External Regulation
of Health (ERH)
POSITIVE PROACTIVITY (+1)
2. External Non-Regulation of Health (ENH)
NEUTRAL (0)
3. External Dys-Regulation
of Health (EDH)
NEGATIVE PRO-ACTIVITY (−1)
Before (planning)
Introduces tasks
Favors well-adjusted goals
Favors self-motivation
Before (no planning)
Does not present tasks
Does not suggest goals
Does not induce motivation
Before (planning amiss)
Erroneous tasks
Favors self-handicapping goals
Induces demotivation
During (favors monitoring)
Promotes self-observation
Promotes self-analysis
Promotes self-correction
During (indifferent)
No self-observation
No self-analysis
No self-correction
During (discourages monitoring)
Promotes self-distraction,
Cognitive self-avoidance,
Self-handicapping, Procrastination
After (prompts assessment)
Promotes self-reflection
Promotes adjusted self-attributions
Promotes positive adjusted self-affect
After (ignores assessment)
No reflection
No attributions
Indifferent in affect
After (misguides assessment)
Promotes erroneous self-assessment
Erroneous self-attributions
Promotes maladjusted self-affect
Example:
alcohol use
Context
(environmental characteristics)
Externally regulatory
Norms/limits on consumption
Non-regulatory
No norms/consequences
Dys-regulatory
Encourages alcohol abuse
Table 3. Types of self and external regulation of health.
Table 3. Types of self and external regulation of health.
External RegulationSelf-Regulation
SRH (+1)NRH (0)DRH (−1)
ERH (+1)+
ENH (0)+
EDH (−1)++
Table 4. Preliminary Analyses.
Table 4. Preliminary Analyses.
Statistical IndexSRSRHNRHDRHERHENHEDHREACTSH
Mean3.4373.5762.6702.5643.4972.4802.2542.6573.571
Mean
Standard Error
0.0350.0450.0390.0430.0550.0460.0480.0330.038
Mode3.243.332.832.503.672.502.002.443.60
Standard
Deviation
0.5880.7530.6520.7160.9050.7530.8000.5520.630
Asymmetry0.088−0.1680.1820.091−0.3590.3180.4430.257−0.262
Standard
asymmetry error
0.1490.1490.1490.1490.1490.1490.1490.1490.149
Kurtosis−0.480−0.143−0.062−0.345−0.157−0.180−0.2140.263−0.079
Standard
kurtosis error
0.2960.2960.2970.2970.2960.2970.2960.2960.296
Range3.093.673.503.674.003.833.673.253.30
Minimum1.861.331.171.001.001.001.001.001.70
Maximum4.955.004.674.675.004.834.674.255.00
Note. SR = Self-Regulation behavior; SRH = Self-Regulation of Health behavior; NRH = Non-Regulation of Health behavior; DRH = Dys-regulation of Health behavior; ERH = External Regulation of Health behavior; ENH = External Nonregulation of Health behavior; EDH = External Dysregulation of Health behavior; REACT = Psychological Reactance, total score; SH = Student Health.
Table 5. Bivariate correlations between self-regulation and types of health regulation (internal and external) (n = 269).
Table 5. Bivariate correlations between self-regulation and types of health regulation (internal and external) (n = 269).
VariablesSRSRHNRHDRHERHENHEDH
SR
SRH405 ***
NRH−0.321 ***−0.418 ***
DRH−0.140 *−0.0180.321 ***
ERH365 ***310 ***−0.016−0.097
ENH−0.341 ***−0.139 *0.395 ***−0.394 ***−0.336 ***
EDH−0.231 ***0.0320.245 ***0.492 **−0.211 **0.499 ***
Note. SR = Self-Regulation behavior; SRH = Self-Regulation of Health behavior; NRH = Non-Regulation of Health behavior; DRH = Dys-regulation of Health behavior; ERH = External Regulation of Health behavior; ENH = External Nonregulation of Health behavior; EDH = External Dysregulation of Health behavior; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.00.
Table 6. Correlations between types of internal and external regulation, psychological reactance and health (n = 269).
Table 6. Correlations between types of internal and external regulation, psychological reactance and health (n = 269).
VariableSRHNRHDRHERHENHEDHREACTSH
REACTAN−0.0350.1060.341 ***−0.232 ***0.285 ***0.284 ***
SH183 **−0.123 *−0.1130.252 ***−0.201 **−0.156 *−0.151 *
Note. SRH = Self-Regulation of Health behavior; NRH = Non-Regulation of Health behavior; DRH = Dys-Regulation of Health behavior; ERH = External Regulation of Health behavior; ENH = External Nonregulation of Health behavior; EDH = External Dysregulation of Health behavior; REACT = Psychological Reactance, total score; SH = Student Health; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 7. Models of structural linear results of the variables.
Table 7. Models of structural linear results of the variables.
ModelChi2DFCH/dfSRMRp<NFIRFIIFITLICFIRMSEAHOELT
0.05
HOELT 0.01
1227.217514.4550.120.0010.7980.7350.7240.8760.8230.0868192
2227.217514.4550.120.0010.8100.8890.8208340.8180.084181192
32028.7599322.770.050.0010.9440.9040.9700.9380.9650.064133137
Note. Model 1: Gender → ERH → SRH → Reactance → Health; Model 2: ERH → SRH → Reactance → Health; Model 3: 6 factors of Health Regulation → SR → Reactance → Health; DF = Degrees of freedom; CH/df = Chi Square/Degrees of Freedom; SRMR = Standardized Root-Mean-square Residual; p< = probability level; NFI = Normed Fit Index; RFI = Relative Fit Index; IFI = Incremental Fit Index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; HOELT = Hoelter index.
Table 8. Standardized direct effects (default model).
Table 8. Standardized direct effects (default model).
VariablesF1 (SRH)F2 (NRH)F3 (DRH)F4 (ERH)F5 (ENH)F6 (EDH)SRREACTSH
F1 0.316
F2−0.550 0.433
F3 0.203 0.584
F4
F5 −0.409
F6 0.604
SR0.440 0.226 −0.322
REACT 0.543−0.208
SH 0.790
SRERH10.719
SHERH20.771
SRERH30.719
SRERH40.684
SRERH50.692
SRERH60.579
SRERH7 0.568
SRERH8 −0.212
SRERH9 0.457
SRERH10 0.527
SRERH11 0.747
SRERH12 0.471
SRERH13 0.589
SRERH14 0.226
SRERH15 0.481
SRERH16 0.549
SRERH17 0.609
SRERH18 0.566
SRERH19 0.768
SRERH20 0.813
SRERH21 0.846
SRERH22 0.808
SRERH23 0.755
SHERH24 0.663
SRERH25 0.411
SRERH26 0.459
SRERH27 0.551
SRERH28 0.723
SRERH29 0.804
SRERH30 0.659
SRERH31 0.714
SRERH32 650
SRERH33 0.696
SRERH34 0.731
SRERH35 0.572
SRERH36 0.696
GOALS 0.733
PERSEV 0.712
DECIS 0.448
MISTAKES 0.654
REACT1 0.834
REACT2 0.533
REACT3 0.641
HEALTH1 0.497
HEALTH2 0.284
Note. F1 = Self-Regulation of Health; F2 = Self Non-Regulation of Health; F3 = Self Dys-Regulation of Health; F4 = External Regulation of Health; F5 = External Nonregulation of Health; F6 = External Dysregulation of Health; SR = Self-regulation of general behavior; REACT = Reactance; SH = Student health; SRER = items of SRH vs. ERH questionnaire; GOALS = Goals; PERSEV = Perseverance; DECIS = Decisions; MISTAKES = Learning from mistakes; REACT = Factors of reactivity: HEALTH1 = physical health; HEALTH2 = psychological health.
Table 9. Standardized indirect effects (default model).
Table 9. Standardized indirect effects (default model).
Variables F1
(SRH)
F2
(NRH)
F3
(DRH)
F4
(ERH)
F5
(ENH)
F6
(EDH)
SRREACTSH
F1
F2 −0.351
F3−0.111 −0.2160.441
F4
F5
F6 0.247
SR0.019−0.035−0.1750.243−0.077−0.102
REACT−0.0600.110 −0.1170.2390.317
HEALTH0.363−0.028−0.1380.371−0.061−0.081 −0.254
SRERH1 0.227
SHERH2 0.243
SRERH3 0.227
SHERH4 0.216
SRERH5 0.218
SRERH6 0.183
SRERH7−0.313 −0.0750.246
SRERH80.117 −0.160−0.092
SRERH9−0.251 −0.1850.198
SRERH10−0.290 −0.2620.228
SRERH11−0.411 −0.1650.323
SRERH12−0.259 −0.1650.204
SRERH13−0.0660.119 −0.1270.2600.344
SRERH14−0.0250.046 −0.0490.1000.132
SRERH15−0.0540.098 −0.1040.2120.281
SRERH16−0.0610.111 −0.1180.2420.321
SRERH17−0.0680.123 −0.1310.2690.356
SRERH18−0.0630.115 −0.1220.2490.331
SRERH19 0.2600.344
SRERH20 0.1000.132
SRERH21
SRERH22
SRERH23
SHERH24
SRERH25 −0.168
SRERH26 −0.188
SRERH27 −0.225
SRERH28 −0.296
SRERH29 −0.329
SRERH30 −0.270
SRERH31 −0.1760.431
SRERH32 −0.1610.393
SRERH33 −0.1720.420
SRERH34 −0.1810.442
SRERH35 −0.1410.346
SRERH36 −0.1620.396
GOALS0.337−0.026−0.1280.344−0.056−0.075
PERSEV0.327−0.025−0.1240.334−0.055−0.073 −0.236
DECIS0.206−0.016−0.0780.210−0.035−0.046 −0.229
MISTAKES0.300−0.023−0.1140.307−0.050−0.067 −0.144
REACT1−0.0500.0920.453−0.2710.2000.265 −0.211
REACT2−0.0320.0590.289−0.1730.1270.169
REACT3−0.0390.0700.348−0.2080.1530.203
HEALTH10.180−0.014−0.0690.185−0.040−0.0400.393−0.127
HEALTH20.103−0.008−0.0390.105−0.023−0.0230.224−0.072
Note. F1 = Self-Regulation of Health; F2 = Self Non-Regulation of Health; F3 = Self Dys-Regulation of Health; F4 = External Regulation of Health; F5 = External Nonregulation of Health; F6 = External Dysregulation of Health; SR = Self-regulation behavior; REACT = Reactance; SH = Student Health; SRERH = items of SRH vs. ERH questionnaire; GOALS = Goals; PERSEV = Perseverance; DECIS = Decisions; MISTAKES = Learning from mistakes; REACT = Factors of reactivity: HEALTH1 = Physical health; HEALTH2 = Psychological health.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pachón-Basallo, M.; de la Fuente, J.; Gonzáles-Torres, M.C. Regulation/Non-Regulation/Dys-Regulation of Health Behavior, Psychological Reactance, and Health of University Undergraduate Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3793. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18073793

AMA Style

Pachón-Basallo M, de la Fuente J, Gonzáles-Torres MC. Regulation/Non-Regulation/Dys-Regulation of Health Behavior, Psychological Reactance, and Health of University Undergraduate Students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(7):3793. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18073793

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pachón-Basallo, Mónica, Jesús de la Fuente, and María Carmen Gonzáles-Torres. 2021. "Regulation/Non-Regulation/Dys-Regulation of Health Behavior, Psychological Reactance, and Health of University Undergraduate Students" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 7: 3793. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18073793

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop