Next Article in Journal
What Are the Best Pollinator Candidates for Camelia oleifera: Do Not Forget Hoverflies and Flies
Next Article in Special Issue
Morphology of the Female Reproductive System of the Soybean Thrips, Neohydatothrips variabilis (Beach, 1896) (Thysanoptera: Thripidae)
Previous Article in Journal
Microscopic Identification of Anatomical Elements and Chemical Analysis of Secondary Nests of Vespa velutina nigrithorax du Buyson
Previous Article in Special Issue
Correction: Loredo Varela, R.C.; Fail, J. Host Plant Association and Distribution of the Onion Thrips, Thrips tabaci Cryptic Species Complex. Insects 2022, 13, 298
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Colour Response in Western Flower Thrips Varies Intraspecifically

by
Karla Lopez-Reyes
1,*,
Karen F. Armstrong
1,†,
David A. J. Teulon
2,†,
Ruth C. Butler
2,
Coby van Dooremalen
3,
Monika Roher
4 and
Robert W. H. M. van Tol
3,5
1
Bio-Protection Research Centre, Lincoln University, Lincoln 7647, New Zealand
2
New Zealand Institute for Plant and Food Research Limited, Private Bag 4704, Christchurch 8140, New Zealand
3
Bees@wur, Wageningen University and Research Centre, Droevendaalsesteeg 1, 6708 PB Wageningen, The Netherlands
4
Faculty of Agriculture, University of Zagreb, Svetošimunska cesta 25, 10000 Zagreb, Croatia
5
BugResearch Consultancy, Ambachtstraat 29, 6707 JJ Wageningen, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Better Border Biosecurity (B3, B3nz.org.nz), New Zealand.
Submission received: 10 May 2022 / Revised: 6 June 2022 / Accepted: 8 June 2022 / Published: 10 June 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Economic Pest Thrips: Biology, Ecology, and Population Genetics)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

The western flower thrips (WFT) is a major glasshouse pest worldwide. Colour and odour are used to attract WFT to traps for monitoring in pest management. However, the use of colour attraction for surveillance, or its potential as a control tool for mass trapping, has not progressed due to ongoing debate about the optimal colour. Blue is commonly used for monitoring, but yellow can also be preferred over blue, confounding the implications for field application. As methodological differences amongst studies could cause this discrepancy, in the present study we evaluated populations from Germany (DE) and the Netherlands (NL) that had previously expressed different preferences for blue and yellow. Examining the behavioural response of WFT to blue or yellow LEDs in a wind tunnel by using the same methodology and facilities, the two populations maintained their original differential preferences. This remained after several generations under common conditions, although a significant increase in attraction to yellow was observed for both populations when reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. This is the first report demonstrating that WFT populations can differ in their colour preferences even under the same experimental circumstances. Given that research is typically achieved with populations isolated from the wild for many generations, caution is still needed when extrapolating this to field situations.

Abstract

Discrepancies in the published research as to the attraction of the economically important pest western flower thrips (WFT) to different colours confounds the optimisation of field traps for pest management purposes. We considered whether the different experimental conditions of independent studies could have contributed to this. Therefore, the behavioural response (i.e., landings) to different colour cues of two WFT laboratory populations from Germany (DE) and The Netherlands (NL), which had previously been independently shown to have different colour preferences, were tested in the same place, and under the same experimental conditions. Single-choice wind tunnel bioassays supported previous independent findings, with more of a NL population landing on the yellow LED lamp (588 nm) than the blue (470 nm) (p = 0.022), and a not-statistically significant trend observed in a DE population landing more on blue compared to yellow (p = 0.104). To account for potential original host rearing influences, both populations were subsequently established on bean for ~20 weeks, then yellow chrysanthemum for 4–8 and 12–14 weeks and tested in wind tunnel choice bioassays. Laboratory of origin, irrespective of the host plant rearing regime, remained a significant effect (p < 0.001), with 65% of the NL WFT landing on yellow compared to blue (35%), while 66% of the DE WFT landed on blue compared to yellow (34%). There was also a significant host plant effect (p < 0.001), with increased response to yellow independent of laboratory of origin after rearing on chrysanthemum for 12–14 weeks. Results suggest that differing responses of WFT populations to colour is, in this case, independent of the experimental situation. Long-term separate isolation from the wild cannot be excluded as a cause, and the implications of this for optimising the trap colour is discussed.

1. Introduction

The western flower thrips (WFT) (Frankliniella occidentalis Pergande) is an economically important pest insect with a worldwide distribution affecting numerous horticultural crops. It causes direct cosmetic damage to flowers and fruits and is an important vector of tospoviruses in plants, which can lead to significant yield losses. Due to the pesticide resistance of WFT [1,2,3] and a stricter regulation of pesticides over environmental and human health concerns [4,5,6], more sustainable practices for pest control in agricultural production are desirable [6,7,8]. Colour is commonly used to attract WFT to the sticky traps to monitor populations [9,10], sometimes in combinations with odorant semiochemicals [11]. However, a better understanding of the attraction to colour could enhance captures for earlier detection and optimal timing of insecticide sprays as well as improve integrated pest management practices through mass trapping, lure and kill, or lure and infect tools [9,12]. Accordingly, although the effectiveness of traps for WFT has been explored extensively with the use of semiochemicals [11], more recently, the attraction of WFT to coloured light-emitting diodes (LEDs) [13,14,15,16] has been explored. For scientific research and for more a precise evaluation of thrips colour attraction, the use of LEDs has some advantages over otherwise coloured traps. In particular, LEDs do not rely on the reflection of ambient illumination and the intensity of the lights can be accurately controlled to remove any confounding interactions between the wavelength composition and light intensity. Additionally, LEDs emit narrower spectral curves compared to broader curves reflected by the coloured or painted surfaces used as traps, which allows for more specific wavelengths to be examined.
Despite multiple independent field and laboratory behavioural studies using coloured sticky traps or LEDs, plus continued widespread use of blue and yellow traps for pest management, there are still inconsistencies in the literature regarding the most effective colour for catching pest species of thrips in general [13,17]. For WFT, various studies have reported that blue is the most attractive colour [15,18,19,20,21], although other studies report that it is yellow [16,22,23,24,25] and some have shown differences in colour preference within the same study [26,27,28]. This apparent difference for blue and yellow is confounded by the range of extrinsic and intrinsic factors used in these different studies, which are known to have an effect on the behaviour of arthropods [29,30] including thrips [14,31,32,33,34]. Amongst these factors, the most obvious relates to methodological differences such as the rearing conditions, the preparation of insects prior to experiments, different experimental apparatus, and different conditions such as temperature, illumination, time of evaluation, duration of experiment, etc. Even the type of glue used on traps can influence the blue and yellow preference of WFT [35]. However, there are currently no studies that have considered such differences in the methodology as the cause for varying conclusions as to WFT attraction to blue and yellow. To test this, we used the same experimental design, conditions, and place to examine the responses to colour of two WFT populations recorded elsewhere as having different levels of attraction to blue and yellow.
Two laboratory WFT populations were tested here, reared at Wageningen University and Research (Wageningen, The Netherlands), which had previously shown a higher attraction to yellow compared to other colours including blue [35], and Leibniz University Hannover (Hannover, Germany), which had independently shown a clear preference for blue compared to other colour cues including yellow [15,20]. The main objective was to observe these two populations in the same wind tunnel facility, under the same experimental conditions and host plant rearing regime to test the hypothesis that the differing response of these populations is independent of the experimental protocol. We also examined whether changing the host plant species had an influence on their colour preference. If differences in the behavioural responses to blue and yellow colours were reduced, this might infer that previous discrepancies were a consequence of different methodologies as opposed to biological intraspecific variation. In this way, alternative hypotheses may then be developed to advance our understanding of factors underlying the colour preference of WFT.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Insects: Management and Rearing of Colonies from The Netherlands and Germany

Base WFT populations were obtained from two different laboratory colonies: Wageningen University and Research (WUR), The Netherlands “NL” and the Institute of Horticultural Production Systems (Section of Phytomedicine), Leibniz Universität Hannover (LUH) “DE”. These were used in a molecular test for species status. All laboratory colonies derived from these two base WFT populations “DE” and “NL” were annotated according to the country of the institution from which they were obtained. All colonies were reared at WUR (Biointeractions and Plant Health Department) for the duration of this study. They were strictly isolated from each other in different climate chambers and rooms but with the same environmental conditions of 25–26 °C, 50–70% relative humidity (RH), and 16:8 light:dark period with lighting provided with Philips GreenPower LED Interlighting production module (Philips Electronics, Eindhoven, The Netherlands).
The Dutch base colony (NL) had been reared for at least 15 years, mostly on bean pods (Phaseolus vulgaris L.) to maintain a subsistence population and then occasionally on outsourced chrysanthemum potted plants (Chrysanthemum morifolium Ramat.) to increase the numbers for experimental work when needed. This was the same colony used by van Tol et al. [35] (see Figure 5 in that paper). A derived colony NLc was established from NL by rearing on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum potted plants (white-flowered plants were occasionally used if yellow could not be provided) for ~52 weeks and tested for attraction as a single-choice for a range of colours (Experiment i) (Figure 1).
The base German colony (DE) had been reared for approximately 20 years, mostly on whole bean plants (P. vulgaris L.). After receiving a small sample of this population at WUR (25 September 2018), the colony DEc was derived from it (Figure 1) and reared on yellow chrysanthemum plants for ~6 weeks. Logistical issues prevented an assessment of the DEc at 52 weeks and ~6 weeks was the minimum time needed to build up the colony at WUR to establish its response to blue and yellow in a single-choice colour preference test (Experiment ii).
For each NL and DE base colony, a further three derived colonies were established on different host-plant rearing regimes for comparison in the colour choice tests (Experiment iii). Colonies NLb and DEb were established from NL and DE, respectively, and reared on fresh green bean pods for at least ~20 weeks. The NLbc4-8 and DEbc4-8 colonies were established from NLb and DEb, respectively, and reared on chrysanthemum for 4 to 8 weeks. Finally, colonies NLbc12-14 and DEbc12-14 were a continuation of NLbc4-8 and DEbc4-8, respectively, and continued to be reared on chrysanthemum for 12 to 14 weeks (Figure 1).
Reflectance spectra of the chrysanthemum flowers and bean pods used for the rearing of all colonies can be found in Supplementary Figure S3.

2.2. DNA Analysis

To confirm the species status of the WFT colonies, standard cytochrome oxidase subunit I (COI) barcoding was undertaken for NL and DE as well as the other populations for an ‘evolutionary’ context. The populations for evolutionary analysis were collected in New Zealand, which included the New Zealand greenhouse WFT and biological strain variant New Zealand “Lupin” WFT [36], in addition to closely related species outgroups of Frankliniella intonsa, and Thrips tabaci (Onion Thrips, OT) (Supplementary Table S2). Three specimens from each of the six thrips taxa described above were used. Specimens were stored in 96% EtoH or propylene glycol at ~4 °C until DNA extraction. Total DNA was extracted from whole single insects based on a standardised method [37] (Appendix 1 therein), using the Animal Tissue protocol from the DNeasy Blood and Tissue Kit (Qiagen, Venlo, The Netherlands) and final elution with 100 µL of elution buffer pre-heated to 56 °C to increase the final DNA concentration. PCR amplification and sequencing were carried out using the method from OEPP/EPPO [37] (Appendix 1 therein) with PCR primers LCO1490 and HCO2198 [38]. PCR amplification followed the manufacturer’s instructions for the Bio-X-ACT Short mix (BiolinePty. Ltd., Sydney, Australia) with a PCR reaction volume of 25 µL including 5 µL of the DNA template. The PCR cycle was 3 min at 94 °C, followed by 40 cycles of 30 s at 94 °C, 1 min at 51 °C, 1 min at 72 °C, and a final extension of 10 min at 72 °C. Sanger sequencing was carried out with BigDye™ Terminator v3.1 Cycle Sequencing Kit and the 3500XL Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA).
Consensus sequences for each specimen were generated by aligning the forward and reverse sequences using the MUSCLE algorithm in MEGA X [39]. All sequences obtained were deposited in the NCBI GenBank under accession numbers ON310562–ON310579; populations from which those specimens were taken are vouchered in the Lincoln University Entomology Research Collection. Additionally, 76 DNA sequences for the same COI region of closely related species Frankliniella occidentalis, F. borinquen, F. intonsa, and Thrips tabaci (as the outgroup) were randomly selected and downloaded from BOLD (https://www.boldsystems.org/; accessed on 5 March 2021) (Supplementary Table S3). All sequences were used in a neighbour-joining analysis in MEGA X [39] using the pair-wise Kimura-2-parameter (K2P) model [40] and a bootstrap of 10,000 replicates. Genetic distances among the taxa were visualised in a neighbour-joining tree as a summary of intra- and inter-specific diversity. Species identification of each of the 18 specimens of six different thrips samples was conducted using the BOLD Identification System (IDS) (https://www.boldsystems.org/; accessed on 5 March 2021), which uses >5000 thrips barcode sequences for comparison.

2.3. Wind Tunnel Setup

The experimental arrangement was similar to that described in van Tol et al. [35]. All experiments were conducted at the WUR facility in a wind tunnel 3.0 m (l) × 1.30 m (w) × 0.7 m (h) made of transparent glass (Figure 2). The test arena was located in the middle of the wind tunnel and the ceiling above it was covered with a transparent polyethylene diffusing sheet (Suncover Nectarine C-980, Ginegar Plastic Products Ltd., Ginnegar, Israel). The floor of the three compartments was black. Wind speed inside the wind tunnel was set at 0.3 m/s, temperature at 26 °C, and RH at 65–70%.
The LED lamp, used as the main colour stimulus for the thrips in the wind tunnel, consisted of a 3D-printed black dome (Figure 3) with a 18.5 cm diameter covered inside with aluminium foil to increase the light reflection inside the lamp with a division in the dome that effectively partitioned the lamp in half (Figure 3A). A light diffusing glass plate (Edmund Optics Ltd., York, UK) was glued at the distal part of the lamp with an effective area of 103.86 cm2 where thrips could land (Figure 3C). For each trial, a thin layer of sticky glue P300I35 (Intercol Industrial adhesives, Ede, The Netherlands) was evenly spread on one side of a 100 µm transparent polypropylene sheet (Staples Solutions, Amsterdam, The Netherlands), which was placed on the exterior of the glass plate and taped to the removable frame of the lamp (Figure 3C,D). The LEDs were placed in the back of the lamp (Figure 3E) and positioned so that light would shine forward through the diffusing glass plate. Up to two coloured LED lights could be housed in the dome at the same time (Figure 3B,F), which could be the same or a different colour. When only one or two LEDs were used, the holes without LEDs were covered with cork to avoid light escaping. The lamp was connected to a clamp and positioned at an angle of 45° in relation to the wind-tunnel floor, facing down on the test arena. The centre of the lamp was fixed at 60 cm from the floor and the release platform (height 8 cm) was placed 56 cm away from the base of the lamp.
General lighting in the wind tunnel was provided by LED strips situated on the ceiling over the test arena (Figure 2, black arrows). This had a light spectrum component in the visible range 400–750 nm (LED-strip–Full-colour RGB + Warm White—24V High Power Protected 5050, LuxaLight, Eindhoven, The Netherlands) and a component in the ultraviolet light (UV-A) range from 360 to 390 nm (strip of UV LED Engin, LZ4-04UV00, Osram Sylvania Inc., Wilmington, MA, USA) (Supplementary Figure S1). The amount of UV-A emitted from the ceiling LEDs was set as 2% of the visible light to approximate that found in natural sunlight.
The emission spectra and intensity for the LED ceiling strips and lamp stimulus were measured and adjusted inside the wind tunnel using a broadband spectroradiometer Specbos 1211UV (JETI Technische Instrumente GmbH, Jena, Germany). The intensity of the light from the lamp was controlled by manipulating the amperes in the power controllers connected to the LEDs. All LEDs used in the lamp were set to a spectral radiance of 9.0 × 1017 photons·s−1·sr−1·m−2 (i.e., all LEDs emitted the same amount of light). No difference in LED spectral radiance was found with or without the polypropylene sheet and glue that were placed on the lamp to catch the WFT when measured with the spectroradiometer.

2.4. Experiments with LED Colours

Adult females (distinguished from males by their larger size) of unknown age and mating status were removed from the relevant colony by an aspirator, sedated with CO2 for 15–20 s, and then 100 were transferred to a plastic transparent container (‘release container’) with the dimensions of 7 cm (h) × 7 cm (d). The release container was covered on the top with Parafilm (Bemis, Neenah, WI, USA) and placed on the release platform for 1 h before starting the experiments without water or food for the thrips. Each replicate tested the response of 100 insects. The thrips were released simultaneously into the tunnel from the release platform by removing the Parafilm with minimal physical disturbance. Observations were made on the position of the thrips in the test arena after 6 h. Several replicates were conducted per experiment, but only one 6 h trial was undertaken per day to ensure that the wind tunnel was clear of any WFT remaining from the previous trial. The experiments conducted were:
Experiment i:Single-choice response of NLc to different LED colours.
Seven different LED spectra were presented to the thrips in separate trials (Table 1). The spectral range spanned the UV and visible light from 360 to 700 nm (Supplementary Figure S2, Supplementary Table S1; the spectral sensitivity of WFT shown in Supplementary Figure S4). Data from the treatments evaluating the higher light intensities were included in this experiment (data not shown as no statistically significant differences were observed), but all data were combined and included in the final statistical analysis to consider the effect of colour. Four replicate runs of each light spectrum were carried out. The running order of the treatments and replicates was generated with CycDesign 5.1 (VSN International Ltd. 2013, Hemel Hempstead, UK) using a Latinised resolvable block design to distribute replicates of treatments in “blocks” for the even distribution of treatments over time. Each block comprised six days. The number of thrips trapped on the glass plate of the LED lamp and the ones remaining in the release container (dead and alive) were recorded.
Experiment ii:Single-choice response of DEc to blue and yellow LEDs.
This experiment was conducted to assess whether the previously observed preference for blue of the DE population [15,20] was reproduced at the WUR facilities. The same experimental setup and conditions as described in Experiment i were used, but only the colours blue (477 nm) or yellow (588 nm) (Supplementary Figure S2) were presented to the thrips as a single treatment, with four replicates per treatment. Treatments were run in a systematic way, alternating between the colours. The number of thrips caught on the glass plate of the LED lamp and the ones remaining inside the release container (dead and alive) were recorded at the end of each replicate.
Experiment iii:Choice-test response to blue and yellow by NL and DE WFT from different host plant rearing regimes.
To test the potential influence of immediate colony history as an aspect beyond experimental setup on the attraction toward blue and yellow, a choice bioassay examined the effects of the laboratory of origin and host-plant rearing regime using the colonies derived from NL and DE (Figure 1). In contrast to Experiments i and ii, the blue (477 nm) and yellow (588 nm) LEDs were presented at the same time, adjacent to each other in the lamp. Six derived colonies were tested, being those reared on bean pods (NLb and DEb), transferred from bean to chrysanthemum for 4–8 weeks (NLbc4-8 and DEbc4-8), and transferred from bean to chrysanthemum for 12–14 weeks (NLbc12-14 and DEbc12-14) (Figure 1). Each of the six treatments was replicated six times. The running order of the WFT from both countries was partly randomised. Practical necessity meant that thrips from NLb and DEb were run over two blocks of days, whilst all of the replicates for NLbc4-8 and DEbc4-8 were run in a single block, and similarly NLbc12-14 and DEbc12-14 were also run in a single block. The left and right position of the colours was alternated between replicates. Light spectra of the blue and yellow LEDs used are illustrated in Supplementary Figure S2.
After 6 h, the number of WFT were counted and included in five categories: either (1) remaining inside the release container (dead or alive), (2) on the ceiling under the UV-A illumination, or having landed on (3) the blue or (4) the yellow sides of the sticky glass plate. All thrips that flew out of the release container but did not respond to any light stimuli from the previous categories were assumed to be (5) elsewhere inside the wind tunnel.

2.5. Data Analysis

Experiment i and ii: Percentages were calculated for insects caught on the glass plate of the LED lamp out of the 100 released for each treatment and replicate, and the mean percentages calculated per treatment. For both Experiments i and ii, the number of thrips caught on the sticky glass plate after six hours, out of the total number of thrips that were released inside the wind tunnel, was analysed using a binomial generalised linear model [41] with a logit link function and the dispersion was estimated. Because there was a median of only one dead thrips per run, the number of WFT released (100) was used as the binomial total, not adjusting for dead WFT. Treatment effects were assessed using F-tests. The percentage of thrips on the sticky glass plate and associated 95% confidence limits were calculated from predictions made on the link (logit) scale, and back-transformed for presentation. All analyses were carried out with Genstat [42].
Experiment iii: Initial data analyses (not shown) showed no significant evidence of a difference between the left or right position of the blue and yellow LEDs (p > 0.05). Therefore, the data were analysed ignoring the LED side position. Because the proportion of thrips dead or remaining in the release container did not vary strongly between treatments (p > 0.2 and p > 0.3, respectively), these categories were ignored in the subsequent analyses. Data were analysed in two ways, referred to as ‘analysis a’ and ‘analysis b’ here after.
  • Analysis a: WFT that landed on blue or yellow on the glass plate of the LED lamp.
This analysis focused simply on the attraction to the lamp as reported in other studies (see Discussion), considering only the percentage of thrips that landed on blue or yellow only, out of the total that landed on both colours (see Section 2.4, Experiment iii—sum of categories (3) and (4)). Data were analysed using a binomial generalised linear model (GLM) [41] with a logit link, the dispersion estimated, and the total on the sticky glass plate as the binomial total. The effect of host rearing regime, laboratory of origin, and the interaction between them were assessed using F-tests for both analyses. The estimated percentages of thrips on each light colour and their associated 95% confidence intervals (CI) were obtained on the link scale and back-transformed for presentation.
  • Analysis b: Response of WFT relative to the thrips that left the release container.
This analysis was carried out to include thrips that did not land on the sticky glass plate but flew out of the release container. Thus, the data were analysed as the percentage of thrips at each of the four remaining categories ((2) blue, (3) yellow, (4) ceiling, and (5) elsewhere, see Section 2.4, Experiment iii) out of the total number of thrips that left the release container. In this analysis, because there were more than two categories included, the data were analysed using a Poisson log-linear model for multinomial data [41] with the dispersion estimated. The estimated percentages and associated confidence limits were obtained using separate binomial analysis for each of the categories, similarly to the binomial analysis above, but using the estimated dispersion from the multinomial analysis.

3. Results

3.1. Species Status of the Dutch (NL) and German (DE) Colonies

The DNA barcode identification for the NL and DE specimens showed a 100% match to other F. occidentalis in the large thrips dataset (Supplementary Table S4) in the BOLD Identification System (IDS). Furthermore, the neighbour-joining analysis of 18 new sequences generated in this study, together with the 76 selected independently published sequences, confirmed that the NL and DE populations used for the experiments in this study clearly fall into the F. occidentalis clade (Supplementary Figure S5). Consistent with this, the ~1.18% genetic distance between the NL and DE colonies (Supplementary Table S5) does not support them as different species based on the >10-fold genetic distances between F. occidentalis and other species within the genus such as F. borinquen (16%) and F. intonsa (23%) (Supplementary Table S5). The distance between the biological “glasshouse” and “lupin” strains of F. occidentalis was ~4%, which is greater than between the NL and DE F. occidentalis (Supplementary Table S5). Genetic variation within the DE colony was lower (0.12%) than that within the NL colony (2.08%).

3.2. Experiment i: Response of NLc to Different LED Colours

In a single-choice test, the percentage of NL WFT reared on chrysanthemum that were caught on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp varied significantly with the light colour (p < 0.001) (Figure 4). The two most preferred colours were UV-A and yellow. UV-A light attracted the highest mean percentage with 37.3%, closely followed by yellow with a mean of 35.5% (p = 0.789 compared to UV). Blue was the third most attractive colour, with a mean of 21%, but this was significantly lower than the WFT caught by UV-A and yellow (p = 0.011 and 0.022, respectively). The rest of the colours evaluated attracted similar mean percentages of WFT to each other (violet at 12.8%, red 10.8%, cyan 7.5% and green 7.3%), which were considerably lower than that of UV-A, yellow, or blue (Supplementary Table S6).

3.3. Experiment ii: Response of DEc to Blue and Yellow LEDs

In a single-choice test, the DE WFT reared on chrysanthemum showed a trend towards blue over yellow, with 34% caught on blue compared to 24% on yellow (Figure 5), although the difference was not statistically significant (p = 0.104).

3.4. Experiment iii: Blue and Yellow LED Choice Test of Dutch (NL) and German (DE) WFT from Different Host Plant Rearing Regimes

The influence of the laboratory of origin and immediate host history, and host rearing regime were evaluated by observing (a) how the numbers of WFT caught were divided between the yellow and blue halves of the lamp, and (b) that number relative to the insects that left the release container and were registered in other places inside the wind tunnel.

3.4.1. Analysis a: Response of WFT that Landed on Blue or Yellow on the Glass Plate of the LED Lamp

Considering only those WFT that had been attracted to the lamp, there was a statistically significant variation between the countries of origin and rearing regimes (p < 0.001 for both the laboratory of origin and rearing regime the as main effects) for the percentage that were caught on the yellow or blue half of the lamp, with a similar difference between rearing on different host plants for the laboratory of origin (p = 0.118 for the interaction) (Figure 6). Related to the laboratory of origin, on average, a higher percentage of NL thrips landed on yellow (65%) compared to the DE thrips (34%), while a higher percentage of DE thrips landed on blue (66%) compared to the NL thrips (35%). This difference remained even after rearing on different host plants, with the exception that NLb showed no clear preference to blue or yellow. Related to host rearing regime, on average, a lower proportion of thrips that had been reared on bean (NLb) and DEb) landed on yellow (39%) compared to those that landed on blue (61%). In contrast, for the colonies reared on chrysanthemum for 4–8 weeks (NLbc4-8 and DEbc4-8), on average, landings on yellow (61%) were higher compared to landings on blue (39%). This trend was maintained for the 12–14 weeks of chrysanthemum rearing (NLbc12-14 and DEbc12-14), with a higher average of WFT landing on yellow (56%) compared to blue (44%). Effectively, the average landings of thrips on yellow from both the NL and DE colonies increased when reared on chrysanthemum after being reared on bean.

3.4.2. Analysis b: Response of WFT Relative to the Thrips That Left the Release Container

In addition to WFT landing on the blue or yellow halves of the sticky glass plate, the distributions of those that left the release container were also recorded as either on the ceiling (UV-A illuminated section) or elsewhere. The overall pattern of the distribution of thrips across the four locations varied significantly between the laboratory of origin (p < 0.001) as well as rearing regime (p = 0.001) with a statistically significant interaction between the effects (p = 0.004 for the interaction) (Figure 7).
As for analysis a, irrespective of the host rearing regime, the percentage of WFT landing on yellow was greater on average for the NL (21%) compared to the DE WFT (8%) (p < 0.001). For all thrips landing on yellow, the interaction between the laboratory of origin and rearing regime was not significant (p = 0.803), reflecting the similar landing patterns observed for the three rearing regimes for the two countries of origin. However, in contrast to analysis a, the percentage of landings on blue did not vary significantly between the laboratory of origin, with 11% of the NL thrips and 15% of the DE thrips (p = 0.124).
Similarly, as for analysis a, by the host rearing regime irrespective of the laboratory of origin, the average percentage of WFT that landed on yellow was lowest for the thrips reared on bean, (10%) but increased to 15% when reared on chrysanthemum after 4–8 weeks and to 18% after 12–14 weeks (p = 0.021). In contrast, for landings on blue, a trend with the host rearing regime was not observed in analysis b or in analysis a, with 15% when reared on bean, 10% on chrysanthemum for 4-8 weeks, and 14% on chrysanthemum for 12–14 weeks (p = 0.142; p = 0.178 for the interaction).
For those attracted to the UV-A light from the ceiling, the percentage varied with both laboratory of origin and host rearing regime (p < 0.001 for the interaction). The mean percentage of NL WFT that landed on the ceiling varied substantially between rearing regimes with only 3.9% from those on bean (NLb) compared to 26.5% and 17.2% from chrysanthemum (NLbc4-8 and NLbc12-14, respectively). For the DE WFT, there was no such host correlation with the mean percentage landing on the ceiling, with those from bean (DEb) at 23.4% and those from chrysanthemum (DEbc4-8 and DEbc12-14) at 23.9% and 22.6%, respectively.
For the thrips considered to be elsewhere in the wind tunnel, the number varied little between the laboratory of origin (p = 0.523) but was significantly different amongst the host rearing regimes (p = 0.004 for the main effect, p = 0.151 for the interaction between the laboratory and rearing regime). More of those reared on bean (NLb and DEb) were elsewhere (mean of 62.5%) compared to the two chrysanthemum regimes of 50% (NLbc4-8 and DEbc4-8) and 48% (NLbc12-14 and DEbc12-14) (Figure 7).
For the thrips considered to be elsewhere in the wind tunnel, the number varied little between the laboratory of origin (p = 0.523) but was significantly different amongst the host rearing regimes (p = 0.004 for the main effect, p = 0.151 for the interaction between the laboratory and rearing regime). More of those reared on bean (NLb and DEb) were elsewhere (mean of 62.5%) compared to the two chrysanthemum regimes of 50% (NLbc4-8 and DEbc4-8) and 48% (NLbc12-14 and DEbc12-14) (Figure 7).

4. Discussion

Observing the behaviour of the two different WFT populations in the same laboratory and under the same conditions allowed us to examine whether within-species discrepancy in colour preference, recorded amongst separate studies [15,20,35], was purely a function of methodology. The data presented in our study did in fact indicate that methodology alone could not account for the consistently different responses, in particular to blue and yellow, as each population maintained its original colour preference when evaluated under the same conditions. Specifically, with the exception of NL thrips reared on beans (NLb), the NL population was on average more attracted to yellow compared to blue, in either a single-choice situation (Experiment i), or with a two-choice scenario (Experiment iii), as also observed in prior bioassays using coloured sticky plates by van Tol et al. [35]. On the other hand, the DE population had a stronger preference for blue over yellow (Experiments ii and iii), as reported in bioassays using LEDs with thrips taken from the source laboratory by Otieno et al. [15] and Stukenberg et al. [20].
Our results demonstrate for the first time that two different populations of WFT accessed from independent laboratories showed a different colour preference, even when evaluated under the same experimental setup. However, beyond evaluating the use of a standardised setup, the manipulation of rearing regime indicated that the host plant species could influence this yellow/blue attraction (Experiment iii). Factors such as genetics and previous experience might therefore have impacted on the response of the populations to colour, both of which are known to influence the behaviour of insects towards sensory stimuli in general [29,30], but little attention has been paid to these aspects in the case of colour, especially in small insects such as thrips.
To account for any potentially species-level genetic difference, using COI barcoding [36,43,44], our molecular data indicated that the NL and DE populations were both F. occidentalis. The divergence between them at 1.18% was consistent with intraspecific variation in WFT reported elsewhere typically as 2–3% [43,45], while inter-species divergences of closely related species of the genus Frankliniella and other thrips genera were generally around 10–21% [43,45,46], although it can be as low as 4.4% for cryptic species like some of those found in the genus Pseudophilothrips [46]. Caution about species status was also driven by the evidence building for F. occidentalis as a cryptic species complex, revealing clades for “glasshouse” and “lupin” through COI barcoding [36,45], which exhibit reproductive and developmental differences and cause varying levels of crop damage [47]. Sub-specific genetic differences, which may or may not have resulted in the observed behavioural responses of these two populations, is nonetheless possible. The colonies evaluated in this study likely originated from a different gene pool and have been kept geographically isolated from each other for more than two decades. The populations could therefore continue to adapt to their local rearing conditions and diverge independently. Much more rapidly evolving markers would need to be employed to explore this further. For example, restricted gene flow between populations has been proposed as the reason for genetic differences among geographically distinct WFT populations [48,49]. Laboratory “strains” of WFT through physical and temporal isolation have been suggested to develop and present differences in the feeding damage and reproductive success compared to wild WFT “greenhouse” strain populations from around The Netherlands within the short time frame of ~100 generations [49]. However, intra-specific genetic differences based on microsatellite data have been associated with extrinsic habitat-related factors such as temperature and precipitation, and not necessarily geographic distance or origin [50]. In the current study, if any genetic differences do exist between the two populations, they could have arisen as a result of laboratory adaptation after more than two decades (>150 generations) of being reared in laboratory conditions, or host-related epigenetic inheritance, or even by chance, as has been proposed for behavioural intraspecific variation observed in other insects [30,51].
For the aspect of our study that considered differences between populations and the host plant (Experiment iii), we also took the opportunity to present two analyses of the same data. First, to make the data more comparable with the common practice elsewhere of not presenting the data of insects that did not respond to the stimuli in choice studies in a flight cage [14,15,16,20,35], analysis a delimited evaluation to only the proportion of WFT that landed on the blue or yellow side of the sticky glass plate. This confirmed the general preference difference established between colonies and that the tendency for yellow was improved with rearing on yellow chrysanthemum; it did not indicate whether blue just became less attractive in the presence of yellow after rearing on chrysanthemum. However, in analysis b, observing all WFT that flew and left the release container in the wind tunnel showed that the proportion landing on blue remained similar, regardless of the laboratory of origin or rearing regime. The results instead suggest that chrysanthemum increased responsiveness to yellow in both populations, drawing in thrips from those that were otherwise elsewhere in the wind tunnel as opposed to reducing the attractiveness of blue or UV-A per se. This was not possible to be concluded from analysis a. One other potential explanation for the increased response to yellow from both populations is that it was a consequence of the unintended introduction of wild WFT from the organically grown chrysanthemum brought to the laboratory for the rearing. However, this has been discounted as being extremely unlikely. Not only is there is no evidence to suggest that wild populations of WFT from The Netherlands have a unique relative preference of yellow over blue [35], but any thrips present were undetectable through standard inspection of the flowers on arrival; at best, the numbers would have been too low to have genetically impacted on the colonies over the short number of generations (~3–4) that thrips were put on chrysanthemum for this experiment.
The increased response to yellow observed in Experiment iii by the varying rearing regimes suggests an influence of previous host experience [52]. Colour is thought to play an important role in host-searching behaviour in thrips as well as other groups of insects [53,54,55,56,57,58]. Associative learning is also common in insect species that strongly depend on flowers for food [59,60,61,62,63,64]. Even though WFT is highly polyphagous [65], the presence of flowers [66] and flower colour are important [58] for their ecology and reproductive success. Additionally, WFT can show a preference and perform differently on certain types of flowering plants or specific plant species [49,67,68] including those that have yellow flowers [69] or that provide pollen [70]. Therefore, for both populations, the increased attraction to yellow after the shift from beans to yellow-flowered chrysanthemum could potentially be explained as WFT perhaps being able to associate colour with food reward. Certainly, yellow chrysanthemum flowers are highly suitable as a food source for thrips [71,72,73,74,75], probably due to pollen being an important supplement to their diet for fitness and reproduction success [55,70,76,77,78,79,80], and which is lacking with the vegetative bean pods.
The thrips species Frankliniella schulzei has been inferred to make this connection between colour and host by adult females showing a strong wavelength-specific response to red [81], the same colour as the flowers of their postulated ‘primary host’ plant Malvaviscus arboreus [81,82]. In other insect orders, different populations of the same species can also exhibit affinities for a different colour such as the Sardinian Island population of Bombus terrestris sassaricus, which showed a preference for artificial red flowers in choice tests, compared to the mainland populations of B. terrestris that only showed a preference towards violet and the blue range of the light spectrum [51,83].
Associative learning has not been studied in Thysanoptera, but it has been found in a variety of orders of insects where it has been proposed that any animal with a nervous system has the potential to learn [84]. Extremely small insects with very small brains such as the featherwing beetle (Nephanes titan) (Coleoptera: Ptiilidae) have been found to learn to associate colour with food [85], showing that brain size is not an impediment for learning [86]. However, whether species of Thysanoptera are capable of shifting their colour preference based on the colour of host plant species remains a hypothesis for further study. Both blue and yellow are relevant colours for WFT and wavelengths in the blue and yellow range of the light spectrum are both found in flowers in nature, but whether the resulting differences in preference of these two colours are a result of adaptation to environment, selection pressure, learning, and experience or mere chance is not yet understood.
Although the focus of this study was to establish the population response to yellow and blue, on average, only around 20–35% of WFT released into the wind tunnel across all experiments landed on the colour stimuli. As shown in analysis b of Experiment iii, the majority of thrips did not respond to the light colours coming from the LED lamp and were captured either on ‘UV-A ceiling’ or found ‘elsewhere’. The low number of responsive thrips could have been partly caused by the large size of the wind tunnel used. This would be consistent with the higher percentages of thrips attracted to light stimuli recorded in other studies that used much smaller apparatus [15,20], some using extremely confined space to evaluate thrips phototaxis [87]. Nonetheless, results with similar proportions of WFT landing on visual cues to those presented in this study have been found in comparable wind tunnel bioassays evaluating the attraction of WFT to colour and odour stimuli [22,31,35]. In our study, of those that did not land on the sticky glass plate but were “elsewhere” in the wind tunnel, a greater proportion had been reared on bean compared to chrysanthemum, although it is not clear why. Additionally, the UV-A ceiling light elicited a relatively strong response similar to that of the main blue and yellow cues. The exception was the low proportion of thrips from NLb that were attracted to the UV-A source, with no clear explanation as to why. From the preliminary experiments conducted with the UV-light off (unpublished data) in the same facilities and with the same setup, the percentage of NLc WFT attracted to the LED lights remained similar while the numbers of WFT found on the ceiling dropped considerably. Thus, it is thought that the response of WFT flying towards the ceiling was mainly caused by the presence of UV-A light and not by the white light illuminating the wind tunnel. UV-light is known to be an important cue in aiding invertebrates in navigation, orientation [88], and the dispersion of pest insects [89,90,91] including thrips [92]. It also elicits phototaxis in insects [93,94] and is thought to be related to wavelength-specific behaviours [88,93,94] such as migration or escape response [94]. Potentially, therefore, the proportion of WFT attracted to UV-A light in our experiments could have been in a different physiological state to those attracted to blue or yellow, whose response instead may have been elicited by hunger-related physiology, leading to host-seeking behaviour.

5. Conclusions

The most important finding here is that with consistent experimental methods, blue and yellow attraction was confirmed for the first time to vary significantly between different populations of WFT. This suggests the presence of underlying biological or environmental factors, which complicates the extrapolation and generalisation of laboratory data on the response of WFT to colour, especially to improve trapping in the field. Additionally, despite similar long-term maintenance on bean for both base colonies used here, recent host plant rearing on yellow chrysanthemum was shown to influence their relative colour preference. The mechanisms responsible for the differences found between the NL and DE evaluated in this study remain unclear and warrant further investigation. However, to understand whether either blue or yellow are inherent colour preferences of WFT, studies with strictly naïve WFT would be needed. Innate preferences towards wavelengths in the blue spectrum have been reported for other insects that have a strong association with flowers, but this may depend on the background colour or intensity in some cases [95,96,97,98,99,100,101]. Likewise, in other herbivorous insects, colour cues in the green-yellow part of the light spectrum can elicit strong behavioural responses that are possibly an innate preference and known as ‘wavelength specific behaviours’ [56,91,102,103]. In any event, the extrapolation of studies conducted on colour with established laboratory colonies for application for WFT trapping in the field needs to be carefully considered. Potentially, there are strains or biotypes of WFT that naturally vary in their attraction to certain colours, so that no one colour will be the most effective to help maximise trap catches. Therefore, the behavioural responses of local wild WFT populations should be considered for trapping and/or monitoring.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/article/10.3390/insects13060538/s1, Figure S1: Light spectrum of ceiling illumination inside the wind tunnel. The UV-A spectrum (365–390 nm) represents 2% of the amount of light found in the visible light spectrum (400–750 nm). Figure S2: Light spectra of the LEDs inside the wind tunnel used for Experiments i, ii, and iii. Figure S3: Reflectance spectra of chrysanthemum flower and bean pods used for the rearing of WFT. Figure S4: Whole eye electroretinogram (ERG) spectral sensitivity curve of western flower thrips (WFT) (Frankliniella occidentalis) adapted from Otani et al. [104]. Figure S5: COI barcode neighbour-joining tree of sequences generated here for the F. occidentalis Dutch (Fo-NL1-3) and German (Fo-DE1-3) colonies, New Zealand “glasshouse” (Fo-NZ1-3) and “lupin” strains (Fo-NZ-L1-3), plus New Zealand F. intonsa (Fi-NZ1-3) and Thrips tabaci (Tt-NZ1-3), together with additional independent Genbank data (Supplementary Table S3). Posterior probabilities above 50% are illustrated. * Reference sequences “GBA8711-12|Frankliniella occidentalis” and “USTHY113-10|Frankliniella intonsa” are considered to be a result of Long Branch Attraction (LBA). ** Reference sequence “GBMHT2007-19|Frankliniella borinquen” is likely to have been misidentified Virgilio et al. [105] in the database based on its position inside the F. occidentalis clade and it is not likely to be a result of LBA. Table S1: Specifications of LEDs used (high power 1 Watt) for Experiments i and ii. Table S2: General information of the 18 specimens used to generate the DNA barcode sequences of the thrips used to support species identification and phylogenetic analysis of the Dutch (NL) and German (DE) Frankliniella occidentalis colonies used in this study. Table S3: Reference sequences of Frankliniella occidentalis, F. intonsa, F. borinquen, and Thrips tabaci downloaded from the BOLD database for phylogenetic analysis. Table S4: Results of the species identification queries to the BOLD database for the 18 new sequences developed here. BOLD Identification System (IDS) (https://www.boldsystems.org/; accessed on 5 March 2021). Table S5: Mean ± SE estimates of the intra- and interspecific genetic distances of thrips COI gene sequences included in the phylogenetic analysis. Distances were calculated using the Kimura 2-parameter model with 1000 bootstrap replications. Intraspecific distances of each group are shown in bold. Table S6: Summary of the results from Experiment i: mean percentage of the Dutch WFT (NLc) caught on the sticky plate of the LED lamp at seven different wavelengths as single choice treatments six hours after release (95% confidence limits). Table S7: Summary of the results from Experiment ii. Mean percentage of German WFT(DEc) attracted to blue and yellow light six hours after release. Comparative data from Supplementary Table S6 are included for Dutch WFT (NLc). Table S8: Summary of the results of Experiment iii, analysis a. Percentage of the different host reared WFT colonies on either yellow or blue light of only those that landed on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp. Table S9: Summary of results for Experiment iii, analysis b. The distribution of different host reared WFT colonies at the four locations inside the wind tunnel six hours after release.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, K.L.-R., M.R. and R.W.H.M.v.T.; Methodology and investigation, M.R., K.L.-R. and R.W.H.M.v.T.; Formal statistical analysis, R.C.B., M.R. and C.v.D.; Resources, R.W.H.M.v.T. and K.F.A.; Writing—original draft preparation, K.L.-R.; Writing—review and editing, K.L.-R., K.F.A., R.W.H.M.v.T., D.A.J.T., R.C.B., C.v.D. and M.R.; Supervision, K.F.A., R.W.H.M.v.T. and D.A.J.T.; Project administration, R.W.H.M.v.T. and K.F.A.; Funding acquisition, R.W.H.M.v.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

K. L.R.’s scholarship and research were jointly funded through the Ministry of Agriculture (Topsector Tuinbouw and Uitgangsmaterialen, program number KV 1605-075) [with co-finance from Koppert Biological Systems, LTO Glastuinbouw Nederland, and Stichting Programmafonds Glastuinbouw] (The Netherlands), and the Bio-Protection Research Centre, Lincoln University (Lincoln, New Zealand). D.A.J.T was financially supported by Plant and Food Research’s contribution through the B3 Better Border Biosecurity (https://www.b3nz.org.nz/ accessed on 7 June 2022) research collaboration. This work was in part supported by a grant from the Royal Society of New Zealand (Catalyst: Seeding Contract CSG-PAF1704) (Fundamental to Applied Research for Insect Sensing (FARIS) Network).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We thank Gerrie Wiegers and Jolanda Tom for help with the setup and rearing of the WFT colonies. Our special thanks go to Rainer Meyhöfer and the Institute of Horticultural Production Systems—Phytomedicin (Leibniz University Hannover) for kindly supplying the samples of German (DE) WFT from their colony and Timo Michel for organising shipments of the samples received. Thanks to Kentaro Arikawa for sharing the raw ERG data of WFT used in the Supplementary Materials. We also thank Mette Nielsen, Plant and Food Research Ltd., NZ, for the supply of other thrips populations and species for the DNA analysis. We thank the Travis Glare Bio-Protection Research Centre, Lincoln University as part of the supervisory team for K. L-R for their comments on the wider research project. We also thank Michael Rostas, Göttingen University, Germany for his contributions during the initial stages of this project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Gao, Y.-L.; Lei, Z.; Reitz, S.R. Western Flower Thrips Resistance to Insecticides: Detection, Mechanisms and Management Strategies. Pest Manag. Sci. 2012, 68, 1111–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Jensen, S.E. Insecticide Resistance in the Western Flower Thrips, Frankliniella occidentalis. Integr. Pest Manag. Rev. 2000, 5, 131–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Li, D.; Shang, X.; Reitz, S.R.; Nauen, R.; Lei, Z.; Lee, S.H.; Gao, Y.-L. Field Resistance to Spinosad in Western Flower Thrips Frankliniella occidentalis (Thysanoptera: Thripidae). J. Integr. Agric. 2016, 15, 2803–2808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hillocks, R.J. Farming with Fewer Pesticides: EU Pesticide Review and Resulting Challenges for UK Agriculture. Crop Prot. 2012, 31, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Karabelas, A.J.; Plakas, K.V.; Solomou, E.S.; Drossou, V.; Sarigiannis, D.A. Impact of European Legislation on Marketed Pesticides—A View from the Standpoint of Health Impact Assessment Studies. Environ. Int. 2009, 35, 1096–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Lescourret, F. Toward a Reduced Use of Pesticides in European Farming Systems: An Introduction to the PURE Project. Crop Prot. 2017, 97, 7–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lamichhane, J.R. Pesticide Use and Risk Reduction in European Farming Systems with IPM: An Introduction to the Special Issue. Crop Prot. 2017, 97, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lamichhane, J.R.; Arendse, W.; Dachbrodt-Saaydeh, S.; Kudsk, P.; Roman, J.C.; van Bijsterveldt-Gels, J.E.M.; Wick, M.; Messéan, A. Challenges and Opportunities for Integrated Pest Management in Europe: A Telling Example of Minor Uses. Crop Prot. 2015, 74, 42–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Jacobson, R.J. Integrated Pest Management (IPM) in Glasshouses. In Thrips as Crop Pests; CAB International: Wallingford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 639–666. ISBN 0-85199-178-5. [Google Scholar]
  10. Lewis, T. Field and Laboratory Techniques. In Thrips as Crop Pests; CAB International: Wallingford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 435–476. [Google Scholar]
  11. Kirk, W.D.J.; de Kogel, W.J.; Koschier, E.H.; Teulon, D.A. Semiochemicals for Thrips and Their Use in Pest Management. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2021, 66, 101–119. [Google Scholar]
  12. van Tol, R.W.; Davidson, M.M.; Butler, R.C.; Teulon, D.A.; de Kogel, W.J. Visually and Olfactorily Enhanced Attractive Devices for Thrips Management. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2020, 168, 665–677. [Google Scholar]
  13. Johansen, N.S.; Torp, T.; Solhaug, K.A. Phototactic Response of Frankliniella occidentalis to Sticky Traps with Blue Light Emitting Diodes in Herb and Alstroemeria Greenhouses. Crop Prot. 2018, 114, 120–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Makabe, T.; Futamura, T.; Noudomi, T.; Wakakuwa, M.; Arikawa, K. Phototaxis of Western Flower Thrips, Frankliniella occidentalis and Onion Thrips, Thrips Tabaci and the Possibility of Controlling Thrips Using Ultraviolet-Emitting Trap in the Greenhouse of Satsuma Mandarin (Citrus Unshiu). Jpn. J. Appl. Entomol. Zool. 2014, 58, 187–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Otieno, J.A.; Stukenberg, N.; Weller, J.; Poehling, H.-M. Efficacy of LED-Enhanced Blue Sticky Traps Combined with the Synthetic Lure Lurem-TR for Trapping of Western Flower Thrips (Frankliniella occidentalis). J. Pest Sci. 2018, 91, 1301–1314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Yang, J.-Y.; Sung, B.-K.; Lee, H.-S. Phototactic Behavior 8: Phototactic Behavioral Responses of Western Flower Thrips, Frankliniella occidentalis Pergande (Thysanoptera: Thripidae), to Light-Emitting Diodes. J. Korean Soc. Appl. Biol. Chem. 2015, 58, 359–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Lewis, T. (Ed.) Thrips as Crop Pests; CAB International: Wallingford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1997; ISBN 0-85199-178-8. [Google Scholar]
  18. Brødsgaard, H.F. Coloured Sticky Traps for Frankliniella occidentalis (Pergande)(Thysanoptera, Thripidae) in Glasshouses. J. Appl. Entomol. 1989, 107, 136–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Matteson, N.A.; Terry, L.I. Response to Color by Male and Female Frankliniella occidentalis during Swarming and Non-swarming Behavior. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1992, 63, 187–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Stukenberg, N.; Pietruska, M.; Waldherr, A.; Meyhöfer, R. Wavelength-Specific Behavior of the Western Flower Thrips (Frankliniella occidentalis): Evidence for a Blue-Green Chromatic Mechanism. Insects 2020, 11, 423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Vernon, R.S.; Gillespie, D.R. Response of Frankliniella occidentalis (Thysanoptera: Thripidae) and Trialeurodes Vaporariorum (Homoptera: Aleyrodidae) to Fluorescent Traps in a Cucumber Greenhouse. J. Entomol. Soc. Br. Columbia 1990, 87, 38–41. [Google Scholar]
  22. Davidson, M.M.; Butler, R.C.; Teulon, D.A.J. Response of Female Frankliniella occidentalis (Pergande) to Visual Cues and Para-Anisaldehyde in a Flight Chamber. J. Insect Behav. 2012, 25, 297–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Prema, M.S.; Ganapathy, N.; Renukadevi, P.; Mohankumar, S.; Kennedy, J.S. Coloured Sticky Traps to Monitor Thrips Population in Cotton. J. Entomol. Zool. Stud. 2018, 6, 948–952. [Google Scholar]
  24. Ren, X.; Wu, S.; Xing, Z.; Xu, R.; Cai, W.; Lei, Z. Behavioral Responses of Western Flower Thrips (Frankliniella occidentalis) to Visual and Olfactory Cues at Short Distances. Insects 2020, 11, 177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Smits, P.H.; van Deventer, P.; De Kogel, W.J. Western Flower Thrips: Reactions to Odours and Colours. West. Flower Thrips React. Odours Colours 2000, 11, 175–180. [Google Scholar]
  26. Cruz-Esteban, S.; Rojas, J.C.; Hernández-Ledesma, P. Trap Colour and Aggregation Pheromone Dose Affect the Catch of Western Flower Thrips in Blackberry Crops. J. Appl. Entomol. 2020, 144, 755–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hoddle, M.S.; Robinson, L.; Morgan, D. Attraction of Thrips (Thysanoptera: Thripidae and Aeolothripidae) to Colored Sticky Cards in a California Avocado Orchard. Crop Prot. 2002, 21, 383–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Yudin, L.S.; Mitchell, W.C.; Cho, J.J. Color Preference of Thrips (Thysanoptera: Thripidae) with Reference to Aphids (Homoptera: Aphididae) and Leafminers in Hawaiian Lettuce Farms. J. Econ. Entomol. 1987, 80, 51–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Campan, R. Tactic Components in Orientation. EXS 1997, 84, 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Jaenike, J. Host Specialization in Phytophagous Insects. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1990, 21, 243–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Davidson, M.M.; Butler, R.C.; Teulon, D.A.J. Starvation Period and Age Affect the Response of Female Frankliniella occidentalis (Pergande)(Thysanoptera: Thripidae) to Odor and Visual Cues. J. Insect Physiol. 2006, 52, 729–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fatnassi, H.; Pizzol, J.; Senoussi, R.; Biondi, A.; Desneux, N.; Poncet, C.; Boulard, T. Within-Crop Air Temperature and Humidity Outcomes on Spatio-Temporal Distribution of the Key Rose Pest Frankliniella occidentalis. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0126655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Nielsen, M.-C. Factors Affecting the Response of Thrips to an Olfactory Cue. Ph.D. Dissertation, Lincoln University, Lincoln, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  34. Teulon, D.A.J.; Hollister, B.; Butler, R.C.; Cameron, E.A. Colour and Odour Responses of Flying Western Flower Thrips: Wind Tunnel and Greenhouse Experiments. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1999, 93, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Van Tol, R.W.; Tom, J.; Roher, M.; Schreurs, A.; van Dooremalen, C. Haze of Glue Determines Preference of Western Flower Thrips (Frankliniella occidentalis) for Yellow or Blue Traps. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 6557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Rugman-Jones, P.F.; Hoddle, M.S.; Stouthamer, R. Nuclear-Mitochondrial Barcoding Exposes the Global Pest Western Flower Thrips (Thysanoptera: Thripidae) as Two Sympatric Cryptic Species in Its Native California. J. Econ. Entomol. 2010, 103, 877–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. OEPP/EPPO. PM 7/129 (1) DNA Barcoding as an Identification Tool for a Number of Regulated Pests. EPPO Bull 2016, 46, 501–537. [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Folmer, O.; Black, M.; Hoeh, W.; Lutz, R.; Vrijenhoek, R. DNA Primers for Amplification of Mitochondrial Cytochrome c Oxidase Subunit I from Diverse Metazoan Invertebrates. Mol. Mar. Biol. Biotechnol. 1994, 3, 294–299. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kumar, S.; Stecher, G.; Li, M.; Knyaz, C.; Tamura, K. MEGA X: Molecular Evolutionary Genetics Analysis across Computing Platforms. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2018, 35, 1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kimura, M. A Simple Method for Estimating Evolutionary Rates of Base Substitutions through Comparative Studies of Nucleotide Sequences. J. Mol. Evol. 1980, 16, 111–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. McCullagh, P.; Nelder, J.A. Generalized Linear Models; Chapman and Hall: London, UK, 1989; ISBN 0-203-75373-9. [Google Scholar]
  42. Payne, R.; Murray, D.; Baird, D. The Guide to the Genstat Command Language (Release 19); VSN International: Hemel Hempsted, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kadirvel, P.; Srinivasan, R.; Hsu, Y.-C.; Su, F.-C.; De La Pena, R. Application of Cytochrome Oxidase I Sequences for Phylogenetic Analysis and Identification of Thrips Species Occurring on Vegetable Crops. J. Econ. Entomol. 2013, 106, 408–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Zhang, G.F.; Meng, X.Q.; Min, L.; Qiao, W.N.; Wan, F.H. Rapid Diagnosis of the Invasive Species, Frankliniella occidentalis (Pergande): A Species-specific COI Marker. J. Appl. Entomol. 2012, 136, 410–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Gunawardana, D.N.; Li, D.; Masumoto, M.; Mound, L.A.; O’Donnel, C.A.; Skarlinsky, T.L. Resolving the Confused Identity of Frankliniella Panamensis (Thysanoptera: Thripidae). Zootaxa 2017, 4323, 125–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Mound, L.A.; Wheeler, G.S.; Williams, D.A. Resolving Cryptic Species with Morphology and DNA; Thrips as a Potential Biocontrol Agent of Brazilian Peppertree, with a New Species and Overview of Pseudophilothrips (Thysanoptera). Zootaxa 2010, 2432, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Nielsen, M.-C.; Teulon, D.A.J.; Chapman, R.B.; Butler, R.C.; Drayton, G.M.; Philipsen, H. Comparison of Life History Parameters of Two Frankliniella occidentalis (Thysanoptera: Thripidae) Strains in New Zealand. Environ. Entomol. 2010, 39, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Fang, J.; Kritzman, A.; Yonash, N.; Gera, A.; Pollak, N.; Lavi, U. Genetic Variation of Thrips Populations Assessed by Amplified Fragment Length Polymorphism (Thysanoptera: Thripidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 2005, 98, 351–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mirnezhad, M.; Schidlo, N.; Klinkhamer, P.G.; Leiss, K.A. Variation in Genetics and Performance of Dutch Western Flower Thrips Populations. J. Econ. Entomol. 2012, 105, 1816–1824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Brunner, P.C.; Frey, J.E. Habitat-Specific Population Structure in Native Western Flower Thrips Frankliniella occidentalis (Insecta, Thysanoptera). J. Evol. Biol. 2010, 23, 797–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Chittka, L.; Ings, T.C.; Raine, N.E. Chance and Adaptation in the Evolution of Island Bumblebee Behaviour. Popul. Ecol. 2004, 46, 243–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Szentesi, Á.; Jermy, T. The Role of Experience in Host Plant Choice by Phytophagous Insects. In Insect-Plant Interactions; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1990; ISBN 978-0-203-71173-6. [Google Scholar]
  53. Döring, T.F. How Aphids Find Their Host Plants, and How They Don’t. Ann. Appl. Biol. 2014, 165, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Döring, T.F.; Chittka, L. Visual Ecology of Aphids—A Critical Review on the Role of Colours in Host Finding. Arthropod-Plant Interact. 2007, 1, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Kirk, W.D.J. Ecological Studies on Thrips Imaginis Bagnall (Thysanoptera) in Flowers of Echium Plantagineum L. in Australia. Aust. J. Ecol. 1984, 9, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Prokopy, R.J.; Owens, E.D. Visual Detection of Plants by Herbivorous Insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1983, 28, 337–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Reeves, J.L. Vision Should Not Be Overlooked as an Important Sensory Modality for Finding Host Plants. Environ. Entomol. 2011, 40, 855–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Rogge, S.A.; Meyhöfer, R. The Role of Plant Physiology and Cultivar of Chrysanthemum in the Resistance against Western Flower Thrips. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2021, 169, 275–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Banschbach, V.S. Colour Association Influences Honey Bee Choice between Sucrose Concentrations. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1994, 175, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Giurfa, M. Conditioning Procedure and Color Discrimination in the Honeybee Apis Mellifera. Naturwissenschaften 2004, 91, 228–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Muth, F.; Papaj, D.R.; Leonard, A.S. Bees Remember Flowers for More than One Reason: Pollen Mediates Associative Learning. Anim. Behav. 2016, 111, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Nicholls, E.; Hempel de Ibarra, N. Bees Associate Colour Cues with Differences in Pollen Rewards. J. Exp. Biol. 2014, 217, 2783–2788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Nicholls, E.; Hempel de Ibarra, N. Assessment of Pollen Rewards by Foraging Bees. Funct. Ecol. 2017, 31, 76–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Wilmsen, S.; Gottlieb, R.; Junker, R.R.; Lunau, K. Bumblebees Require Visual Pollen Stimuli to Initiate and Multimodal Stimuli to Complete a Full Behavioral Sequence in Close-range Flower Orientation. Ecol. Evol. 2017, 7, 1384–1393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Tommasini, M.G.; Maini, S. Frankliniella Occidentalis and Other Thrips Harmful to Vegetable and Ornamental Crops in Europe; Landbouwuniversiteit Wageningen (Wageningen Agricultural University): Wageningen, The Netherlands, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  66. Gerin, C.; Hance, T.H.; Van Impe, G. Impact of Flowers on the Demography of Western Flower Thrips Frankliniella occidentalis (Thysan., Thripidae). J. Appl. Entomol. 1999, 123, 569–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Mound, L.A. Thysanoptera: Diversity and Interactions. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2005, 50, 247–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Pearsall, I.A. Flower Preference Behaviour of Western Flower Thrips in the Similkameen Valley, British Columbia, Canada. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2000, 95, 303–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Blumthal, M.R.; Spomer, L.A.; Warnock, D.F.; Cloyd, R.A. Flower Color Preferences of Western Flower Thrips. HortTechnology 2005, 15, 846–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Kirk, W.D.J. Feeding. In Thrips as Crop Pests; CAB International: Wallingford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 119–174. [Google Scholar]
  71. Broadbent, A.B.; Matteoni, J.A.; Allen, W.R. Feeding Preferences of the Western Flower Thrips, Frankliniella occidentalis (Pergande)(Thysanoptera: Thripidae), and Incidence of Tomato Spotted Wilt Virus among Cultivars of Florist’s Chrysanthemum. Can. Entomol. 1990, 122, 1111–1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. De Jager, C.M.; Butôt, R.P.T.; De Jong, T.J.; Klinkhamer, P.G.L.; Van Der Meijden, E. Population Growth and Survival of Western Flower Thrips Frankliniella occidentalis Pergande (Thysanoptera, Thripidae) on Different Chrysanthemum Cultivars: Two Methods for Measuring Resistance. J. Appl. Entomol. 1993, 115, 519–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. De Jager, C.M.; Butôt, R.P.T.; Klinkhamer, P.G.L.; De Jong, T.J.; Wolff, K.; Van Der Meijden, E. Genetic Variation in Chrysanthemum for Resistance to Frankliniella occidentalis. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1995, 77, 277–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Kirk, W.D.J. Distribution, Abundance and Population Dynamics. In Thrips as Crop Pests; CAB International: Wallingford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 217–257. [Google Scholar]
  75. Lewis, T. Appendix II: Major Crops Infested by Thrips with Main Symptoms and Predominant Injurious Species. In Thrips as Crop Pests; CAB International: Wallingford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 675–709. [Google Scholar]
  76. Brodbeck, B.V.; Funderburk, J.; Stavisky, J.; Andersen, P.C.; Hulshof, J.; Marullo, R.; Mound, L.A. Recent Advances in the Nutritional Ecology of Thysanoptera, or the Lack Thereof. In Proceedings of the 7th International Symposium on Thysanoptera; Australian National Insect Collection: Canberra, Australia, 2002; Volume 7, pp. 145–153. [Google Scholar]
  77. Hulshof, J.; Vanninen, I. Western Flower Thrips Feeding on Pollen, and Its Implications for Control. In Proceedings of the 7th International Symposium on Thysanoptera; Australian National Insect Collection: Canberra, Australia, 2002; Volume 7, pp. 173–179. [Google Scholar]
  78. Kirk, W.D.J. Aggregation and Mating of Thrips in Flowers of Calystegia Sepium. Ecol. Entomol. 1985, 10, 433–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Kirk, W.D.J. Pollen-feeding and the Host Specificity and Fecundity of Flower Thrips (Thysanoptera). Ecol. Entomol. 1985, 10, 281–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Leskey, T.C.; Teulon, D.A.; Cameron, E.A. Effects of Temperature and Sugar Maple Pollen on Oviposition and Longevity of Pear Thrips (Thysanoptera: Thripidae). Environ. Entomol. 1997, 26, 566–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Yaku, A.; Walter, G.H.; Najar-Rodriguez, A.J. Thrips See Red–Flower Colour and the Host Relationships of a Polyphagous Anthophilic Thrips. Ecol. Entomol. 2007, 32, 527–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Milne, M.; Walter, G.H. Feeding and Breeding across Host Plants within a Locality by the Widespread Thrips Frankliniella Schultzei, and the Invasive Potential of Polyphagous Herbivores. Divers. Distrib. 2000, 6, 243–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Skorupski, P.; Döring, T.F.; Chittka, L. Photoreceptor Spectral Sensitivity in Island and Mainland Populations of the Bumblebee, Bombus Terrestris. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2007, 193, 485–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Hollis, K.L.; Guillette, L.M. What Associative Learning in Insects Tells Us about the Evolution of Learned and Fixed Behavior. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2015, 28, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Polilov, A.A.; Makarova, A.A.; Kolesnikova, U.K. Cognitive Abilities with a Tiny Brain: Neuronal Structures and Associative Learning in the Minute Nephanes Titan (Coleoptera: Ptiliidae). Arthropod Struct. Dev. 2019, 48, 98–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Hollis, K.L.; Guillette, L.M. Associative Learning in Insects: Evolutionary Models, Mushroom Bodies, and a Neuroscientific Conundrum. Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev. 2011, 6, 24–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. Egri, Á.; Farkas, P.; Bernáth, B.; Guerin, P.M.; Fail, J. Spectral sensitivity of L2 biotype in the Thrips tabaci cryptic species complex. J. Insect Physiol. 2020, 121, 103999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Cronin, T.W.; Bok, M.J. Photoreception and Vision in the Ultraviolet. J. Exp. Biol. 2016, 219, 2790–2801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Dáder, B.; Moreno, A.; Gwynn-Jones, D.; Winters, A.; Fereres, A. Aphid Orientation and Performance in Glasshouses under Different UV-A/UV-B Radiation Regimes. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2017, 163, 344–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Doukas, D.; Payne, C.C. Greenhouse Whitefly (Homoptera: Aleyrodidae) Dispersal under Different UV-Light Environments. J. Econ. Entomol. 2007, 100, 389–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Stukenberg, N.; Gebauer, K.; Poehling, H.-M. Light Emitting Diode (LED)-based Trapping of the Greenhouse Whitefly (Trialeurodes Vaporariorum). J. Appl. Entomol. 2015, 139, 268–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Nguyen, T.H.N.; Borgemeister, C.; Max, J.; Poehling, H.-M. Manipulation of Ultraviolet Light Affects Immigration Behavior of Ceratothripoides Claratris (Thysanoptera: Thripidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 2009, 102, 1559–1566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Goldsmith, T.H. Ultraviolet Receptors and Color Vision: Evolutionary Implications and a Dissonance of Paradigms. Vision Res. 1994, 34, 1479–1487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Menzel, R. Spectral Sensitivity and Color Vision in Invertebrates. In Comparative Physiology and Evolution of Vision in Invertebrates; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1979; pp. 503–580. [Google Scholar]
  95. Gumbert, A. Color Choices by Bumble Bees (Bombus Terrestris): Innate Preferences and Generalization after Learning. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2000, 48, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Kelber, A. Innate Preferences for Flower Features in the Hawkmoth Macroglossum Stellatarum. J. Exp. Biol. 1997, 200, 827–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Kuenzinger, W.; Kelber, A.; Weesner, J.; Travis, J.; Raguso, R.A.; Goyret, J. Innate Colour Preferences of a Hawkmoth Depend on Visual Context. Biol. Lett. 2019, 15, 20180886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  98. Lunau, K. Colour Saturation Triggers Innate Reactions to Flower Signals: Flower Dummy Experiments with Bumblebees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1990, 166, 827–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Ostroverkhova, O.; Galindo, G.; Lande, C.; Kirby, J.; Scherr, M.; Hoffman, G.; Rao, S. Understanding Innate Preferences of Wild Bee Species: Responses to Wavelength-Dependent Selective Excitation of Blue and Green Photoreceptor Types. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2018, 204, 667–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Satoh, A.; Kinoshita, M.; Arikawa, K. Innate Preference and Learning of Colour in the Male Cotton Bollworm Moth, Helicoverpa Armigera. J. Exp. Biol. 2016, 219, 3857–3860. [Google Scholar]
  101. Yoshida, M.; Itoh, Y.; Ômura, H.; Arikawa, K.; Kinoshita, M. Plant Scents Modify Innate Colour Preference in Foraging Swallowtail Butterflies. Biol. Lett. 2015, 11, 20150390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Stukenberg, N.; Poehling, H.-M. Blue–Green Opponency and Trichromatic Vision in the Greenhouse Whitefly (Trialeurodes Vaporariorum) Explored Using Light Emitting Diodes. Ann. Appl. Biol. 2019, 175, 146–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Farnier, K.; Dyer, A.G.; Steinbauer, M.J. Related but Not Alike: Not All Hemiptera are Attracted to Yellow. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2014, 2, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Otani, Y.; Wakakuwa, M.; Arikawa, K. Relationship between Action Spectrum and Spectral Sensitivity of Compound Eyes Relating Phototactic Behavior of the Western Flower Thrips, Frankliniella occidentalis. Jpn. J. Appl. Entomol. Zool. 2014, 58, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Virgilio, M.; Backeljau, T.; Nevado, B.; De Meyer, M. Comparative Performances of DNA Barcoding across Insect Orders. BMC Bioinform. 2010, 11, 206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Diagram showing the derivations of WFT colonies from Wageningen, The Netherlands (NL) and Hannover, Germany (DE) from the original base colonies and used in Experiments i–iii.
Figure 1. Diagram showing the derivations of WFT colonies from Wageningen, The Netherlands (NL) and Hannover, Germany (DE) from the original base colonies and used in Experiments i–iii.
Insects 13 00538 g001
Figure 2. The wind tunnel setup used for the WFT behavioural experiments viewed from different perspectives illustrating: (A) The complete equipment arrangement; (B) the yellow stimulus, release container and wind direction; (C) the release container in relation to the LED stimulus (blue). Red arrow, LED lamp (dome); green arrow, insect release platform; yellow arrow, insect release container; blue arrows, wind direction inside the wind tunnel; black arrow, general ceiling illumination provided for the wind tunnel (includes wavelengths in the visible light spectrum and UV-A light).
Figure 2. The wind tunnel setup used for the WFT behavioural experiments viewed from different perspectives illustrating: (A) The complete equipment arrangement; (B) the yellow stimulus, release container and wind direction; (C) the release container in relation to the LED stimulus (blue). Red arrow, LED lamp (dome); green arrow, insect release platform; yellow arrow, insect release container; blue arrows, wind direction inside the wind tunnel; black arrow, general ceiling illumination provided for the wind tunnel (includes wavelengths in the visible light spectrum and UV-A light).
Insects 13 00538 g002
Figure 3. Aspects of the 3D printed dome housing the LEDs. (A) Frontal view showing the interior covered with aluminium foil, red arrow shows the division inside the dome. (B) Frontal view with the frame attached. (C) LED lamp with the diffusing glass plate and transparent propylene sheet with sticky glue. (D) Removable frame where the transparent sticky plastic was placed for catching the WFT. (E) Back side of the dome showing the four visible orifices where LEDs were placed. (F) LED lamp inside the wind tunnel shining a yellow and blue LED from inside the dome.
Figure 3. Aspects of the 3D printed dome housing the LEDs. (A) Frontal view showing the interior covered with aluminium foil, red arrow shows the division inside the dome. (B) Frontal view with the frame attached. (C) LED lamp with the diffusing glass plate and transparent propylene sheet with sticky glue. (D) Removable frame where the transparent sticky plastic was placed for catching the WFT. (E) Back side of the dome showing the four visible orifices where LEDs were placed. (F) LED lamp inside the wind tunnel shining a yellow and blue LED from inside the dome.
Insects 13 00538 g003
Figure 4. Experiment i: Mean percentage of Dutch WFT (NLc) caught on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp with different colours six hours after release. N = 4 replicates. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Peak wavelength of LEDs used: Pink (UV-A 369 nm), Violet (422 nm), Blue (477 nm), Cyan (502 nm), Green (529 nm), Yellow (588 nm), Red (651 nm). Each LED was presented as a single choice. All information from this figure is provided in Supplementary Table S6.
Figure 4. Experiment i: Mean percentage of Dutch WFT (NLc) caught on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp with different colours six hours after release. N = 4 replicates. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Peak wavelength of LEDs used: Pink (UV-A 369 nm), Violet (422 nm), Blue (477 nm), Cyan (502 nm), Green (529 nm), Yellow (588 nm), Red (651 nm). Each LED was presented as a single choice. All information from this figure is provided in Supplementary Table S6.
Insects 13 00538 g004
Figure 5. Experiment ii: Mean percentage of German WFT (DEc) caught on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp with blue or yellow light six hours after the release of the insects. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Peak wavelength of LEDs: Blue (477 nm), Yellow (588 nm). Each LED was presented as a single choice. All information from this figure is provided in Supplementary Table S7.
Figure 5. Experiment ii: Mean percentage of German WFT (DEc) caught on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp with blue or yellow light six hours after the release of the insects. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Peak wavelength of LEDs: Blue (477 nm), Yellow (588 nm). Each LED was presented as a single choice. All information from this figure is provided in Supplementary Table S7.
Insects 13 00538 g005
Figure 6. Experiment iii, analysis a: Stacked bar chart showing the percentages of WFT found on the yellow or blue light per laboratory of origin and rearing regime out of the total number of thrips that landed on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp. Peak wavelength of LEDs: Blue (477 nm), Yellow (588 nm). DEb: colony of German WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; DEbc 4-8: DEb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; DEbc 12-14: continuation of colony DEbc 4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. NLb: colony of Dutch WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; NLbc 4-8: NLb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; NLbc 12-14: continuation of colony NLbc4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. All information from this figure including mean percentages and 95% confidence intervals is provided in Supplementary Table S8.
Figure 6. Experiment iii, analysis a: Stacked bar chart showing the percentages of WFT found on the yellow or blue light per laboratory of origin and rearing regime out of the total number of thrips that landed on the sticky glass plate of the LED lamp. Peak wavelength of LEDs: Blue (477 nm), Yellow (588 nm). DEb: colony of German WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; DEbc 4-8: DEb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; DEbc 12-14: continuation of colony DEbc 4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. NLb: colony of Dutch WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; NLbc 4-8: NLb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; NLbc 12-14: continuation of colony NLbc4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. All information from this figure including mean percentages and 95% confidence intervals is provided in Supplementary Table S8.
Insects 13 00538 g006
Figure 7. Experiment iii, analysis b: Stacked bar chart showing the percentages of WFT that left the release container as per the laboratory of origin and rearing regime for each of the four categories in which the WFT was grouped inside the wind tunnel. Peak wavelength of LEDs: Blue (477 nm), Yellow (588 nm). DEb: colony of German WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; DEbc 4-8: DEb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; DEbc 12-14: continuation of colony DEbc 4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. NLb: colony of Dutch WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; NLbc 4-8: NLb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; NLbc 12-14: continuation of colony NLbc4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. All information from this figure including mean percentages and 95% confidence intervals is provided in Supplementary Table S9.
Figure 7. Experiment iii, analysis b: Stacked bar chart showing the percentages of WFT that left the release container as per the laboratory of origin and rearing regime for each of the four categories in which the WFT was grouped inside the wind tunnel. Peak wavelength of LEDs: Blue (477 nm), Yellow (588 nm). DEb: colony of German WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; DEbc 4-8: DEb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; DEbc 12-14: continuation of colony DEbc 4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. NLb: colony of Dutch WFT reared on bean pods for ~20 weeks; NLbc 4-8: NLb colony transferred and reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants for 4 to 8 weeks; NLbc 12-14: continuation of colony NLbc4-8 until reaching 12 to 14 weeks reared on yellow-flowered chrysanthemum plants. All information from this figure including mean percentages and 95% confidence intervals is provided in Supplementary Table S9.
Insects 13 00538 g007
Table 1. The list of LED treatments to test for the colour preference of Dutch (NLc) WFT.
Table 1. The list of LED treatments to test for the colour preference of Dutch (NLc) WFT.
TreatmentLED Colour aPeak Wavelength (nm)
1UV-A369
2Violet422
3Blue477
4Cyan502
5Green529
6Yellow588
7Red651
a The spectral radiance of 9.0 × 1017 photons·s−1·sr−1·m−2.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lopez-Reyes, K.; Armstrong, K.F.; Teulon, D.A.J.; Butler, R.C.; van Dooremalen, C.; Roher, M.; van Tol, R.W.H.M. Colour Response in Western Flower Thrips Varies Intraspecifically. Insects 2022, 13, 538. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/insects13060538

AMA Style

Lopez-Reyes K, Armstrong KF, Teulon DAJ, Butler RC, van Dooremalen C, Roher M, van Tol RWHM. Colour Response in Western Flower Thrips Varies Intraspecifically. Insects. 2022; 13(6):538. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/insects13060538

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lopez-Reyes, Karla, Karen F. Armstrong, David A. J. Teulon, Ruth C. Butler, Coby van Dooremalen, Monika Roher, and Robert W. H. M. van Tol. 2022. "Colour Response in Western Flower Thrips Varies Intraspecifically" Insects 13, no. 6: 538. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/insects13060538

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop