Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Patterns of and Motivations for Concurrent Use of Video Games and Substances
Previous Article in Journal
A Multilevel Analysis of Neighbourhood Built and Social Environments and Adult Self-Reported Physical Activity and Body Mass Index in Ottawa, Canada
Previous Article in Special Issue
Online Social Networking and Addiction—A Review of the Psychological Literature
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Playing Video Games While Using or Feeling the Effects of Substances: Associations with Substance Use Problems

1
School of Social Work, Adelphi University, 1 South Avenue, Garden City, NY 11530, USA
2
Institute for Special Populations Research, National Development and Research Institutes, 8th Floor, 71 West 23rd Street, New York, NY 10010, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2011, 8(10), 3979-3998; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph8103979
Submission received: 19 August 2011 / Revised: 13 October 2011 / Accepted: 13 October 2011 / Published: 18 October 2011
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Substance and Behavioral Addictions: Co-Occurrence and Specificity)

Abstract

:
This study tested the hypothesis that playing video games while using or feeling the effects of a substance—referred to herein as “concurrent use”—is related to substance use problems after controlling for substance use frequency, video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby, and demographic factors. Data were drawn from a nationally representative online survey of adult video gamers conducted by Knowledge Networks, valid n = 2,885. Problem video game playing behavior was operationalized using Tejeiro Salguero and Bersabé Morán’s 2002 problem video game play (PVP) measure, and measures for substance use problems were taken from the National Survey of Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). Separate structural equation modeling analyses were conducted for users of caffeine, tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana. In all four models, concurrent use was directly associated with substance use problems, but not with PVP. Video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby was associated with substance use problems via two indirect paths: through PVP for all substances, and through concurrent use for caffeine, tobacco, and alcohol only. Results illustrate the potential for “drug interaction” between self-reinforcing behaviors and addictive substances, with implications for the development of problem use.

1. Introduction

Between self-reinforcing behaviors and substances, there may exist a “drug interaction” which exacerbates addictive patterns. Addictive substances stimulate the brain’s natural reward pathways [1], and behavioral addictions involve the endogenous cannabinoid and related brain systems [2]. Dopamine plays a role in the self-reinforcing nature of substance use [35], and elevated dopamine plays a “gain-amplifying role” [6] in responses to “rewarding” Pavlovian stimuli [7]. As would be expected based on these shared biological mechanisms, survey studies find relationships between substance use problems and self-reinforcing behaviors like eating, gambling, internet use, sex, exercise, "workaholism," shopping, television viewing, and video games [813].
Such a “drug interaction” between self-reinforcing behaviors and addictive substances has been discovered in survey and laboratory findings that alcohol exacerbates problem gambling [1418]. Part of the explanation offered for this is that alcohol contributes to disinhibition and risky decision making [19]. If this “drug interaction” effect is not, however, unique to the specific context of the alcohol/gambling combination but part of a larger pattern in which behavioral and substance addictions contribute to each other, then it should be evident in other substance/behavior combinations. It should also hold that engaging in self-reinforcing behavior while using or feeling the effects of a substance is uniquely associated with not only behavioral addiction, but substance use problems as well. In the present study, we focus on video game play. Video gaming is both similar to and distinct from gambling [20] in its self-reinforcing features [21] and degrees of prevalence and social acceptability, which all contribute to its unique characteristics as an addictive behavior [13].
The premise that video game playing is a potentially addictive behavior has empirical support [22]. Although the idea of video game addiction is not universally accepted [23], particularly as a discrete diagnostic category [24], and is not apparently likely to be included in the forthcoming DSM-V [25], problem video gaming behavior remains an area of scholarly and clinical [26,27] concern. A report by the American Medical Association’s Council on Science and Public Health called for more research on it [28]. A growing body of research on problem video gaming behaviors has emerged from within work on problem gambling [29] and adapted its measures [27], which originally came from Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) substance abuse/dependence criteria [30,31]. Prevalence estimates of significant problem video gaming behavior hover between 4.9% and 9% among video gamers internationally [11,3235], with smaller numbers fitting stricter criteria for dependence [22,36].
Video game addiction has identifiable biological dimensions, which are similar to those for other behavioral addictions as well as substance dependence. An electroencephalography study found differences in processing of game-related cues between “casual” and “excessive” video game players suggestive of an addiction-like process among the “excessive” players [37]. Video game play stimulates dopamine [38] through the “reward” structures programmed into video games [39,40]. Dopaminergic medications indicated for ADHD and substance dependence help remediate problem video gaming behavior [41,42], and video games are associated with development of attention problems in children [43,44].
In addition to shared biological mechanisms, problem video gaming behavior and substance use have several risk correlates in common. Examples include mood disorders [33,45,46], impulsivity [33,47], attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms [42,4852], low social competence [53,54], low academic performance [5558], and (in the case of violent video games) violence [59,60]. Although many of these studies on negative effects of problem video gaming behavior focus on children and youth [24], there are similar findings on adults [22], which implies that it would not be accurate to frame video game addiction as a youth problem behavior. Additional studies on adults would help distinguish problem video gaming behavior as an issue with addiction, rather than something parsimoniously explained by youth problem behavior theory [61].
Given shared biological mechanisms and risk correlates of problem video gaming behavior and substance use problems, as well as the general trend for behavioral and substance addictions to co-occur [13], this study investigated unique potential of playing video games while using or feeling the effects of a substance—referred to herein as “concurrent use”—to contribute to problem video gaming behavior and substance use problems. Its hypothesis was that concurrent use would explain variance in problem video gaming behavior and substance use problems that would not be explained by substance use frequency, video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby (video game play frequency, enjoyment, and consumer involvement), or demographics, including gender [30,34,62,63], race [64], age, and socioeconomic indicators. Analyses tested this hypothesis with respect to caffeine, tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and Recruitment

Participants were a subset of a nationally representative KnowledgePanel® maintained by Knowledge Networks, a commercial online survey service provider. Knowledge Networks selects panel members via random-digit dial and address-based sampling methods, provides panel members with computers and internet access if needed, establishes informed consent, and collects basic demographic information. Once in the panel, members are randomly recruited via e-mail for client surveys, including this study. For each survey, participants receive “points” toward cash and other incentives offered by Knowledge Networks. For some client surveys, panel members are presented with screening questions and only allowed to participate if they meet specific criteria. For this survey, 15,642 e-mails were sent to panel members age 18 and over, and 9,215 (59%) completed the screening instrument. The valid sample was 42% female, 69% white, 11% Black, 13% Latino, 4% Asian, 2% Native (includes American Indian, Alaska Native, Native Hawaiian, and Pacific Islander), and 2% multiracial or other. More than half (58%) were currently either employed for wages or self-employed. The average participant was 40.4 years old (SD = 15.7), with annual income between $35,000 and $39,999, and had some college education but no degree.
The screener asked whether participants “regularly,” “occasionally,” or “never” participated in 11 different hobby activities in the past year, including video games. Participants who responded “regularly” or “occasionally” about video games were then asked how many hours of video and/or computer games they played in the past seven days. Participants who reported one or more hours, n = 3,380 (37%), were allowed to take the survey. Screening and the survey itself were conducted in English and Spanish. The Spanish version was professionally translated by Knowledge Networks using multiple translators and back-translation. It was also reviewed by the first author, who is fluent in written Spanish. Completion of the measure took an average of 10 minutes, which was the maximum median length feasible given constraints of the method and budget. The protocol for this study was reviewed and approved by all investigators’ Institutional Review Boards.

2.2. Measures

Substance use frequency

These measures were adapted from the National Survey of Drug Use and Health. (NSDUH, [65]). Participants chose, from a list, those substances they had used in the past 30 days. For each they chose, they were asked on how many of the past 30 days they had used it.

Substance use problems

NSDUH-based [65] abuse and dependence symptom items were presented for each substance used in the past 30 days. A set of questions was adapted for caffeine by using the full set of abuse/dependence items and leaving out those inapplicable to it (e.g., spending a lot of time obtaining/using/recovering from it, neglecting work and social life in order to use it). Because the full-length measures could not realistically be included in a survey constrained to a median length of 10 minutes, subsets were selected based on factor analyses of another dataset collected in this project (not yet published) which did include full-length measures—seven dichotomous items for caffeine, 11 Likert-scale items for tobacco, and 14 dichotomous items for each of alcohol and marijuana. For sets of dichotomous items, tetrachoric correlation matrices were factor analyzed. The goal of item selection was not to reproduce DSM-IV diagnoses but to measure problem substance use as a matter of degrees as authentically as possible within time constraints. Items were chosen that were highly correlated with the first/only factor, but not redundant—when collinearity resulted from everyone who reported one symptom also having reported another symptom, the less-frequently-reported symptom was left out. The final 5-item measure for caffeine included symptoms of tolerance, difficulty controlling use, desire to quit or cut down, withdrawal, and disregarding negative emotional or physical health consequences of use, CFI from confirmatory factor analysis = 0.962. Final measures for alcohol (CFI = 0.997) and marijuana (CFI = 0.998) included these plus neglecting positive activities and spending a lot of time obtaining or using, a total of 7 items each. The final measure for tobacco included four Likert-scale items for symptoms presently experienced including withdrawal, craving, worry over running out, and tolerance, CFI = 0.990.

Concurrent use with video games

For each substance that participants reported having used in the past 30 days, they were asked, “During the past 30 days, have you played video games while using (substance in question) or feeling its effects?”

Video game use and enjoyment

Participants were asked to list, via text entry, up to five video game titles they had “spent a lot of time playing in the past 12 months.” They were asked a series of questions about each title, including on how many days of the past 30 they had played it and how much they enjoyed it. Enjoyment was a single 7-point Likert scale in which 1 indicated “it was the worst game I’ve ever played,” four indicated “about the same as most games,” and 7 indicated “it was my single all-time favorite.” Because all dependent variables in the present analyses were person-level, within-person means were calculated to reflect the properties of the average game that person played. Entries for use and enjoyment variables were only considered valid for 2,885 participants who entered at least one valid title, and titles were considered valid if they could be uniquely identified in GameFaqs [66], a large and comprehensive database of user-generated content maintained and edited by an industry group.

Consumer involvement in video games

Another indicator of video game playing as an enthusiastic hobby [27,6769] as distinct from addiction [70] was a measure of consumer involvement adapted from leisure and marketing studies [7173]. It addresses attraction, centrality/importance, and self-expression, Cronbach’s α= 0.70.

Problem video game playing (PVP)

Because there is still debate about the definition of video game abuse/dependence [23,74,75], and symptoms of any disorder may constitute a problem in living even if they do not meet a clinical threshold, this study operationalized problem video gaming behavior with a continuous Likert scale measure (PVP, [31]). Like our measures for substance use problems, this scale also had to be abridged to fit into the 10-minute median time limit for this survey. Items were selected based on factor analysis of the same data we used to derive the substance use measures. For that study, the original 9-item Likert scale PVP measure had been slightly edited by splitting the longest item (“I have tried to control, cut back, or stop playing, or I usually play with the video games over a longer period than I intended”) into two separate items and deleting “with the” to produce a measure with a total of 10 items. The five highest loading items on the first/only factor across all four estimation procedures available in STATA 11.0 (principal factor, principal-component factor, iterated principal factor, and maximum likelihood factor) addressed increasing time spent playing (tolerance), difficulty controlling time spent playing, restlessness/irritability when can’t play (withdrawal), play to relieve negative affect (self-medication), and engaging in problem behavior in order to play games, CFI = 0.959, α = 0.74. Response choices ranged from “not at all true” to “extremely true,” so that participants who scored anywhere above the lowest possible score at least slightly positively identified with at least one item.

Demographics

Age, race/ethnicity, gender, education, income, and employment status were taken from the Knowledge Networks’ basic demographic survey. No data were missing on these variables. Income was categorized into 19 increments beginning with “less than $5,000” that are increasingly larger further up the scale until the final category, “$175,000 or more.” Education was an ordinal variable with 14 possible categories ranging from “no formal education” to “professional or doctoral degree.” Employment, for purposes of these analyses, was collapsed into categories of (1) working, either for wages or self-employed, or (2) non-working for any reason, e.g., disability, retirement, layoff, etc.

2.3. Approach to Analyses

All analyses employed post-stratification weights provided by Knowledge Networks so that estimates approximate what would have been obtained from a true random sample of English- and Spanish-speaking American adult video game players [76]. Calculated based on current data from the U.S. Census Current Population Survey, Knowledge Networks’ weights adjust for survey non-response and client surveys’ own sample designs, such as our screening procedure. We only present weighted estimates in the results section.
Bivariate relationships among study variables were computed using scale values for consumer involvement, PVP, and tobacco dependence, and count variables for the number of abuse/dependence symptoms for caffeine, alcohol, and marijuana. For these tables, significance flags were adjusted relative to convention in light of multiple tests, so that * indicates p < 0.01 rather than p < 0.05, ** indicates p < 0.001 rather than p < 0.01, etc.
For main hypothesis tests, structural equation models were run in MPlus 6.0, separately by substance. In each model, PVP and substance use problems were continuous latent variables measured by their observed components and allowed to correlate. MPlus is capable of creating a continuous latent variable from binary observed indicators, and this was done for the five indicators of caffeine use problems and the seven indicators of each of marijuana and alcohol use problems. PVP and substance use problems were regressed over the concurrent use binary variable, the observed indicator of substance use frequency, and a latent indicator of video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby. In a separate statement, concurrent use was regressed over substance use frequency and the latent variable for video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby. Substance use frequency and the latent variable for video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby were allowed to correlate. The latent variable of video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby was measured by the observed indicators of game enjoyment, hours played, and the scale score for consumer involvement. In the last statement specifying the model, all of these variables were regressed over demographic variables, so that the structural models described below refer to effects after demographic controls. The binary variable for concurrent use was accommodated in a mediating role through theta parameterization, and post-stratification weights were accommodated using means and variance adjusted weighted least squares (WLSMV) estimation.
Models were determined to have acceptable fit based on RMSEA < 0.05, even though the lowest CFI was 0.89 and the highest was 0.94, which are just below the usual strict standard of 0.95 [77,78]. Alternative specifications were attempted that included only either the observed indicator for consumer involvement or video game play frequency in place of the “enthusiastic hobby” factor, and left “problem behavior to play games” out of PVP. These yielded CFI ≥ 0.95 for all four models and significant paths between concurrent use and PVP for caffeine, tobacco, and marijuana users. However, this specification would have limited the conceptual scope of the study. Observed components of the enthusiastic hobby factor were selected for conceptual completeness according to the logic of a formative indicator [79]; they were not expected to be redundant. Including all components of the enthusiastic hobby indicator was necessary in order to fully distinguish problem video gaming behavior from mere engagement [27]. Without them, we could not be sure the significant paths between concurrent use and PVP were not type I error due to underspecification. Another choice made for conceptual completeness was to leave the PVP measure intact despite the low-loading item and not reduce it even further relative to the original. Given that all RMSEAs were still < 0.05 [77,78] and some diminishment of CFI is forgivable when including variables that are not expected to be correlated but still need to be in the model for conceptual reasons [80], we determined the models described in Figures 14 to be the most authentic representation of the findings among the possibilities.

3. Results

Table 1 provides descriptive statistics on video game and substance use variables, as well as differences by categorical demographic factors.
Concurrent use with all substances was prevalent. Males reported higher consumer involvement, but females report higher enjoyment, and males (within this video gamers only sample) exhibited only marginally higher PVP. Females had more frequent use of and problems with tobacco. Males were, however, more frequent concurrent users with caffeine and alcohol than females. Although Blacks, Asians, and Native Americans had higher PVP than whites, Blacks had higher enjoyment and consumer involvement but not frequency, Asians had higher frequency but not enjoyment or consumer involvement, and Native Americans had higher consumer involvement but not frequency or enjoyment. Whites, however, were the only group to exhibit clearly higher rates of concurrent use issues, and then only with caffeine. The only demographic factor consistently associated with risk for problem use patterns was non-working status, and this only held for video games, tobacco, and alcohol.
Table 2 describes bivariate correlations among continuous study variables. Frequency of game playing, enjoyment of average game, consumer involvement, and PVP were all correlated with each other. All substance use problems variables were correlated with each other except for marijuana with tobacco and PVP. The positive correlation between days played and age was not, according to follow-up analyses (not shown), because of meaningful curvilinearity—the slope of the positive relationship between age and days played was steeper for younger participants and still positive, although more shallow, for older participants. Age was, however, negatively correlated with other game playing variables, including PVP. Consistent with findings about employment in Table 1, income and education were negatively correlated with video game playing frequency, consumer involvement, PVP, tobacco use frequency, tobacco use problems, and alcohol use problems.
Figures 14 describe results of path analysis models for caffeine, tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana users (respectively). For all models, the path from concurrent use to substance use problems was significant, and the correlation between PVP and substance use problems also was significant. Concurrent use was not directly associated with PVP. Video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby was indirectly associated with substance use problems through two paths. The first path was via PVP, which was significant for all four substances. The second was via concurrent use, which was only significant for caffeine, tobacco, and alcohol. An effect via concurrent use with marijuana may have been hard to distinguish because of the low sample size and the high rate of concurrent use among marijuana users (see Table 1). The pattern of significant paths described here also held if a dosage variable (e.g., number of caffeinated drinks, cigarettes, or alcoholic drinks per day of alcohol use) was used instead of the substance use frequency variable.

4. Discussion

Results confirmed that, in models accounting for shared variance between substance use problems and PVP [8,10] and controlling for frequency of substance use, video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby, and demographics, concurrent use (playing video games while using or feeling the effects of substances) was uniquely associated with substance use problems. They did not, however, confirm the hypothesized direct association between concurrent use and PVP. The same pattern of results occurred for all four substances studied. The lack of a significant direct association between concurrent use and PVP makes these results not wholly congruent with previous research findings that alcohol exacerbates problem gambling [1419]. Rather, video gaming as an enthusiastic hobby emerged as a possible “third variable” associated with both concurrent use and PVP. Our models also distinguish the enthusiastic hobby factor as indirectly associated with substance use problems, through PVP (for all for substances) and concurrent use (for caffeine, alcohol, and tobacco). Although demographics are background variables in our models, our bivariate results for demographic variables echo earlier findings that socioeconomic stressors are just as relevant to problem video gaming behavior [64,81] as they are to substance addiction [82].
Factors contributing to this study’s validity are its nationally representative sample collected by a provider whose data are frequently used in research [83] and its use of sampling weights calculated by the provider to correct for biases and authentically represent the population under study [76]. The online nature of the sample and 59% response rate to the screening instrument probably cannot be argued to be limitations in and of themselves: Online data collection has demonstrated validity in this area [84] and, while new communications technology has diminished response rates to all survey methods (phone and postal mail included), point estimates remain stable across methods and at much lower response rates than ours [85]. Another strength of this study was its sample of adults, filling a need noted in previous work for more studies on adults [24] and ensuring that our findings cannot be parsimoniously explained by youth problem behavior theory [61].
In order to meet the 10-minute median length requirement for this survey, established measures for PVP [31] and substance use [65] had to be abridged. We ensured validity of our measures, in part, through use of structural equation modeling – had measures really been invalid, our models would not have had adequate overall fit. Further confidence in our substance use problems and problem video gaming behavior measures can be drawn from their significant bivariate correlations with each other, which are consistent with other research using these concepts [8,10,11]. Another limitation of our measures was that operationalization of our key construct of concurrent use depended on a single question: “During the past 30 days, have you played video games while using [substance in question] or feeling its effects?” Some participants may have interpreted it either too strictly (as an exclusive rather than inclusive “or”) or loosely (reporting on usual behavior rather than thinking specifically about the past 30 days). Although this variation in interpretation may have increased random error, it did not necessarily introduce bias. Further confidence in the validity of this measure can be drawn from the same pattern of associations involving it holding for all four substances under study. Other limitations of this study are those that it shares with every survey study, e.g., dependence on self-report data and participants’ recall of events 30 days or even a year ago. Finally, the scope of the implications of these findings is limited by their basis in cross-sectional data. Although our graphics include arrows indicating directions of effects, structural equation modeling in the context of a study like this mainly offers a heuristic for understanding associations among variables. Our results are not meant to support firm conclusions about causality or how these factors influence each other over time.
Even with these limitations, these findings contribute to a growing understanding that behavioral and substance addictions co-occur [813] and may contribute to each other through influencing or complementing each other’s actual use practices [1418] and activating biologically mediated addictive processes [27]. Understanding concurrent engagement in self-reinforcing behavior and use of addictive substances may be an important consideration in addiction specificity [86], i.e., variability in the development of co-occurring addictive patterns among those engaged in the prerequisite behaviors. Clinical implications of these findings, to the extent that they can be drawn from a non-clinical population, are that clients who present with behavioral addictions or substance use problems should be screened for both and also assessed for concurrent use.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grant R01-DA027761, “Video Games’ Role in Developing Substance Use”, from the National Institute of Drug Abuse.
  • Conflict of InterestNone of the authors has any conflict of interest that might affect the integrity of the results.

References

  1. Kelley, AE; Berridge, KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: Relevance to addictive drugs. J. Neurosci 2002, 22, 3306–3311. [Google Scholar]
  2. López-Moreno, JA; González-Cuevas, G; Moreno, G; Navarro, M. The pharmacology of the endocannabinoid system: Functional and structural interactions with other neurotransmitter systems and their repercussions in behavioral addiction. Addict. Biol 2008, 13, 160–187. [Google Scholar]
  3. Di Chiara, G; Bassareo, V. Reward system and addiction: What dopamine does and doesn’t do. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol 2007, 7, 69–76. [Google Scholar]
  4. Volkow, ND; Fowler, JS; Wang, GJ; Swanson, JM. Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: Results from imaging studies and treatment implications. Mol. Psychiatr 2004, 9, 557–569. [Google Scholar]
  5. Kalivas, PW; O’Brien, C. Drug addiction as a pathology of staged neuroplasticity. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007, 33, 166–180. [Google Scholar]
  6. Robbins, TW; Cador, M; Taylor, JR; Everitt, BJ. Limbic-striatal interactions in reward-related processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 1989, 13, 155–162. [Google Scholar]
  7. De Lecea, L; Jones, BE; Boutrel, B; Borgland, SL; Nishino, S; Bubser, M; DiLeone, R. Addiction and arousal: Alternative roles of hypothalamic peptides. J. Neurosci 2006, 26, 10372–10375. [Google Scholar]
  8. Rozin, P; Stoess, C. Is there a general tendency to become addicted? Addict. Behav 1993, 18, 81–87. [Google Scholar]
  9. El-Guebaly, N; Patten, SB; Currie, S; Williams, JVA; Beck, CA; Maxwell, CJ; Wang, JL. Epidemiological associations between gambling behavior, substance use & mood and anxiety disorders. J. Gamb. Stud 2006, 22, 275–287. [Google Scholar]
  10. Greenberg, JL; Lewis, SE; Dodd, DK. Overlapping addictions and self-esteem among college men and women. Addict. Behav 1999, 24, 565–571. [Google Scholar]
  11. Porter, G; Starcevic, V; Berle, D; Fenech, P. Recognizing problem video game use. Austr. New Zeal. J. Psychiatr 2010, 44, 120–128. [Google Scholar]
  12. Wood, RTA; Gupta, R; Derevensky, JL; Griffiths, MD. Video game playing and gambling in adolescents: Common risk factors. J. Child Adol. Sub. Abuse 2004, 14, 77–100. [Google Scholar]
  13. Sussman, S; Lisha, N; Griffiths, M. Prevalence of the addictions: A problem of the majority or the minority? Eval. Health Profess 2011, 34, 3–56. [Google Scholar]
  14. Baron, E; Dickerson, M. Alcohol consumption and self-control of gambling behaviour. J. Gamb. Stud 1999, 15, 3–15. [Google Scholar]
  15. O’Connor, J; Dickerson, M. Definition and measurement of chasing in off-course betting and gaming machine play. J. Gamb. Stud 2003, 19, 359–386. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cronce, JM; Corbin, WR. Effects of alcohol and initial gambling outcomes on within-session gambling behavior. Experimen. Clin. Psychopharmacol 2010, 18, 145–157. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ellery, M; Stewart, SH; Loba, P. Alcohol’s effects on video lottery terminal (vlt) play among probable pathological and non-pathological gamblers. J. Gamb. Stud 2005, 21, 299–324. [Google Scholar]
  18. Kyngdon, A; Dickerson, M. An experimental study of the effect of prior alcohol consumption on a simulated gambling activity. Addiction 1999, 94, 697–707. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dickerson, M; Baron, E. Contemporary issues and future directions for research into pathological gambling. Addiction 2000, 95, 1145–1159. [Google Scholar]
  20. King, D; Delfabbro, P; Griffiths, M. The convergence of gambling and digital media: Implications for gambling in young people. J. Gamb. Stud 2010, 26, 175–187. [Google Scholar]
  21. King, D; Delfabbro, P; Griffiths, M. Video game structural characteristics: A new psychological taxonomy. Int. J. Mental Health Addict 2010, 8, 90–106. [Google Scholar]
  22. Kuss, D; Griffiths, M. Internet gaming addiction: A systematic review of empirical research. Int J Mental Health Addict 2011. Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Wood, RTA. Problems with the concept of video game “addiction”: Some case study examples. Int. J. Mental Health Addict 2008, 6, 169–178. [Google Scholar]
  24. Petry, NM. Commentary on Van Rooij et al. (2011): Gaming addiction—A psychiatric disorder or not? Addiction 2011, 106, 213–214. [Google Scholar]
  25. American Psychiatric Association. Substance Use and Addictive Disorders; American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA, 2010. Available online: http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevision/Pages/SubstanceUseandAddictiveDisorders.aspx accessed on 13 July 2011.
  26. Young, KS. Understanding online gaming addiction and treatment issues for adolescents. Am. J. Famil. Therap 2009, 37, 355–372. [Google Scholar]
  27. Griffiths, M; Meredith, A. Videogame addiction and its treatment. J. Contemp. Psychotherap 2009, 39, 247–253. [Google Scholar]
  28. Khan, MK; Kantof, EP. Emotional and Behavioral Effects, Including Addictive Potential, of Video Games; American Medical Association: Chicago, IL, USA, 2007. Available online: www.ama-assn.org/ama1/pub/upload/mm/467/csaph12a07.doc accessed on 19 January 2011.
  29. Griffiths, MD. Amusement machine playing in childhood and adolescence: A comparative analysis of video games and fruit machines. J. Adol 1991, 14, 53–73. [Google Scholar]
  30. Fisher, S. Identifying video game addiction in children and adolescents. Addict. Behav 1994, 19, 545–553. [Google Scholar]
  31. Tejeiro Salguero, RA; Bersabé Morán, RM. Measuring problem video game playing in adolescents. Addiction 2002, 97, 1601–1606. [Google Scholar]
  32. Grüsser, SM; Thalemann, R; Griffiths, MD. Excessive computer game playing: Evidence for addiction and aggression? Cyberpsychol. Behav 2007, 10, 290–292. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gentile, DA; Choo, H; Liau, A; Sim, T; Li, D; Fung, D; Khoo, A. Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gentile, DA. Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8 to 18: A national study. Psychol. Sci 2009, 20, 594–602. [Google Scholar]
  35. Desai, RA; Krishnan-Sarin, S; Cavallo, D; Potenza, MN. Video-gaming among high school students: Health correlates, gender differences, and problematic gaming. Pediatrics 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Rehbein, F; Psych, G; Kleimann, M; Mediasci, G; Moßle, T. Prevalence and risk factors of video game dependency in adolescence: Results of a german nationwide survey. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw 2010, 13, 269–277. [Google Scholar]
  37. Thalemann, R; Wölfling, K; Grüsser, SM. Specific cue reactivity on computer game-related cues in excessive gamers. Behav. Neurosci 2007, 121, 614–618. [Google Scholar]
  38. Koepp, MJ; Gunn, RN; Lawrence, AD; Cunningham, VJ; Dagher, A; Jones, T; Brooks, DJ; Bench, CJ; Grasby, PM. Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature 1998, 393, 266–267. [Google Scholar]
  39. Wood, RTA; Griffiths, MD; Chappell, D; Davies, MNO. The structural characteristics of video games: A psycho-structural analysis. Cyberpsychol. Behav 2004, 7, 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  40. Hill, J. Ethical Dilemmas: Exploitative Multiplayer Worlds Don’t Deserve to be Called Art; The Sydney Morning Herald: Sydney, Australia, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  41. Han, DH; Hwang, JW; Renshaw, PF. Bupropion sustained release treatment decreases craving for video games and cue-induced brain activity in patients with internet video game addiction. Experimen. Clin. Psychopharmacol 2010, 18, 297–304. [Google Scholar]
  42. Han, DH; Lee, YS; Na, C; Ahn, JY; Chung, US; Daniels, MA; Haws, CA; Renshaw, PF. The effect of methylphenidate on internet video game play in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Comprehen. Psychiatr 2009, 50, 251–256. [Google Scholar]
  43. Swing, EL; Gentile, DA; Anderson, CA; Walsh, DA. Television and video game exposure and the development of attention problems. Pediatrics 2010, 126, 214–221. [Google Scholar]
  44. Rowan, C. Unplug—Don’t drug: A critical look at the influence of technology on child behavior with an alternative way of responding other than evaluation and drugging. Eth. Human Psychol. Psychiatr. Int. J. Crit. Inquir 2010, 12, 60–68. [Google Scholar]
  45. Grant, BF; Stinson, FS; Dawson, DA; Chou, SP; Dufour, MC; Compton, W; Pickering, RP; Kaplan, K. Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independent mood and anxiety disorders: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr 2004, 61, 807–816. [Google Scholar]
  46. Merikangas, KR; Mehta, RL; Molnar, BE; Walters, EE; Swendsen, JD; Aguilar-Gaziola, S; Bijl, R; Borges, G; Caraveo-Anduaga, JJ; Dewit, DJ; et al. Comorbidity of substance use disorders with mood and anxiety disorders: Results of the international consortium in psychiatric epidemiology. Addict. Behav 1998, 23, 893–907. [Google Scholar]
  47. Dawe, S; Loxton, NJ. The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 2004, 28, 343–351. [Google Scholar]
  48. Bioulac, S; Arfi, L; Bouvard, MP. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and video games: A comparative study of hyperactive and control children. Europ. Psychiatr 2008, 23, 134–141. [Google Scholar]
  49. Chan, PA; Rabinowitz, T. A cross-sectional analysis of video games and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adolescents. Ann Gener Psychiatr 2006, 5, 16:1–16:10. [Google Scholar]
  50. McClernon, FJ; Fuemmeler, BF; Kollins, SH; Kail, ME; Ashley-Koch, AE. Interactions between genotype and retrospective adhd symptoms predict lifetime smoking risk in a sample of young adults. Nicot. Tob. Res 2008, 10, 117–127. [Google Scholar]
  51. Ohlmeier, MD; Peters, K; Wildt, BTT; Zedler, M; Ziegenbein, M; Wiese, B; Emrich, HM; Schneider, U. Comorbidity of alcohol and substance dependence with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (adhd). Alcohol Alcohol 2008, 43, 300–304. [Google Scholar]
  52. Walker, LR; Abraham, AA; Tercyak, KP. Adolescent caffeine use, adhd, and cigarette smoking. Child. Health Care 2010, 39, 73–90. [Google Scholar]
  53. Becker, SJ; Curry, JF. Interactive effect of substance abuse and depression on adolescent social competence. J. Clin. Child Adol. Psychol 2007, 36, 469–475. [Google Scholar]
  54. Lemmens, JS; Valkenburg, PM; Peter, J. Psychosocial causes and consequences of pathological gaming. Comput. Human Behav 2011, 27, 144–152. [Google Scholar]
  55. Anand, V. A study of time management: The correlation between video game usage and academic performance markers. Cyberpsychol. Behav 2007, 10, 552–559. [Google Scholar]
  56. Cox, RG; Zhang, L; Johnson, WD; Bender, DR. Academic performance and substance use: Findings from a state survey of public high school students. J. School Health 2007, 77, 109–115. [Google Scholar]
  57. Gentile, DA; Lynch, PJ; Linder, JR; Walsh, DA. The effects of violent video game habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive behaviors, and school performance. J. Adol 2004, 27, 5–22. [Google Scholar]
  58. Madden, D; Brueckman, J; Littlejohn, KV. A Contrast of Amount and Type of Activity in Elementary School Years between Academically Successful and Unsuccessful Youth; Education Resources Information Center: Washington, DC, USA, 1997. Available online: http://eric.ed.gov/PDFS/ED411067.pdf accessed on 17 October 2011.
  59. Anderson, CA; Gentile, DA; Buckley, KE. Violent Video Game Effects on Children and Adolescents: Theory, Research, and Public Policy; Oxford University Press: Oxford NY, USA, 2007; p. 190. [Google Scholar]
  60. Moss, HB; Tarter, RE. Substance abuse, aggression, and violence: What are the connections? Am. J. Addict 1993, 2, 149–160. [Google Scholar]
  61. Duncan, SC; Duncan, TE; Strycker, LA. Risk and protective factors influencing adolescent problem behavior: A multivariate latent growth curve analysis. Ann. Behav. Med 2000, 22, 103–109. [Google Scholar]
  62. Chiu, SI; Lee, J-Z; Huang, D-H. Video game addiction in children and teenagers in Taiwan. Cyberpsychol. Behav 2004, 7, 571–581. [Google Scholar]
  63. Griffiths, MD; Hunt, N. Dependence on computer games by adolescents. Psychol. Rep 1998, 82, 475–480. [Google Scholar]
  64. Roberts, DF; Foehr, UG. Kids and Media in America; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004; p. 380. [Google Scholar]
  65. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Statistics (SAMHSA). Methodology Reports for the National Household Survey on Drug Abuse & the National Survey on Drug Use & Health; SAMHSA: Rockville, MD, USA, 2009. Available online: http://oas.samhsa.gov/nsduh/methods.cfm accessed on 25 November 2010.
  66. CBS Interactive Inc. Video Game Cheats, Reviews, Faqs, Message Boards, and More— Gamefaqs; CBS Interactive Inc.: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2011. Available online: http://www.gamefaqs.com/ accessed on 15 February 2011.
  67. Charlton, JP; Danforth, IDW. Validating the distinction between computer addiction and engagement: Online game playing and personality. Behav. Inform. Technol 2010, 29, 601–613. [Google Scholar]
  68. Skoric, MM; Teo, LLC; Neo, RL. Children and video games: Addiction, engagement, and scholastic achievement. Cyberpsychol. Behav 2009, 12, 567–572. [Google Scholar]
  69. Griffiths, M. The role of context in online gaming excess and addiction: Some case study evidence. Int. J. Mental Health Addict 2010, 8, 119–125. [Google Scholar]
  70. Griffiths, M. A components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J. Sub. Use 2005, 10, 191–197. [Google Scholar]
  71. Laurent, G; Kapferer, J-N. Measuring consumer involvement profiles. J. Market. Res 1985, 22, 41–53. [Google Scholar]
  72. Wiley, CGE; Shaw, SM; Havitz, ME. Men’s and women’s involvement in sports: An examination of the gendered aspects of leisure involvement. Leisure Sci 2000, 22, 19–31. [Google Scholar]
  73. Gursoy, D; Gavcar, E. International leisure tourists’ involvement profile. Ann. Tour. Res 2003, 30, 906–926. [Google Scholar]
  74. Wood, RTA. A response to Blaszczynski, Griffiths and Turners’ comments on the paper “problems with the concept of video game ‘addiction’: Some case study examples” (this issue). Int. J. Mental Health Addict 2008, 6, 191–193. [Google Scholar]
  75. Blaszczynski, A. Commentary: A response to “problems with the concept of video game ‘addiction’: Some case study examples”. Int. J. Mental Health Addict 2008, 6, 179–181. [Google Scholar]
  76. DiSogra, C. Overview of Knowledgepanel® Statistical Weighting Protocol; Knowledge Networks: New York, NY, USA, 2009. Avaiable online: http://www.knowledgenetworks.com/ganp/docs/knweighting-synopsis.pdf accessed on 25 April 2011.
  77. McDonald, RP; Ho, M-HR. Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psychol. Methods 2002, 7, 64–82. [Google Scholar]
  78. Hooper, D; Coughlan, J; Mullen, MR. Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Electron. J. Busin. Res. Methods 2008, 6, 53–60. [Google Scholar]
  79. Diamantopoulos, A; Winklhofer, H. Index construction with formative indicators: An alternative to scale development. J. Market. Res 2001, 38, 269–277. [Google Scholar]
  80. Kenny, DA. Measuring Model Fit. 2011. Available online: http://www.davidakenny.net/cm/fit.htm accessed on 12 September 2011.
  81. Brady, SS; Matthews, KA. Effects of media violence on health-related outcomes among young men. Arch. Pediatr. Adol. Med 2006, 160, 341–347. [Google Scholar]
  82. Sinha, R. Chronic Stress, Drug Use, and Vulnerability to Addiction. In Annals of the New York Academy of Science; Uhl, GR, Ed.; John Wiley&Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2008; Volume 1141, pp. 105–130. [Google Scholar]
  83. Knowledge Networks. Knowledge Networks Bibliography; Knowledge Networks: New York, NY, USA, 2011. Available online: http://www.knowledgenetworks.com/ganp/docs/KNBibliography.pdf accessed on 17 September 2011.
  84. Griffiths, M. The use of online methodologies in data collection for gambling and gaming addictions. Int. J. Mental Health Addict 2010, 8, 8–20. [Google Scholar]
  85. Zukin, C. Game Change: The Challenge of Finding and Interviewing a Random Sample in 2010; Knowledge Networks Seminars: New York, NY, USA, 28 October 2010. [Google Scholar]
  86. Sussman, S; Leventhal, A; Bluthenthal, RN; Freimuth, M; Forster, M; Ames, SL. A framework for the specificity of addictions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2011, 8, 3399–3415. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and caffeine use problems among caffeine users.N = 1,961. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.035, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.893. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.850. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and caffeine use problems among caffeine users.N = 1,961. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.035, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.893. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.850. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 08 03979f1
Figure 2. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and tobacco use problems among tobacco users.N = 683. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.026, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.934. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.904. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and tobacco use problems among tobacco users.N = 683. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.026, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.934. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.904. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 08 03979f2
Figure 3. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and alcohol use problems among alcohol users.N = 1,018. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.025, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.936. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.915. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 3. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and alcohol use problems among alcohol users.N = 1,018. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.025, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.936. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.915. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 08 03979f3
Figure 4. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and marijuana use problems among marijuana users.N = 133. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.038, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.891. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.857. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 4. Structural equations model for effect of concurrent use on PVP and marijuana use problems among marijuana users.N = 133. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.038, Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.891. Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = 0.857. Heavy lines indicate paths testing study hypotheses, light lines indicate control/measurement model paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Coefficients standardized after estimation; † indicates parameter constrained to be 1 for estimation. Model controls for age, educational level, gender, income, employment status, and race. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 08 03979f4
Table 1. Overall means/proportions for game playing and substance use variables, and differences by demographic factors.
Table 1. Overall means/proportions for game playing and substance use variables, and differences by demographic factors.
Overall mean(sd) or proportionGenderRaceWorking



MaleFemaleWhiteBlackLatinoAsianNativeOtherYesNo
Video Games
weighted n(users) = 2,869.5
Days played average game9.8(8.5)9.510.2 +9.9 (ref)9.39.312.2 *9.27.98.611.4 ***
Enjoyment of average game5.2(1.1)5.195.30 *5.21 (ref)5.44 ***5.245.145.305.125.225.26
Consumer involvement2.1(0.8)2.232.03 ***2.12 (ref)2.38 ***2.02 +2.182.44 *2.202.102.20 *
Problem video game play1.6(0.6)1.581.53 +1.53 (ref)1.64 *1.541.73 *1.78 *1.641.501.64 ***



Caffeine
weighted n(users) = 1,849.3
Any use of caffeine64%63%67% +70% (ref)44% ***51% ***63%72%66%66%62%
Days/past 30 used caffeine a24.4(8.7)24.424.425.6 (ref)19.6 ***20.8 ***22.0 **21.5 *23.224.624.1
Caffeine use problems a1.08(1.40)1.011.18 *1.07 (ref)1.021.081.321.411.011.061.12
Concurrent use with caffeine a41%45%35% ***44% (ref)34% +27% ***19% **35%47%39%43%



Tobacco
weighted n(users) = 748.7
Any use of tobacco26%27%25%27% (ref)26%19% *19%44% +27%23%30% **
Days/past 30 used tobacco a24.0(10.6)22.027.2 ***24.4 (ref)24.622.626.419.8 +19.0 +22.425.8 ***
Tobacco use problems a2.55(1.20)2.372.83 ***2.58 (ref)2.512.323.19 +2.442.082.362.76 ***
Concurrent use with tobacco a61%62%59%61% (ref)66%49%28% *76%89% +52%70% ***



Alcohol
weighted n(users) = 964.6
Any use of alcohol34%40%25% ***35% (ref)33%31%15% **38%41%37%29% ***
Days/past 30 used alcohol a11.0(8.8)11.310.311.8 (ref)9.0 *8.48.08.2 **12.610.412.0 *
Alcohol use problems a1.33(1.70)1.341.331.28 (ref)1.441.500.742.86 **0.971.211.55 *
Concurrent use with alcohol a38%42%28% **40% (ref)42%21%14%46%38%34%44% *



Marijuana
weighted n(users) = 162.0
Any use of marijuana5.6%6.4%4.7%5.7% (ref)7.4%5.5% **0%3.5%7.5%5.0%6.6%
Days/past 30 used marijuana a19.1(11.3)19.718.119.1 (ref)17.321.1bb16.919.518.8
Marijuana use problems a2.28(2.10)2.501.862.07 (ref)2.802.91bb2.341.982.60
Concurrent use with marijuana a80%84%72%80% (ref)76%77%bb100%78%81%
aOnly users of the substance in question included in these substance-specific analysis.
bFigures based on <5 real cases omitted.
+p < 0.05,
*p < 0.01,
**p < 0.001,
***p < 0.0001.
Table 2. Correlations among game playing, substance use, and continuous demographic variables.
Table 2. Correlations among game playing, substance use, and continuous demographic variables.
Days playedGame enjoymentConsumer involvementProblem play (PVP)Caffeine daysaCaffeine problemsaTobacco daysaTobacco problemsaAlcohol daysaAlcohol problemsaMarijuana daysaMarijuana problemsa
Enjoyment of avg. game0.14 ***10.18 ***0.16 ***0.020.07 *0.050.08 +−0.040.040.120.13
Consumer involvement0.21 ***0.18 ***10.57 ***−0.06 *0.07 *0.060.13 **−0.050.14 ***0.070.19 +
Problem video game play0.28 ***0.16 ***0.57 ***1−0.08 **0.24 ***0.08 +0.33 ***−0.030.22 ***−0.060.27 *
Days caffeine use0.11 ***0.02−0.06 *−0.08 **10.09 **0.14 *0.080.16 ***−0.060.44 ***−0.10
Caffeine use problems0.010.07 *0.07 *0.24 ***0.09 **10.020.33 ***−0.12 *0.37 ***0.160.51 ***
Days tobacco use0.17 ***0.050.060.08 +0.14 *0.0210.48 ***0.11−0.010.26 +−0.21
Tobacco use problems0.16 ***0.08 +0.13 **0.33 ***0.080.33 ***0.48 ***10.030.21 **0.08−0.11
Days used alcohol0.08 +−0.04−0.05−0.030.16 ***−0.12 *0.110.0310.23 ***0.16−0.25 +
Alcohol use problems0.020.040.14 ***0.22 ***−0.060.37 ***−0.010.21 **0.23 ***10.010.31 *
Days used marijuana0.18 +0.120.07−0.060.44 ***0.160.26 +0.080.160.0110.24 *
Marijuana use problems0.24 *0.130.19 +0.27 *−0.100.51 ***−0.21−0.11−0.25 +0.31 *0.24 *1

Age0.22 ***−0.13 ***−0.18 ***−0.13 ***0.24 ***−0.16 ***0.13 *0.030.29 ***−0.20***0.11−0.14
Education−0.13 ***−0.05 *−0.07 **−0.09 ***0.000.00−0.16 ***−0.19 ***0.03−0.14 ***−0.05−0.13
Income−0.11 ***−0.05−0.11 ***−0.12 ***0.06 *−0.04−0.13 **−0.16 ***0.00−0.13 **−0.18 +−0.01
aOnly users of the substance in question included in substance-specific analyses.
+p < 0.05,
*p < 0.01,
**p < 0.001,
***p < 0.0001.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ream, G.L.; Elliott, L.C.; Dunlap, E. Playing Video Games While Using or Feeling the Effects of Substances: Associations with Substance Use Problems. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2011, 8, 3979-3998. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph8103979

AMA Style

Ream GL, Elliott LC, Dunlap E. Playing Video Games While Using or Feeling the Effects of Substances: Associations with Substance Use Problems. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2011; 8(10):3979-3998. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph8103979

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ream, Geoffrey L., Luther C. Elliott, and Eloise Dunlap. 2011. "Playing Video Games While Using or Feeling the Effects of Substances: Associations with Substance Use Problems" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 8, no. 10: 3979-3998. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph8103979

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop