Next Article in Journal
Parental Mental Health Problems and the Risk of Child Maltreatment: The Potential Role of Psychotherapy
Next Article in Special Issue
The Impact of Civic and Religious Social Capital on the Antisocial Attitudes of the Youth: A Multi-Level Cross-National Study
Previous Article in Journal
Slumming on Social Media? E-Mediated Tourist Gaze and Social Representations of Indian, South African, and Brazilian Slum Tourism Destinations
Previous Article in Special Issue
Student Perception of the Social Value of Responsible Management
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Revised Version of the Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale and Its Relation to Psychosocial Functioning and Online Behavioral Problems

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Josip Juraj Strossmayer University of Osijek, 31000 Osijek, Croatia
Submission received: 31 July 2021 / Revised: 31 August 2021 / Accepted: 1 September 2021 / Published: 2 September 2021

Abstract

:
Cyber-violence is the type of online risk behavior inclined to harm others. Development of new forms of cyber-violent behavior leads to the need to revise specific-item measures of cyber-violence periodically. The aim of this research was to explore the psychometric properties of the revised Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale: its latent structure, reliability, and descriptive statistics of underlying dimensions, as well as the relation of some known correlates of cyber-violence, like indicators of psychosocial functioning and online behavioral problems, with cyber-violence. Online questionnaires (cyber-violence, depression, anxiety and stress, problematic Internet use, and problematic gaming) were filled out by 1725 adolescents from a convenient sample. Using exploratory factor analysis and hierarchical regression analysis, the questionnaire’s latent structure and contribution of relevant correlates for explaining cyber-violence variance was examined. Results: Exploratory factor analysis showed a five-factor solution with satisfactory reliability: shaming, information manipulation, hate speech, technology abuse, and information sharing. Participants commit and experience cyber-violence rarely, leading to a positive distribution of data in the factors. The Committing and Experiencing Cyber-violence subscales have a large positive correlation. Gender (male), grades, maternal education, depression, anxiety, stress, problematic Internet use, and problematic gaming are positive predictors of experiencing cyber-violence, whereas gender (male), grades, hours spent online on weekdays, depression, anxiety, stress, problematic Internet use, and problematic gaming are positive predictors of committing cyber-violence. Conclusions: Cyber-violence is connected with lower psychosocial functioning and more risky behavior online (problematic Internet use, problematic online gaming).

1. Introduction

During the COVID-19 pandemic, there were major changes in the lives of people of all ages. For many children, young people, and adults, work and education has moved from the physical to the digital world over time. Children and young people around the world have experienced lockdowns of varying magnitude. In Croatia, from 13 March 2020 to 11 May 2020 (primary schools) and 18 May 2020 (secondary school), in-person classes were stopped and students attended classes by watching a TV program and/or doing assignments that were sent by teachers or by attending classes on online platforms [1,2]. At that time, movement within the municipality of residence was restricted [3] and open-air children’s and sports playgrounds were fenced to prevent the gathering of children and young people [4]. All of this contributed to the increased amount of time in front of screens. This has raised concerns among experts about the consequences of both lockdown and the pandemic, as well as the impact on education and social development of children and young people [5]. Although these circumstances contribute to the reduction of traditional peer violence [6], an increase in domestic violence [7] or cyber-violence, at least for some adolescents [8], was found.

1.1. Cyber-Violence Definition and Measurement

Cyber-violence was recognized at the end of the 1990s as a type of online deviant behavior [9]. Later on, the terms “cyber-bullying” and “cyber-victimization” were mostly used. Cyber-bullying is defined as “willful and repeated harm inflicted through computers, cell phones, and other electronic devices” [10] (p. 11). The influences of Olweus’ definition of traditional bullying [11] are easily recognized in this definition. Patchin and Hinduja [12] stress the importance of using criteria for traditional bullying in defining cyber-bullying—repetition, harm, intention, and imbalance of power. They do recognize that repetition in the digital world could be somewhat different than in traditional bullying since a one-time cyber-attack can reach a large audience and be shared or found on the Internet later. This definition of intention to harm is very questionable. They stress that the victim can perceive particular behavior as intentional, but if the perpetrator did not want to cause harm, it cannot be seen as cyber-bullying. This view could be appropriate in the legal system, and since those two scholars are both experts in criminal justice, it directs them toward the perpetrator’s position. Other researchers [13] warn that this approach to defining cyber-bullying does not consider the position of the victim. Respecting the victim’s position and psycho-social perspective, as well as the possibilities that the material online is later shared or approached, authors decided [14] to use the term “cyber-violence” and not to insist on the perpetrator’s intention to harm and the repetition of behavior two or three times per month to consider something as harmful behavior. It should be noted that interactions with adolescents through a cyber-violence preventive program informed us that they often perceive some forms of cyber-violent behavior (e.g., making fun of someone) as a joke (if they are not in the position of the victim of that joke). On the other hand, in a qualitative study conducted by the author of this paper during 2020 to gain insight into adolescents’ perception of Internet and cyber-violence, and to revise the cyber-violence scale, adolescents found it very common to use jokes as a way to disguise humiliation. Accordingly, an item measuring that behavior was added to the scale. In addition, previous findings indicate that perpetrators and perpetrators/victims assess most violent behaviors as less severe than young people who are not involved in violence [15].
Two main approaches to measuring cyber-violence and cyber-bullying can be found in the literature: global-item and specific-item measures, and a combination. In a global-item measure, it usually describes what cyber-bullying is, and then the participants asses how often they have conducted or experienced cyber-bullying [12]. This approach is based on the premise that the participants will be able to apply a description of general behavior to truly estimate all specific situations they face and to categorize them into categories of bullying or non-bullying behavior [16]. It is questionable how successful adolescents are at this. An additional reason to doubt their ability to assess their behavior or experience on a global-item is due to the fact that the percentage of adolescents who are categorized as perpetrators or victims is several times lower when measuring with a global-item scale than when measuring with a specific-item scale [16].
For that reason, using specific-item measures can prompt recognition that a person has taken part in some cyber-violent behavior they would not recall without a specific item as a recall cue. There are different underlying categories in this type of measure. Some scholars use categories of direct and indirect cyber-violence [17], others differentiate between verbal/written, visual/sexual, and violence through social inclusion [18], cyber-forgery, cyber-verbal bullying, and hiding identity [19], or use widely cited Willard’s typology [20] that includes inflaming and harassment, denigration, impersonation, outing and trickery, exclusion, and cyberstalking [21]. Some authors [21] point out the importance of the timeframe used and suggest using “the past school year”. In our opinion, this can be very ambiguous since research can be conducted at the beginning, in the middle, or at the end of the school year. This motivated us to use “in the previous year” (calendar year) as a time frame. Along with using Willard’s typology of cyber-violence, the Olweus measure of traditional bullying [22] often served as inspiration to develop measures of cyber-bullying [16]. Olweus and Limber consider cyber-bullying as just one form of bullying [23] which is a position that does not consider the special features that the Internet provides to the perpetrator, like the illusion of anonymity, lack of visual feedback on victim suffering, or asynchronicity [24].
Still, traditional bullying was often used in convergent validity studies [16], as well as measures of aggression in the physical world [19]. Starting from the problem behavior theory [25], the same factors (individual, family patterns, peers) can contribute to various risk behaviors, and it can be used to explain cyber-violence [26]. This theory is strongly compliant with Bronfenbrenner’s theory [27]. The problem behavior theory prompted researchers to explore relations among different but connected risk behaviors online, like problematic Internet use and problematic gaming [28] and cyber-violence [29]

1.2. Cyber-Violence in Relation to Psychosocial Functioning

Along with the relation of cyber-violence to other online risk behavior, it is also related to various aspects of psycho-social functioning. One of them is self-esteem or the overall subjective evaluation of personal value. High self-esteem is a protective factor for both committing and experiencing cyber-violence [30], although individuals who can be categorized as perpetrators/victims have the lowest level of self-esteem, and perpetrators have a higher self-esteem than victims [31]. In addition, self-esteem contributes more significantly to explaining the variance of cyber-victimization than the variance of cyber-bullying [32]. In a high-school sample, moderate negative correlation was found between self-esteem and involvement in cyber-bullying, whereas the correlations of this involvement with depression and anxiety were positive and of a large size [33].
Depression, anxiety, and stress are often used as indicators of psychosocial functioning, and have a large negative correlation with self-esteem [34]. This is expected since lower self-esteem characterizes both subclinical and clinical depression, accompanied with a pessimistic view of future success and depressive mood [35]. Anxiety should be differentiated both from depression and fear. It is an overwhelming and preoccupying anticipation of negative outcomes. It often has physical symptoms like nausea, sweating, or shaking [36], which are similar to physical symptoms of stress [37]. Still, stress as a reaction to the realization of a person in a situation that endangers their physical or psychological wellbeing differs from anxiety—the cause of stress is often known, whereas anxiety cannot be attributed to specific cause. Despite this, all three negative emotional moods are highly connected.
They are also related to cyber-violence. Depression predicts cyber-victimization one year later, but cyber-victimization is also connected to increased depression in that time period [38]. Although it is expected that cyber-victimization be correlated with depression, it could be surprising that depression is also correlated with cyber-bullying and that the coefficients are similar (0.24 for cyber-victimization, and 0.21 for cyber-bullying) [39]. It should be borne in mind that 13% of adolescents are both perpetrators and victims of cyber-violence, compared to 8% being solely perpetrators and 15% being solely victims [31]. In addition, perpetrators/victims have the highest results not only for depression, but also for anxiety and stress. Depression has been predicted by relational and physical aggression in the failure model [40]. Children and youth who are aggressive, especially if they use relational aggression, impair their social relationships with peers. Consequently, they are more prone to internalizing problems, depression included.
Both state and trait anxiety are related to cyber-victimization, but only state anxiety is related to cyber-aggressiveness [41]. Anxiety is higher in the perpetrators/victims and victims group than in the perpetrators group [42]. Stress, measured both by biological markers of stress and self-assessed, is higher among perpetrators/victims and victims than among perpetrators or bystanders [42].
As can be seen, involvement in cyber-violence has a detrimental role in psychological functioning. It is also connected to lower grades and more devices owned [31].

1.3. The Present Study

The aim of this research was to explore the psychometric properties of the revised Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale: its latent structure, reliability, and descriptive statistics of underlying dimensions, as well as the relation of some known correlates of cyber-violence, like indicators of psychosocial functioning and online behavioral problems, with cyber-violence.
Using Çetin et al.’s scale [19] as a starting point, in 2015 the Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale was developed [29,31]. In that procedure, some behaviors (e.g., Internet fraud) were omitted as non-typical for peer cyber-violence, and others were concretized or modernized. Experience with implementing a prevention program against cyber-violence in elementary school from 2015 until 2019, and feedback from program participants, made us aware that revision and modernization of the scale is needed. In the summer of 2020, 19 elementary and 19 high-school students evaluated the 2015 scale version, which was used as an insight on the suitability of existing items and suggesting new ones.
The new version of the Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale led to two main goals in the present study. The first was to revise and explore the underlying structure, reliability, and descriptive statistics of latent dimensions of the revised Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale. The second was to explore the contribution of socio-demographic variables (gender, number of family members, parents’ level of education, and grades), Internet-related variables (amount of time online both on weekdays and weekends and the number of devices in the family), indicators of psychological functioning (self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and stress), and online behavioral problems (problematic Internet use and problematic online gaming) in the prediction of committing and experiencing cyber-violence.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The study involved 1725 participants aged 15 to 19 years (M = 17.17; SD = 1.307), with 30.61% or 528 male participants. The invitation to participate in the research was published on social networks. The sample was convenient and self-selected since the research was conducted online. According to the age structure, 13.9% of the participants were aged 15 (n = 240), 18.4% or 317 participants were aged 16, 23.1% were aged 17 (n = 398), 18-year-olds were 26.3% (n = 454) of participants, and 316 or 18.3% of them were 19 years old.

2.2. Measures

Major sociodemographic information (age, grade, type of school, gender, parental educational level, number of family members) and information regarding Internet use were collected (number of hours in online activities, both on weekdays and the weekend, number of ICT devices in their households, the device preferred for using the Internet).

2.2.1. The Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale—2021

The Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale—2021 (CECVS-2021) is a new version of the scale of the same name [29,31]. The scale was refined in a previous study in which children and young people (n = 38) assessed how appropriate the items were and proposed some adaptations (e.g., “I purposefully sent viruses by e-mail” to “I purposefully sent viruses via messages on social media”—examples are from the scale of action; there is an equivalent in the scale of experience) and suggested some new violent behaviors on the Internet that they noticed (e.g.,“Through something that seemed like a joke, a prank, false praise online”). Experts in the field (n = 3) suggested item shape and items that include hate speech. The final version has 34 items for committing cyber-violence, and as many items for experiencing cyber-violence. Participants responded on a five-point scale (1—never to 5—always). The higher results indicate higher levels of committing/experiencing violence, and the overall score for each subscale is the average answer for each scale item (ranging from 1 to 5). Cronbach’s α was 0.93 for committing and 0.94 for experiencing cyber-violence.

2.2.2. The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS—21) [35] is a 21-item questionnaire measuring three negative emotional states: depression, anxiety, and stress. The items were answered using a 4-point scale (1 = does not apply to me at all, 4 = applies to me very much or most of the time) to rate the extent to which they had experienced each state over the preceding week. A higher score represents greater distress. The reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s α) were 0.83 for depression, 0.78 for anxiety, and 0.84 for stress.

2.2.3. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) [43] measures global self-esteem. It has 10 items, and participants answered on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree). A higher score indicates higher self-esteem. Cronbach’s α was 0.80.

2.2.4. The Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale 2

The Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale 2 (GPIUS) [44] contains 15 Likert-type items rated on a 6-point scale (from “definitely disagree” to “definitely agree”). The scale has six factors (preference for online social interaction, using the Internet for mood regulation, compulsive Internet use, cognitive preoccupation with the Internet, and negative outcomes due to Internet use). The overall GPIUS2 index score was computed as a linear combination of answers on each item, which resulted in scores ranging from 15 to 105. The scale was highly reliable (α = 0.93).

2.2.5. Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire Short-Form

The Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire Short-Form (POGQ-SF) [45] measures six underlying dimensions of problematic gaming (i.e., preoccupation, overuse, immersion, social isolation, interpersonal conflicts, and withdrawal). It has 12 items, and participants answered on a five-point scale (1—never to 5—always). The overall level of problematic online gaming was calculated by summarizing answers on each item, and higher scores indicated more pronounced symptoms of gaming.

2.3. Procedure

The Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences in Osijek approved this research, and informed online consent was obtained from the participants. The data were collected during February 2021. The study was announced on SNS sites (e.g., Facebook, Instagram), with all information on the purpose and scope of research, the possibilities of obtaining feedback, and the possibility of obtaining a prize for participation (one participant selected per case received a prize of about EUR 50—HRK 400). Using online research and conventional sample was determined with ongoing epidemiological measures against COVID-19 (e.g., entering high school was forbidden even for parents, and COVID bubbles were used for schooling). The procedure that was used can cause self-selection bias. In addition, there was no information about participants who gave up before submitting their questionnaire. This was taken into account when interpreting the data.

2.4. Analysis

To explore the factor structure of the questionnaire, an exploratory factor analysis was performed. The analysis was performed separately for the item-subscale of committing and item-subscale of experiencing. To examine the contributions of different correlates to the explanation of cyber-violence variance, hierarchical regression analyses were performed separately for experiencing and committing cyber-violence as criteria. The same model was used in both analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Exploratory Factor Analysis

The results of exploratory factor analysis for experiencing cyber-violence are shown in Table 1.
For experiencing cyber-violence, the initial analysis shows that all 34 items had loadings over 0.40. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test indicated very good sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.95). Principal component analysis was performed to determine latent factors and resulted in seven factors with an Eigenvalue greater than 1. The parallel analysis with the 95% percentile criterion indicated that only the first five factors should be used and they explained 54.2% of the variance. The extracted factors were rotated with the Varimax method.
The pattern matrix for the Experiencing subscale is shown in Table 2. Participants with high results for the first factor experienced various types of shaming (e.g., rude comments, gossiping, exclusion from groups), so this factor was named “Shaming” and it had 13 items. The second factor consisted of seven items that describe manipulation with information to cause harm behavior (abbr. “Information manipulation”). Participants with higher results on that factor experienced situations like their conversations being shared via screenshot without permission or being contacted by people who used a fake identity or other uses of the Internet to find out private information about participants. The third factor was called “Hate speech,” had three items, and included insults, ridicule, and calls for attacks because of group affiliation. The fourth 7-item factor was called “Use of technology to cause harm” (abbr. “Technology abuse”). It involves hacking into profiles to destroy photos or posts, deliberately sending viruses through social networks, and extortion. The last factor was called “Sharing information about a person” (abbr. “Information sharing”) and included sharing photos, videos, or embarrassing information about a person, and editing photos in a shameful way. There were four items in the fifth factor.
The extracted factors had low to moderate intercorrelations (Table 3). The internal consistency of factors ranged between 0.75 (for technology abuse) and 0.91 (for shaming).
Analyses were also performed on data from the Committing subscale. For committing cyber-violence, initial analysis shows that all 34 items had loadings over 0.35 (only two items had a loading lower than 0.40, but were kept in the analysis as good indicators of specific cyber-violence behavior). The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test again indicated very good sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.96). Principal component analysis was performed to determine latent factors and resulted in five factors with Eigenvalues greater than 1, and the parallel analysis with a 95% percentile criterion indicated that only three factors should be used. The five-factor solution explained 57.6% of the variance, and the three-factor solution explained 50.7% of the Committing subscale variance. Since Eigenvalues for the fourth and fifth factors were slightly lower than those obtained in parallel analysis, it was decided to keep all five factors. The extracted factors were rotated with the Varimax method (Table 4).
The item distribution by factors shown in Table 5 differed significantly from the distribution for the Experiencing subscale. As can be seen from Table 6, the reliability for the fifth factor was 0.66. Efforts were made to name the factors, but theoretically, the distribution of items by factors for the Experiencing subscale is much more meaningful. Therefore, the item distribution by factors obtained for Experiencing was applied to this subscale. A reliability analysis was performed on the items distributed by factor, which for the Sharing factor was 0.87, for Information manipulation was 0.72, for Hate speech was 0.87, for Technology abuse was 0.88, and for Information sharing was 0.73 (Cronbach’s α).
Descriptive statistics for the Experiencing and Committing subscales factors are presented in Table 7. As is common with risk behaviors, most factors were positively distributed (most individuals had low scores on these factors). In Appendix A, the scale in English and Croatian can be found.

3.2. Explaination of Variance in Experiencing and Committing Cyber-Violence

To explore the contribution of socio-demographic variables, Internet-related variables, indicators of psychological functioning, and online behavioral problems in the explanation of the variance in committing and experiencing of cyber-violence, hierarchical regression analyses were conducted. In the first step, gender, number of family members, parents’ level of education, and grades were included. In the second step, information on the amount of time online both on weekdays and weekends and the number of devices in the family were added. In the third step, depression, anxiety, stress, and self-esteem were included. In the fourth step, problematic use of the Internet and problematic gaming were added. Prior to this, correlations between predictors and predictors with criteria were calculated (Table 8). The results of HRA are in Table 9 and Table 10.
As can be seen in Table 8, most variables correlated significantly with the committing and experiencing of cyber-violence. In addition, experiencing and committing cyber-violence were strongly correlated. The exception was the number of family members and the father’s level of education (the latter only for committing). There were quite high intercorrelations of the psychological functioning variables (self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and stress), which could cause the problem of multicollinearity.
The model explained a total of 21% of the variance of experience, with only 1% of the variance explained in the first step. The second step did not increase the explained variance at all, but the variables added in the third step explained as much as 18% of the variance, and fourth step explained an additional 2%. By adding variables to psychological functioning, gender became a significant predictor (young men experienced more violence). Grades and the level of maternal education were positive predictors of experiencing cyber-violence (participants with better grades and more educated mothers experienced more violence). Participants who showed a higher level of depression, anxiety, problematic Internet use, and problematic gaming also experienced more violence. Stress ceased to be a significant predictor with the introduction of the variables of problematic gaming and problematic Internet use—from Table 8 it can be seen that stress was correlated with both variables, but a more significant correlation was with problematic Internet use. Therefore, it can be assumed that the mediating effect of problematic Internet use is manifested in such a way that, from the participants who had higher levels of stress, those who used the Internet more problematically will experience higher levels of cyber-violence (as mediation effects were not the focus of this manuscript, no special mediation analysis was conducted).
For committing cyber-violence as criteria, the model explained a total of 15% of the variance. In the first step, 2% of the variance was explained, 1% in the second step, and 6% and 5% of variance was explained in the third and fourth step, respectively. Participants who committed more cyber-violence were boys; those who spent more hours online on weekdays; those who had better grades; participants with higher self-esteem, higher levels of depression, anxiety, and stress; and those who were more prone to problematic Internet use and problematic gaming. It should be noted that self-esteem was positively correlated, whereas it was negatively correlated with committing cyber-violence, indicating multicollinearity effects of depression and self-esteem correlation on committing cyber-violence.

4. Discussion

The first aim of this manuscript was to explore the new version of the Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale. For this purpose, exploratory factor analyses for each of the subscales were conducted. The factor loadings were larger than 0.40 with the exception of two items in PCA for the committing cyber-violence subscale, and those item loadings were 0.391 and 0.385, which is close to the recommended cut-off. Although it is possible to use a more restrictive approach (e.g., excluding all the items with factor loadings below 0.50 or 0.60), that would lead to a version of scale that is not informative relative to specific types of cyber-violent behavior. The final versions of the cyber-violence subscales have 34 items each, which should not be exhausting for participants. This is in line with the intention to produce a scale that explores specific cyber-violent behavior, and to use a specific-item approach where items are a form of retrieval cue for autobiographical memory. In exploratory factor analysis, a five-factor structure was extracted, but the item distribution per factor differed in its composition. The factor reliability analysis (the criteria was that factor and scale reliability should be above 0.70) and correspondence with known typologies [20] prevailed in the decision to give preference to the distribution obtained for the Experiencing cyber-violence subscale. The first factor, Shaming, includes behavior like inflaming, cyber denigration, and exclusion [20], whereas Information manipulation includes impersonation, outing [20], and non-consensual sexting [46].
The third factor, Hate speech, is included since information gained from qualitative research conducted in the summer of 2020 showed that adolescents report cases of hate speech when being asked about experiencing cyber-violence. It indicated that this behavior is related to other cyber-violent behavior directed toward an individual purely based on personal characteristics. Hate speech is defined as “all forms of expression which spread, incite, promote or justify racial hatred, xenophobia, anti-Semitism or other forms of hatred based on intolerance, including: intolerance expressed by aggressive nationalism and ethnocentrism, discrimination and hostility against minorities, migrants and people of immigrant origin” [47]. For instance, one high-school boy, when asked whether he had experienced cyber-violence, responded: “Well, various swearwords were directed toward me, my family, my religion and so on. I did not take it to heart, but I experienced it” [48]. Hate speech correlated negatively with experiencing Shaming and Information manipulation, and positively with Technology abuse and Information sharing. Three items describing hate speech were in one of the factors of the analysis conducted on the Committing cyber-violence subscale (the fourth factor), and it correlated positively with the four other factors in that analysis. It is possible that committing hate speech correlated with other cyber-violent behavior as a form of online aggressive behavior, but group membership was not as important for gossiping or impersonation (it is probably more connected with the abuse of some behavior or the characteristics a person manifests).
The fourth factor is named Technology abuse and it includes, as described by Willard [20], cyberstalking and online harassment. Information sharing is the name of the last factor, and a part of it is the behavior from Willard’s outing and misinformation categories [20]. Those five factors encompass three categories of cyber-violence from the Council of Europe’s Cybercrime Convention Committee’s framework: cyberharassment, ICT-related violation of privacy, and ICT-related hate crimes [49].
What can be concluded from the descriptive statistics is that participants in this study relatively rarely experienced or committed cyber-violence (the average results for all factors and totals were in the range of answers of “never” to “rarely”, and closer to never). Since data were collected online from a conventional sample, self-selection bias could have influenced in lowering the prevalence of cyber-violence among adolescents (e.g., participants with more cyber-violent behavior could decide disproportionally more that they did not want to participate in the study, and it is almost impossible to gain insight into attrition prior to submitting in this type of research). Still, these results are in concordance with results gained with a prior version of this scale from a representative sample [29,31]. In this research, the most frequent were experiencing and committing behaviors from Shaming and Information manipulation, and the rarest were experiencing and committing Technology abuse and Hate speech. This is understandable since those behaviors are under the influence of legal sanctions [50].
The committing and experiencing of cyber-violence correlated significantly and shared about 32% or one third of variance. This is in line with the finding that 15% of adolescents are categorized in the perpetrator/victim group [31].
The second aim was to explore the contribution of socio-demographic variables (gender, number of family members, parents’ level of education, and grades), Internet-related variables (amount of time online both on weekdays and weekends and the number of devices in the family), indicators of psychological functioning (self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and stress), and online behavioral problems (problematic Internet use and problematic online gaming) in the explanation of the variance of the committing and experiencing of cyber-violence. It was found that gender (male), grades, maternal education, depression, anxiety, stress, problematic Internet use, and problematic gaming were positive predictors of experiencing cyber-violence. For committing, significant predictors were gender (male), grades, hours spent online on weekdays, depression, anxiety, stress, problematic Internet use, and problematic gaming were positive predictors. Based on the criteria of explained variance, the model was more suitable for explaining variance of experiencing than committing cyber-violence (21% compared to 15% of explained variance, respectively).
This could be attributed to variables of psychosocial functioning (self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and stress), since those four variables explained 18% of the 21% of experiencing variance, three times more than they contributed to explaining variance of committing cyber-violence (6%). Since some research only explores the relationship between negative emotional states and cyber-victimization [39], or it was found to be less potent in explaining cyber-bullying [41], it is clear that experiencing cyber-violence is related to elevated emotional distress. However, the data supporting the failure model [40] that explains the interrelation among aggression and depression in a way that prior aggression elevates depression, probably through the disruption of social relationships, indicate the possibility that committing cyber-violence could also be detrimental for mental health. Prior data [31] indicate that it is stressful.
Only gender, grades, and maternal education proved to be significant predictors from the socio-demographic group of variables in the model, and those variables explained 1–2% of the criterion variance. Boys reported higher levels of both committing and experiencing cyber-violence. Although it is well known that boys commit more cyber-violence [31], gender differences for experiencing are not as conclusive. Some data [31,51] support that girls experience more cyber-violence, whereas others suggest that boys are more prone to cyber-victimization [52,53,54]. It should be kept in mind that this research was conducted on a convenient, self-selected sample with a gender imbalance, and this requires caution in interpretation. It was unexpected that higher grades and higher maternal education be connected with higher involvement in cyber-violence since academic success and parental education are found to be protective factors [30]. Since those two variables correlated positively with the number of devices and hours online, it is possible that lower maternal supervision due to maternal workload was an underlying mechanism. Good parental monitoring [55] proved to be a protective factor from both committing and experiencing cyber-violence.
Although problematic Internet use and problematic online gaming were significant positive predictors for both committing and experiencing cyber-violence, their contribution was 2.5 times bigger for committing than for explaining. Still, the amount of explained variance was low for both criteria (5% and 2%, respectively). This indicates that online behavioral problems are different behaviors [28], although they share some common variance and correlate with common personal features, which is in line with the problem behavior theory [25]. Both problematic online gaming [25,45] and problematic Internet use [44] are forms of addictive behaviors, and the harm they cause is directed mainly toward the person who manifests them (and somewhat toward their significant others). Cyber-violence by definition is directed against others. For that reason, a higher correlation between problematic Internet use and variables measuring negative emotional mood (depression, anxiety, and stress) than the correlation of cyber-violence (especially the committing of it) with negative emotional mood variables is understandable. For committing cyber-violence, other variables, like the dark triad, could be more relevant [56].
This study demonstrates some of the characteristics of the revised cyber-violence scale. It is important that the revision of the scale use a participative approach. Still, in the circumstances related to the pandemic (very limited access to school; even PTA meetings were only online) forced the authors to conduct their research online and to use a convenient sample. When lockdown measures allow it, the research is expected to be replicated on a representative sample. A convenient and self-selected sample was also imbalanced in terms of gender, which could have affected the results. In future research, it would be good to add variables measuring parental monitoring, traditional aggression, or some personal traits like loneliness, empathy, and the dark triad, especially psychopathy.

5. Conclusions

The revised version of the cyber-violence scale has a five-factor underlying structure: Shaming, Information manipulation, Hate speech, Technology abuse, and Information sharing. Committing and experiencing cyber-violence are rare and highly connected behaviors. They share most of the significant predictors: gender, grades, hours spent online on weekdays, depression, anxiety, stress, problematic Internet use, and problematic gaming. Those who commit or experience cyber-violence more are more depressed, anxious, and stressed; more prone to problematic Internet use and problematic online gaming; and spend more hours online on weekdays, but they also have better grades and better educated mothers. Experiencing cyber-violence is more connected with lower indicators of psychosocial functioning, and those variables explained a similar amount of committing cyber-violence variance as online behavioral problems. Boys are more prone to both committing and experiencing cyber-violence. Maternal education could act through lower maternal supervision due to a higher workload. Future research should include parental and family functioning variables, personal trait variables, and variables measuring other forms of aggression toward others. Still, these results suggest that the mental health of youth involved in cyber-violence is jeopardized, especially those youth who experience cyber-violence. Although it is a relatively rare behavior, this warns us that preventive action should be continuously undertaken.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Faculty of Humanities and Social Science J. J. Strossmayer University of Osijek (protocol code 602-04/20-04/116; 2158-83-07-20-3, date of approval: 20 September 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data supporting reported results can be found with authors per demand.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. The Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale—2021 (CECVS-2021, Šincek, 2021). Translation is only for informative purposes; it is not translated per standard procedure for questionnaires.
Table A1. The Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale—2021 (CECVS-2021, Šincek, 2021). Translation is only for informative purposes; it is not translated per standard procedure for questionnaires.
Instructions In Front of You Are Claims about
Uncomfortable Experiences We Can Have Online. You Need to Answer Whether Something Described in These Claims Happened to You in the Previous Year.
1—Never
2—Rarely
3—Sometimes
4—Often
5—Always
The Following Are Claims Describing Less Desirable Ways of Online Behavior. On a Five-Degree Scale, Assess How Often You Behaved Like This in the Previous Year. Numbers on the Scales Have the Following Meaning:
1—Never
2—Rarely
3—Sometimes
4—Often
5—Always“
Others” Refers to the Peers You Know in Person or Have Been in Contact with Online
Factor
Item Number
1I was gossiped about online.I gossiped about others online.Shaming
2Others referred to me (or mentioned me) online using nicknames that made me upset.I referred to others online using nicknames that made them upset.Shaming
3While talking with me or about me online, they used offensive expressions and symbols.I used expressions and symbols online in
order to offend somebody online.
Shaming
4I was mocked online.I mocked others online.Shaming
5I got anonymous negative comments online (e.g., YouTube, TikTok).I left anonymous negative comments online (e.g., YouTube, TikTok).Shaming
6Something I posted online was ridiculed.I ridiculed content others posted online.Shaming
7They wrote offensive comments under my online posts.I wrote offensive comments under others’ online posts.Shaming
8They used humiliating expressions for me online (e.g., you’re stupid, what are those
pictures you posted, four-eyes, skinny…)
I used humiliating expressions for others online (e.g., you’re stupid, what are those
pictures you posted, four-eyes, skinny…)
Shaming
9They spread falsehoods about me online.I spread falsehoods about others online.Shaming
10They wanted to exclude me or have excluded me from an online group.I excluded others from online groups.Shaming
11I was publicly assessed (e.g., proclaimed the prettiest/ugliest/most boring) online.I publicly assessed others (e.g., proclaimed the prettiest/ugliest/most boring) online.Shaming
12They did not want to tell me who they were while contacting me online.I hid my identity online while contacting another person.Information manipulation
13They reached out to me from profiles with a made-up identity (presenting as somebody else, fake profile).I used a made-up identity online (presenting as somebody else, fake profile).Information manipulation
14They entered my online profiles/accounts without my permission.I entered others’ online profiles/accounts online without their permission.Technology abuse
15They broke into my online profiles/accounts in order to post something bad in my name or delete/destroy some of my earlier posts.I broke into others’ online profiles/accounts in order to post something bad in their name of delete/destroy some of the earlier posts by that person.Technology abuse
16I was purposefully sent viruses via messages on social media.I purposefully sent viruses via messages on social media.Technology abuse
17They posted private, sensitive, or
embarrassing information about me online.
I posted private, sensitive, or
embarrassing information about others online.
Information sharing
18They shared my videos online without my permission.I shared another person’s videos online
without the permission of that person.
Information sharing
19They shared my photos online without my permission.I shared another person’s photos online
without the permission of that person.
Information sharing
20My photos were edited in an offensive way.I edited another person’s photos in an offensive way online.Information sharing
21I was made to talk about sex online, even though I did not want to.I made others to talk about sex online, even though they did not want to.Information manipulation
22I was forwarded photos of other people online (without their permission) that
depicted sexual content.
I forwarded other people’s photos that
depicted sexual content online.
Information manipulation
23I was threatened and blackmailed online.I used the Internet to benefit to the detriment of others.Technology abuse
24They talked to me online just so they could extract private information or services from me (money, game character, photos).I talked to others online so I could extract private information or services from others (money, game character, photos).Information manipulation
25They used the Internet to find out private information about me (address, phone number).I used the Internet to find out private information about others (e.g., address, phone number).Information manipulation
26They asked me to give them money online so they would not embarrass me (blackmailed me).I asked others to give me money online so I would not embarrass them (I blackmailed others).Technology abuse
27They used the Internet to trick me and take my personal information or money.I used the Internet to trick others and take their personal information or money.Technology abuse
28They made fun of my possessions (phone, clothes, shoes…) online.I made fun of others’ possessions (phone, clothes, shoes…) online.Shaming
29They hacked my social media or gaming
profile.
I hacked others’ social media or gaming
profiles.
Technology abuse
30They shared screenshots of my private conversations without my permission.I shared screenshots of others’ private conversations without their permission.Information manipulation
31Through something that looked like a joke, a prank, false praise, they actually tried to
humiliate me online.
Through something that looked like a joke, a prank, false praise, I actually tried to
humiliate another person online.
Shaming
32They made fun of me for being a member of a certain group (e.g., they wrote to me sheep *, gypsies, leftards **, cripple, four-eyes, and such).I made fun of others for being members of a certain group (e.g., I wrote sheep, gypsies, leftards, cripple, four-eyes, and such).Hate speech
33I was offended online just for being a member of a certain group (e.g., they wrote that the members of my group and me are dumb obedient sheep, smelly gypsies, slow cripples, stupid female, and such).I offended others online just for being members of a certain group (e.g., I wrote that they are dumb obedient sheep, smelly gypsies, slow cripples, stupid female, and such).Hate speech
34They wrote online that the members of my group and me should be attacked, banished, or destroyed just for being a member of a
certain group (e.g., banish migrants, send women to the kitchen, send disabled people to special schools…).
I wrote online that some people should be attacked, banished, or destroyed just for being a member of a certain group (e.g., banish
migrants, send women to the kitchen, send disabled people to special schools…).
Hate speech
* Shameful naming of people wearing a mask during the COVID-19 pandemic in Croatia is brnjičari—literally “muzzlers.” It was replaced with the English expression “sheep” used to make fun of people for wearing a mask and following lockdown orders. ** kradezeovci is a shameful nickname for member of the Croatian democratic union/Hrvatska demokratska zajednica: In Croatia they are called hadezeovci, from the Croatian way of spelling the abbreviation (HDZ)—but some people use krade (meaning “steal”) instead of hade to mark the members and supporters of that political party as thieves. It was replaced with the English expression “leftard,” a blend of “left” and “retard” used to offend people for being supporters of left-leaning political parties and the ideological beliefs often associated with said parties. In translation, it should be appropriately replaced with terms from another societal context; it should be something very widespread in a particular society (e.g., in Croatia, Roma people are the group widely racially discriminated against, and in other societies it could be another group—as the polite name for Roma people in Croatian is Rom, the term cd-rom is sometimes used as a hidden way to insult group members). Shaming 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 28, 31. Information manipulation 12, 13, 21, 22, 24, 25, 30. Hate speech 32, 33, 34. Technology abuse 14, 15, 16, 23, 26, 27, 29. Information sharing 17, 18, 19, 20.
Table A2. Skala činjenja i doživljavanja nasilja preko interneta—2021 (SČDNI-2021, Šincek, 2021).
Table A2. Skala činjenja i doživljavanja nasilja preko interneta—2021 (SČDNI-2021, Šincek, 2021).
Uputa Ispred tebe se nalaze tvrdnje koje govore o neugodnim iskustvima koje možemo doživjeti na internetu. Trebaš odgovoriti je li ti se nešto opisano u tvrdnjama dogodilo u zadnjih godinu dana.
1—nikada
2—rijetko
3—ponekad
4—često
5—uvijek
Slijede tvrdnje koje opisuje neke manje poželjne načine ponašanja na internetu. Na skali od pet stupnjeva procijeni koliko često si se ti tako ponašao/la u zadnjih godinu dana. Brojevi na skalama imaju sljedeće značenje:
1—nikada
2—rijetko
3—ponekad
4—često
5—uvijek„
drugi“ se odnosi na vršnjake koje poznaješ uživo ili si u kontaktu s njima preko interneta
Faktor
1Ogovarali su me na internetu.Ogovarao/la sam druge na internetu.Posramljivanje
2Obraćali su mi se (ili su me spominjali) na internetu s nadimcima koji me uzrujaju.Druge sam na internetu nazivao/la nadimcima koji ih uznemiruju.Posramljivanje
3U razgovoru sa mnom ili o meni na internetu koristili su uvredljive izraze i simbole.Koristio/la sam izraze i simbole na internetu kako bih uvrijedio nekog na internetu.Posramljivanje
4Rugali su mi se na internetu.Rugao/la sam se drugima na internetu.Posramljivanje
5Dobivao/la sam anonimne ružne komentare na internetu (npr. YouTube, TikTok).Ostavljao/la sam anonimno ružne komentare na internetu (npr. YouTube, TikTok).Posramljivanje
6Ismijavali su nešto što sam objavio/la na
internetu.
Ismijavao/la sam sadržaje koje bi drugi objavili na internetu.Posramljivanje
7Pisali su uvredljive komentare ispod mojih
objava na internetu.
Pisao/la sam drugima uvredljive komentare ispod objava na internetu.Posramljivanje
8Na internetu su koristili izraze kojima su me ponižavali (npr. glup si, kakve si to slike
objavio, očalko, mršavac…)
Koristio/la sam ponižavajuće izraze koji su se odnosili na druge na internetu (npr. glup si, kakve si to slike objavio, očalko, mršavac…).Posramljivanje
9Širili su neistine o meni na internetu.Širio/la sam neistine o drugima na internetu.Posramljivanje
10Htjeli su me isključiti ili su me isključili iz neke grupe na internetu.Isključivao/la sam druge iz grupa na internetu.Posramljivanje
11Javno su me procjenjivali (npr. proglašavali najljepšom/najružnijom/najdosadnijom osobom) na internetu.Druge sam procjenjivao/la javno na internetu (npr. proglašavao/la najljepšom/najružnijom/najdosadnijom osobom).Posramljivanje
12Nisu mi htjeli reći tko su oni dok su mi se javljali na internetu.Skrivao/la sam identitet na internetu tijekom kontakta s drugom osobom.Manipuliranje informacijama
13Drugi su mi se javljali s profila s izmišljenim identitetom (predstavljali su se kao netko drugi, lažni profil).Koristio sam izmišljeni identitet na internetu (predstavljao/la se kao druga osoba, lažni
profil).
Manipuliranje informacijama
14Ulazili su na moje profile/račune na internetu bez mog dopuštenja.Ulazio/la sam na tuđe profile/račune na
internetu bez dopuštenja.
Zloupotreba tehnologije
15Provalili su na moje profile/račune na
internetu kako bi nešto ružno objavili u moje ime ili brisali/uništavali neke moje ranije objave.
Provalio/la sam na tuđe profile/račune na internetu kako bi nešto ružno objavio/la u tuđe ime ili brisao/la neke ranije objave te osobe.Zloupotreba tehnologije
16Namjerno su mi slali viruse putem poruka na društvenim mrežama.Namjerno sam slao/la viruse putem poruka na društvenim mrežama.Zloupotreba tehnologije
17Objavili su privatne, osjetljive ili sramotne informacije o meni na internetu.Objavljivao/la sam privatne, osjetljiva ili sramotne
informacije o drugima na internetu.
Dijeljenje informacija
18Dijelili su moje video snimke na internetu bez mog dopuštenja.Dijelio/la sam video snimke druge osobe na
internetu bez dopuštenja te osobe.
Dijeljenje informacija
19Dijelili su moje fotografije na internetu bez mog dopuštenja.Dijelio/la sam fotografije druge osobe na
internetu bez dopuštenja te osobe.
Dijeljenje informacija
20Moje fotografije su uređivali na uvredljiv način.Uređivao/la sam fotografije drugih osoba na uvredljiv način na internetu.Dijeljenje informacija
21Navodili su me da razgovaramo o seksu na
internetu, iako ja to nisam htio/la.
Navodio sam druge na razgovor o seksu iako oni to nisu htjeli.Manipuliranje informacijama
22Proslijeđivali su mi slike drugih ljudi (bez dozvole tih ljudi), a seksualnog sadržaja, na
internetu.
Proslijeđivao sam tuđe slike seksualnog sadržaja na internetu.Manipuliranje informacijama
23Prijetili su mi i ucjenjivali me preko interneta.Koristio/la sam internet da ostvarim korist na štetu drugih.Zloupotreba tehnologije
24Dopisivali su se samnom na internetu samo kako bi od mene dobili privatne informacije ili neku uslugu (novac, lika u igrici, fotografije).Dopisivao sam se s drugima na internetu samo kako bi od njih dobio/la privatne informacije ili neku uslugu (novac, lika u igrici, fotografije).Manipuliranje informacijama
25Koristili su internet kako bi saznali privatne informacije o meni (adresu, broj telefona…).Koristio sam internet kako bih saznao tuđe privatne informacije (npr. adresu, broj telefona).Manipuliranje informacijama
26Tražili su novac od mene na internetu kako me ne bi osramotili (ucjenjivali su me).Tražio sam novac od drugih preko interneta kako ih ne bih osramotio (ucjenjivao sam druge).Zloupotreba tehnologije
27Koristili su internet kako bi na prevaru uzeli moje osobne podatke ili novac.Koristio sam internet kako bi na prevaru uzeo tuđe osobne-podatke ili novac.Zloupotreba tehnologije
28Ismijavali su moje stvari (mobitel, odjeću, obuću) preko interneta.Ismijavao sam tuđe stvari (mobitel, odjeću, obuću) preko interneta.Posramljivanje
29Hakirali su moj profil na društvenim mrežama ili igricama.Hakirao sam tuđe profile na društvenim mrežama ili igricama.Zloupotreba tehnologije
30Dijelili su screenshotove mojih privatnih
razgovora bez mog dopuštenja.
Dijelio sam screenshotove tuđih razgovora bez njihovog dopuštenja.Manipuliranje informacijama
31Kroz nešto što je izgledalo kao šala, fora, lažna pohvala zapravo su me nastojali poniziti na internetu.Kroz nešto što je izgledalo kao šala, fora, lažna pohvala zapravo sam nastojao/la poniziti drugu osobu na internetu.Posramljivanje
32Ismijavali su me samo zato što sam
pripadnik/ca neke grupe (npr. pisali su mi—brnjičari, cd-romi, kradezeovci, šepavi, očalko i slično).
Ismijavao sam druge osobe samo zato što su
pripadnici neke grupe (npr. pisao brnjičari, cd-romi, kradezeovci, šepavi, očalko i slično).
Govor mržnje
33Vrijeđali su me na internetu samo zato što sam pripadnik/ca neke grupe (npr. pisali su za mene i članove moje grupe da smo zatucane poslušne ovce, smrdljivi Cigani, zaostali invalid, glupo žensko i slično).Vrijeđao sam druge osobe na internetu samo zato što su pripadnici neke grupe (npr. pisao za njih da su zatucane poslušne ovce, smrdljivi Cigani, zaostali
invalid, glupo žensko i slično).
Govor mržnje
34Drugi su pisali na internetu da treba napadati ili protjerati ili uništiti mene i članove grupe kojima pripadam samo zbog pripadnosti grupi (npr. protjerati migrante, poslati žene u kuhinju, invalide poslati u specijalne škole…).Pisao sam na internetu da treba napadati ili protjerati ili uništiti neke ljude samo zato što su pripadnici neke grupe (npr. protjerati
migrante, poslati žene u kuhinju, invalide poslati u specijalne škole…).
Govor mržnje
Posramljivanje: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 28, 31. Manipuliranje informacijama: 12, 13, 21, 22, 24, 25, 30. Govor mržnje: 32,33,34. Zloupotreba tehnologije:14, 15, 16, 23, 26, 27, 29. Dijeljenje informacija: 17, 18, 19, 20.

References

  1. Odluka o Obustavi Izvođenja Nastave u Visokim Učilištima, Srednjim i Osnovnim Školama te Redovnog Rada Ustanova Predškolskog Odgoja i Obrazovanja i Uspostavi Nastave na Daljinu [Decision on the Suspension of Teaching in Higher Education Institutions, Secondary and Primary Schools and the Regular Operation of Pre-School Education Institutions and the Establishment of Distance Learning]. Available online: https://narodne-novine.nn.hr/clanci/sluzbeni/2020_03_29_670.html (accessed on 1 June 2021).
  2. Obraćanje Ravnateljima—14 Ožujka 2020. (Dan Broja Pi) (Address to the Principals—14 March 2020 (Pi Day). Available online: https://mzo.gov.hr/UserDocsImages//dokumenti/Obrazovanje/Raspored//Transkript%20obracanja%20ministrice%20Divjak%20ravnateljima%20-%2014.03.2020..pdf (accessed on 1 June 2021).
  3. Odluka o Zabrani Napuštanja Mjesta Prebivališta i Stalnog Boravka u Republici Hrvatskoj [Decision on the Prohibition of Leaving the Place of Residence and Permanent Residence in the Republic of Croatia]. Available online: https://civilna-zastita.gov.hr/UserDocsImages/CIVILNA%20ZA%C5%A0TITA/PDF_ZA%20WEB/Odluka%20o%20zabrani%20napu%C5%A1tanja%20prebivali%C5%A1ta....pdf (accessed on 1 June 2021).
  4. Odluka o Stavljanju Izvan Upotrebe Dječjih Igrališta i Otvorenih Sportskih Igrališta [Decision on Putting Out of Use Children’s Playgrounds and Outdoor Sports Playgrounds]. Available online: https://civilna-zastita.gov.hr/UserDocsImages/CIVILNA%20ZA%C5%A0TITA/PDF_ZA%20WEB/Odluka%20o%20stavljanju%20izvan%20uporabe%20dje%C4%8Djih%20igrali%C5%A1ta.pdf (accessed on 1 June 2021).
  5. Dragun, R.; Veček, N.N.; Marendić, M.; Pribisalić, A.; Đivić, G.; Cena, H.; Polašek, O.; Kolčić, I. Have lifestyle habits and psychological well-being changed among adolescents and medical students due to COVID-19 lockdown in Croatia? Nutrients 2020, 13, 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Vaillancourt, T.; Brittain, H.; Krygsman, A.; Farrell, A.H.; Landon, S.; Pepler, D. School bullying before and during COVID-19: Results from a population-based randomized design. Aggress. Behav. 2021, 47, 557–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Piquero, A.R.; Jennings, W.G.; Jemison, E.; Kaukinen, C.; Knaul, F.M. Domestic violence during the COVID-19 pandemic—Evidence from a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Crim. Justice 2021, 74, 101806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lessard, L.M.; Puhl, R.M. Adolescent academic worries amid COVID-19 and perspectives on pandemic-related changes in teacher and peer relations. Sch. Psychol. 2021. online ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Suler, J.R.; Phillips, W.L. The bad boys of cyberspace: Deviant behavior in a multimedia chat community. CyberPsychol. Behav. 1998, 1, 275–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hinduja, S.; Patchin, J.W. Bullying Beyond the Schoolyard: Preventing and Responding to Cyberbullying, 2nd ed.; Corwin Press: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2014; pp. 11–16. [Google Scholar]
  11. Olweus, D. Bullying at School: What We Know and What We Can Do, 1st ed.; Blackwell Publishing: Malden, MA, USA, 1993; pp. 8–10. [Google Scholar]
  12. Patchin, J.W.; Hinduja, S. Measuring cyberbullying: Implications for research. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2015, 23, 69–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Dredge, R.; Gleeson, J.; De la Piedad Garcia, X. Cyberbullying in social networking sites: An adolescent victim’s perspective. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014, 36, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Šincek, D.; Duvnjak, I.; Tomašić-Humer, J. Empathy and gender effects on cyber-violence among Croatian youth. Psihologija 2020, 53, 377–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Milić, M. Percepcija nasilja preko interneta iz perspektive učenika i nastavnika [Perception of cyber violence from the perspective of students and teachers]. Ljetop. Soc. Rada/Annu. Soc. Work 2020, 27, 113–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gradinger, P.; Strohmeier, D.; Spiel, C. Definition and measurement of cyberbullying. Cyberpsychol. J. Psychosoc. Res. Cyberspace 2010, 4, 1. [Google Scholar]
  17. Buelga, S.; Postigo, J.; Martínez-Ferrer, B.; Cava, M.J.; Ortega-Barón, J. Cyberbullying among adolescents: Psychometric properties of the CYB-AGS cyber-aggressor Scale. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Lee, J.; Abell, N.; Holmes, J.L. Validation of measures of cyberbullying perpetration and victimization in emerging adulthood. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 2017, 27, 456–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Çetin, B.; Yaman, E.; Peker, A. Cyber victim and bullying scale: A study of validity and reliability. Comput. Educ. 2011, 57, 2261–2271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Willard, N. Cyberbullying and Cyberthreats: Responding to the Challenge of Online Social Aggression, Threats, and Distress, 1st ed.; Research Press: Champaign, IL, USA, 2007; pp. 5–17. [Google Scholar]
  21. Savage, M. Developing a Measure of Cyberbullying Perpetration and Victimization. Ph.D. Thesis, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  22. Olweus, D. The Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire; Research Center for Health Promotion (HEMIL Center), University of Bergen, Mimeo: Bergen, Norway, 1996; pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  23. Olweus, D.; Limber, S.P. Some problems with cyberbullying research. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 19, 139–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Suler, J. The online disinhibition effect. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2004, 7, 321–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Jessor, R. The Origins and Development of Problem Behavior Theory. The Collected Works of Richard Jessor; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 15–43. [Google Scholar]
  26. Šincek, D.; Duvnjak, I.; Ručević, S. Provjera prikladnosti modela Jessorove Teorije problematičnog ponašanja za objašnjenje vršnjačkog nasilja preko interneta [Checking the suitability of Jessor’s Theory of problematic behavior model for Internet peer violence explanation]. In Kretanja-Europski Realiteti—Teorijska Razmatranja, 1st ed.; Đukić, M., Ed.; Akademija za Umjetnost i Kulturu Sveučilišta, J.J. Strossmayera: Osijek, Croatia, 2021; pp. 150–162. [Google Scholar]
  27. Bronfenbrenner, U. Ecological systems theory (1992). In Making Human Beings Human: Bioecological Perspectives on Human Development, 1st ed.; Bronfenbrenner, U., Ed.; Sage Publications Ltd.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2005; pp. 106–173. [Google Scholar]
  28. Király, O.; Griffiths, M.D.; Urbán, R.; Farkas, J.; Kökönyei, G.; Elekes, Z.; Tamas, D.; Demetrovics, Z. Problematic Internet use and problematic online gaming are not the same: Findings from a large nationally representative adolescent sample. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2014, 17, 749–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Šincek, D.; Tomašić Humer, J.; Duvnjak, I. Correlates of problematic gaming–is there support for proneness to risky behaviour? Psychiatr. Danub. 2017, 29, 302–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Zych, I.; Farrington, D.P.; Ttofi, M.M. Protective factors against bullying and cyberbullying: A systematic review of meta-analyses. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2019, 45, 4–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Šincek, D.; Duvnjak, I.; Milić, M. Psychological outcomes of cyber-violence on victims, perpetrators and perpetrators/victims. Hrvat. Rev. Rehabil. Istraživanja 2017, 53, 98–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Patchin, J.W.; Hinduja, S. Cyberbullying and self-esteem. J. Sch. Health 2010, 80, 614–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Baruah, H.; Dashora, P.; Parmar, A. Impact of cyberbullying on psychological health of adolescents. Int. J. Humanit. Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 137–144. [Google Scholar]
  34. Sinclair, S.J.; Siefert, C.J.; Slavin-Mulford, J.M.; Stein, M.B.; Renna, M.; Blais, M.A. Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the depression, anxiety, and stress scales-21 (DASS-21) in a nonclinical sample of US adults. Eval. Health Prof. 2012, 35, 259–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Lovibond, P.F.; Lovibond, S.H. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 1995, 33, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Nabi, H.; Hall, M.; Koskenvuo, M.; Singh-Manoux, A.; Oksanen, T.; Suominen, S.; Vahtera, J. Psychological and somatic symptoms of anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease: The health and social support prospective cohort study. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 378–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Guthrie, G.M.; Verstraete, A.; Deines, M.M.; Stern, R.M. Symptoms of stress in four societies. J. Soc. Psychol. 1975, 95, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gámez-Guadix, M.; Orue, I.; Smith, P.K.; Calvete, E. Longitudinal and reciprocal relations of cyberbullying with depression, substance use, and problematic internet use among adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 2013, 53, 446–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Bonanno, R.A.; Hymel, S. Cyber Bullying and Internalizing Difficulties: Above and Beyond the Impact of Traditional Forms of Bullying. J. Youth Adolesc. 2013, 42, 685–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Blain-Arcaro, C.; Vaillancourt, T. Longitudinal Associations between Depression and Aggression in Children and Adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2017, 45, 959–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Grigore, A.N.; Maftei, A. Exploring the Mediating Roles of State and Trait Anxiety on the Relationship between Middle Adolescents’ Cyberbullying and Depression. Children 2020, 7, 240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Boca-Zamfir, M.; Turliuc, M.N. Cyberbullying among Romanian adolescents: The relationships between the cyberbullying status and depression, anxiety, and self-esteem. In Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Adult Education—Education for Values—Continuity and Context, Proceedings of CIEA; pp. 375–382.
  43. Rosenberg, M. Conceiving the Self; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  44. Caplan, S.E. Theory and measurement of generalized problematic Internet use: A two-step approach. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2010, 26, 1089–1097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pápay, O.; Urbán, R.; Griffiths, M.D.; Nagygyörgy, K.; Farkas, J.; Kökönyei, G.; Felvinczi, K.; Oláh, A.; Elekes, Z.; Demetrovics, Z. Psychometric properties of the problematic online gaming questionnaire short-form and prevalence of problematic online gaming in a national sample of adolescents. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2013, 16, 340–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Pampati, S.; Lowry, R.; Moreno, M.A.; Rasberry, C.N.; Steiner, R.J. Having a sexual photo shared without permission and associated health risks: A snapshot of nonconsensual sexting. JAMA Pediatr. 2020, 174, 618–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Recommendation, no. R (97) 20 of the Committee of Ministers to Member States on “Hate Speech”. Available online: https://rm.coe.int/1680505d5b (accessed on 14 June 2021).
  48. Šincek, D. Kako naši mladi doživljavaju internet? [How Do Our Young People Experience the Internet?]. In Proceedings of the Festival Psihologije i Medija (The Festival Od Psychology and Media), Rijeka, Croatia, 17–18 September 2020. [Google Scholar]
  49. Council of Europe; CyberCrime Convention Committee. Working Group on Cyberbullying and Other Forms of Online Violence, Especially against Women and Children (CBG): Mapping Study on Cyberviolence (Draft). Available online: https://rm.coe.int/t-cy-2017-10-cbg-study/16808b72da (accessed on 17 June 2021).
  50. Kazneni Zakon [Criminal Law]. Available online: https://www.zakon.hr/z/98/Kazneni-zakon (accessed on 17 June 2021).
  51. Slonje, R.; Smith, P.K. Cyberbullying: Another main type of bullying? Scand. J. Psychol. 2008, 49, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Raskauskas, J.; Stoltz, A.D. Involvement in traditional and electronic bullying among adolescents. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 564–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Welrave, M.; Heriman, W. Cyberbullying: Predicting Victimisation and Perpetration. Child. Soc. 2011, 25, 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Šincek, D. Gender differences in cyber-bullying. In International Multidisciplinary Scientific Conference Social Sciences and Arts SGEM2014 Conference Proceedings; STEF92 Technology Ltd.: Sofia, Bulgaria, 2014; Volume 1, pp. 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kowalski, R.M.; Giumetti, G.W.; Schroeder, A.N.; Lattanner, M.R. Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychol. Bull. 2014, 140, 1073–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Šincek, D.; Duvnjak, I.; Žigmundić, M. Doprinosi osobina ličnosti objašnjenju rizičnih ponašanja na internetu. In 24 Ramiro and Zoran Bujas Days; International Scientific Psychology Conference Book of Abstracts; Jelić, T., Ed.; Department of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Zagreb: Zagreb, Croatia, 2019; p. 138. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Extracted principal components with Eigenvalues, percentage of explained variance, and parallel analysis criterion—experiencing cyber-violence subscale.
Table 1. Extracted principal components with Eigenvalues, percentage of explained variance, and parallel analysis criterion—experiencing cyber-violence subscale.
ComponentInitial SolutionEigenvalueParallel Analysis
% of Variance
Explained
95th Percentile of
Random Eigenvalues
135.70712.1401.268571
26.5282.2201.234820
34.1491.4111.213038
43.9251.3341.193046
53.8971.3251.174709
63.0781.046 *1.157576
72.9951.018 *1.140548
* Eigenvalues for those two factors are lower than those obtained in parallel analysis.
Table 2. Pattern matrix of CECVS—experiencing cyber-violence subscale.
Table 2. Pattern matrix of CECVS—experiencing cyber-violence subscale.
Item12345
40.761
20.744
10.742
30.730
60.639
80.638
90.628
100.536
70.530
310.507
50.466
280.461
110.408
13 0.671
22 0.629
25 0.557
24 0.522
12 0.503
21 0.473
30 0.437
33 0.748
32 0.680
34 0.672
15 0.742
29 0.732
14 0.652
26 0.562
27 0.518
23 0.435
16 0.433
18 0.799
19 0.786
17 0.604
20 0.500
Table 3. Factor intercorrelations—experiencing cyber-violence subscale.
Table 3. Factor intercorrelations—experiencing cyber-violence subscale.
Component12345
Shaming0.91 *0.262−0.374−0.349−0.418
Information manipulation 0.79 *−0.308−0.316−0.362
Hate speech 0.81 *0.2710.411
Technology abuse 0.75 *0.364
Information sharing 0.81 *
* Values on the diagonal are Cronbach’s α coefficients.
Table 4. Extracted principal components with Eigenvalues, percentage of explained variance, and parallel analysis criterion—committing cyber-violence subscale.
Table 4. Extracted principal components with Eigenvalues, percentage of explained variance, and parallel analysis criterion—committing cyber-violence subscale.
ComponentInitial SolutionEigenvalueParallel Analysis
% of Variance
Explained
95th Percentile of
Random Eigenvalues
138.27512.6311.268571
28.1402.6861.234820
34.2771.4111.213038
43.5491.1711.193046
53.4081.1251.174709
Table 5. Pattern matrix of CECVS—committing cyber-violence subscale.
Table 5. Pattern matrix of CECVS—committing cyber-violence subscale.
Item12345
260.857
270.803
160.773
150.764
290.759
210.661
230.644
170.608
240.576
90.491
220.484
200.391
30 0.736
1 0.653
28 0.587
6 0.581
18 0.544
19 0.522
11 0.476
31 0.445
14 0.434
10 0.674
3 0.622
5 0.597
7 0.567
4 0.565
2 0.543
8 0.472
33 0.742
32 0.715
34 0.687
12 0.809
13 0.772
25 0.385
Table 6. Factor intercorrelations—committing cyber-violence subscale.
Table 6. Factor intercorrelations—committing cyber-violence subscale.
Component12345
10.91 *0.3320.3340.3050.428
2 0.82 *0.3760.3500.330
3 0.83 *0.2270.219
4 0.87 *0.277
5 0.66 *
* Values on the diagonal are Cronbach’s α coefficients.
Table 7. Descriptive statistics of Experiencing and Committing subscales.
Table 7. Descriptive statistics of Experiencing and Committing subscales.
SubscaleComponentMinMaxMSDSkewnessKurtosis
ExperiencingShaming1.004.771.530.5531.673.81
Information manipulation1.004.861.570.5771.492.97
Hate speech1.005.001.310.6262.657.83
Technology abuse1.004.861.240.3813.3817.43
Information sharing1.005.001.340.5482.488.84
Total1.004.651.440.4432.127.32
CommittingShaming1.005.001.350.4032.389.45
Information manipulation1.005.001.310.4162.5710.90
Hate speech1.005.001.110.4085.2432.70
Technology abuse1.005.001.080.2817.2666.40
Information sharing1.005.001.160.3993.9922.30
Total1.005.001.250.3263.8123.35
Table 8. Correlations between predictors and predictors with criteria.
Table 8. Correlations between predictors and predictors with criteria.
Variable12345678910111213141516
1. Experiencing
cyber-violence
-0.569 **−0.0040.0260.071 **0.040 *0.051 *0.044 *0.078 **0.051 *−0.269 **0.387 **0.371 **0.344 **0.193 **0.305 **
2. Committing
cyber-violence
-−0.114 **−0.0090.060 **0.0270.055 *0.048 *0.136 **0.093 **−0.130 **0.222 **0.185 **0.191 **0.255 **0.283 **
3. Gender -0.002−0.034−0.0170.016−0.061 **−0.061 **−0.053 *−0.105 **0.085 **0.242 **0.238 **−0.391 **0.155 *
4. Number of family members -−0.015−0.0340.0380.138 **−0.0170.023−0.0240.0060.002−0.0230.0000.014
5. Maternal level of
education
-0.425 **−0.023−0.0140.0230.105 **−0.068 **0.066 **0.0100.0080.052 *0.005
6. Paternal level of
education
-−0.023−0.0350.0000.030−0.0310.0280.0070.0040.039−0.008
7. Grades -0.423 **0.312 **0.204 **0.000−0.033−0.014−0.028−0.047−0.032
8. Number of devices -0.288 **0.190 **−0.006−0.0140.006−0.0170.039−0.004
9. Hours online—
weekday
-0.556 **−0.096 **0.059 *0.0220.0170.063 **0.085 **
10. Hours online—weekend -−0.101 **0.059 *0.0200.0220.075 **0.068 **
11. Self-esteem -−0.683 **−0.515 **−0.483 **−0.158 **−0.350 **
12. Depression -0.650 **0.681 **0.200 **0.435 **
13. Anxiety -0.715 **0.111 **0.419 **
14. Stress -0.076 **0.452 **
15. Problematic gaming -0.284 **
16. Problematic Internet use -
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 9. HRA with experiencing as criteria.
Table 9. HRA with experiencing as criteria.
Predictor1. Step2. Step3. Step4. Step
Gender−0.0030.002−0.086 **−0.060 *
Number of family members0.0250.0260.0260.024
Maternal level of education0.067 **0.065 *0.049 *0.049 *
Paternal level ofeducation0.0130.0140.0130.013
Grades0.052 *0.0260.0500.060 *
Number of devices 0.0120.0080.006
Hours online—weekday 0.066 *0.0450.035
Hours online—weekend −0.001−0.013−0.018
Self-esteem 0.0220.035
Depression 0.218 **0.188 **
Anxiety 0.200 **0.178 **
Stress 0.086 *0.062
Problematic gaming 0.078 **
Problematic
Internet use
0.120 **
R20.010.000.180.02
R20.010.010.190.21
F2.90 *2.3893.08 **20.85 **
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 10. HRA with committing as criteria.
Table 10. HRA with committing as criteria.
Predictor 1. Step2. Step3. Step4. Step
Gender−0.113 **−0.105 **−0.157 **−0.112 **
Number of family members−0.010−0.007−0.004−0.008
Maternal level of education0.057 *0.0520.0440.045
Paternal level of education0.0010.0030.0020.002
Grades0.059 *0.0200.0350.052 *
Number of
devices
−0.001−0.004−0.007
Hours online—weekday 0.114 **0.104 **0.088 **
Hours online—weekend 0.0150.0090.001
Self-esteem 0.0560.076 **
Depression 0.144 **0.094 **
Anxiety 0.090 *0.055 *
Stress 0.091 *0.052 *
Problematic
gaming
0.134 **
Problematic
Internet use
0.196 **
R20.020.010.060.05
R20.020.030.100.15
F60.78 **70.75 **290.05 **530.73 **
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Šincek, D. The Revised Version of the Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale and Its Relation to Psychosocial Functioning and Online Behavioral Problems. Societies 2021, 11, 107. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/soc11030107

AMA Style

Šincek D. The Revised Version of the Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale and Its Relation to Psychosocial Functioning and Online Behavioral Problems. Societies. 2021; 11(3):107. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/soc11030107

Chicago/Turabian Style

Šincek, Daniela. 2021. "The Revised Version of the Committing and Experiencing Cyber-Violence Scale and Its Relation to Psychosocial Functioning and Online Behavioral Problems" Societies 11, no. 3: 107. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/soc11030107

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop