Next Article in Journal
Reputation Incongruence and the Preference of Stakeholder: Case of MBA Rankings
Previous Article in Journal
DAT1 and Its Psychological Correlates in Children with Avoidant/Restrictive Food Intake Disorder: A Cross-Sectional Pilot Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Emotional Regulation and Overeating Behaviors in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review

1
Department of Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
2
Department of Dynamic, Clinical and Health Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 29 December 2020 / Revised: 15 January 2021 / Accepted: 15 January 2021 / Published: 19 January 2021

Abstract

:
The worldwide prevalence of obesity has dramatically increased, mostly in children and adolescents. The Emotional Eating theoretical model has proposed that the failure in emotional regulation could represent a risk factor for establishing maladaptive overeating behavior that represents an inadequate response to negative emotions and allows increasing body-weight. This systematic review investigates the relationship between overeating and both emotional regulation and emotional intelligence in childhood and adolescence, considering both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Moreover, another goal of the review is evaluating whether emotional regulation and emotional intelligence can cause overeating behaviors. The systematic search was conducted according to the PRISMA-statement in the databases Medline, PsychArtcles, PsychInfo, PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Sciences, and allows 484 records to be extracted. Twenty-six studies were selected according to inclusion (e.g., studies focused on children and adolescents without clinical conditions; groups of participants overweight or with obesity) and exclusion (e.g., studies that adopted qualitative assessment or cognitive-affective tasks to measure emotional variables; reviews, commentary, or brief reports) criteria detailed in the methods. Cross-sectional studies showed a negative association between emotional regulation and overeating behavior that was confirmed by longitudinal studies. These findings highlighted the role of maladaptive emotion regulation on overeating and being overweight. The relationship between these constructs in children and adolescents was consistent. The results indicated the complexity of this association, which would be influenced by many physiological, psychological, and social factors. These findings underline the need for further studies focused on emotion regulation in the development of overeating. They should analyze the mediation role of other variables (e.g., attachment style, peer pressure) and identify interventions to prevent and reduce worldwide overweight prevalence.

1. Introduction

Obesity is a heterogeneous syndrome characterized by multifactorial etiology and a substantial imbalance between the assimilated and consumed calories [1]. This clinical condition, commonly defined through body mass index (BMI) [2], is often associated with dysfunctional food-related habits also influenced by individual characteristics [3].
According to the World Health Organization (WHO), the prevalence of obesity in the general population is about 13% [4,5]. The prevalence of obesity in children and adolescents (6–19 years old) has reached around 18% for females and 19% for males [6]. These results have legitimized the definition of the “obesity epidemic” [7] influenced by the maladaptive eating behaviors related to overeating. They represent a risk factor [8] for the increase of body weight and the development of many chronic diseases (e.g., cardiovascular diseases, musculoskeletal complications, diabetes, and cancer; [9,10]).
The excessive intake of foods (i.e., overeating), high in fats and sugars, characterized by an increase in the frequency of intake and quantity of foods consumed, is directly linked to the increase of body weight, causing all the health risks associated with the overweight condition [11]. Several hypotheses have been advanced to explain the onset and development of overeating. Some data reported maladaptive eating behaviors to the current cultural context where the excess of energy-dense foods coexists with low energy-cost availability [12,13,14]. Other authors have hypothesized that overeating is a consequence of cognitive-behavioral conditioning processes [15,16,17], attentional bias for food [18], or altered reward mechanisms that compare it to addiction behaviors [19,20].
The exponential worldwide increase in obesity focused researchers’ attention on the physiological and psychological etiopathogenesis of overeating behaviors [21,22]. Indeed, alongside genetic and environmental factors [23], emotional dysregulation and impairment of interoceptive awareness have been explored as risk factors for overeating and obesity [24,25]. Moreover, in the general population, emotional dysregulation has been associated with high BMI and maladaptive eating behaviors (i.e., overeating; [26,27]). Accordingly, several studies showed significant emotional regulation differences between individuals with excessive body weight and normal weight [28,29,30].
Some empirical data have shown in young adults that an adequate emotional regulation would ensure healthy food choices and a lower probability of losing control of food intake [31]. Conversely, a low level of emotional intelligence and the presence of emotional dysregulation would represent important risk factors for binge eating behaviors [32,33]. Moreover, high levels of alexithymia, found in patients undergoing bariatric surgery, are associated with low weight loss in post-surgical phases [34]. Emotional eating, commonly defined as overeating in response to a negative emotional activation, [35,36] would result from the loss of control in food intake that arises when the attempt to modulate and mentalize unpleasant effects fails [26,37,38].
The relationship between obesity and interoceptive awareness, defined as the ability to identify and differentiate the individual’s perceptions of their internal states [39], was also the subject of scientific analysis. Indeed, some authors showed that low interoceptive awareness appears in young adults, promoting and maintaining excessive food intake [40,41,42,43].
In the face of several data highlighting the strong relationship between emotional dysregulation and overeating in the adult population, some data described the first two decades of life as critical for developing eating habits [9,44,45]. It has been shown that approximately 80% of adolescents with obesity maintain it as adults [44]. In childhood and adolescence, family contexts can significantly impact the relationship between personality and behavior [46]; the parents’ influence could be particularly incisive on the development of maladaptive eating behaviors [47,48,49]. The attachment relationship is an important factor in explaining the intergenerational transmission of eating habits and emotional development during childhood and adolescence. A responsive family context provides children with a greater understanding of the causes, consequences, and the general nature of socioemotional functioning [50]. From late childhood to adolescence (i.e., 12–18 years), the ability to regulate emotions and the likelihood that emotional decisions will be differentiated based on motivation, emotion type, and social-contextual factors increase [51]. Although management of affects would become more and more sophisticated with development, dysregulated emotion processes could lead children and adolescents to learn and use ineffective strategies that place them at risk of being overweight and maintaining obesity in adulthood.
According to some data showing that maladaptive emotional regulation strategies mediate the relationship between impaired parental quality and emotional eating behavior [52,53,54], this systematic review aims to investigate the relationship between overeating and both emotional regulation and emotional intelligence in childhood and adolescence. In particular, the first goal is to verify whether both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies confirm a relationship between maladaptive eating behaviors and one, or more than one, specific domains of emotional regulation and emotional intelligence, such as cognitive reappraisal, emotion suppression, or emotional awareness. The second purpose is to evaluate whether emotional regulation and emotional intelligence can cause overeating behaviors by considering longitudinal studies. The final aim allows assessing the relationship between BMI and both emotional regulation and emotional intelligence by analyzing cross-sectional and longitudinal studies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Research Strategies and Information Sources

This systematic review was conducted according to the PRISMA Statement [55] and no protocol recording has been provided.
The research was conducted by using Medline, PsychArtcles, PsychInfo, PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Sciences databases using the following keywords: “Alexithymia”, “Emotion Regulation”, “Emotion Dysregulation”, “Emotion Self-Monitoring”, “Emotion Recognition”, “Emotional Intelligence”, “Emotional Empathy”, “Toronto Alexithymia Scale”, “Alexithymia Questionnaire for Children”, “Emotional Regulation Questionnaire”, “Eating Behavior”, “Eating Disordered”, “Emotional eating”, “External Eating”, “Binge Eating”, “Overeating”, “Obesity”, “Overweight”, “Mindless Eating”, “Emotional Hunger”, “Absence of Hunger”, “Eat to Cope”, “Unhealthy Food Intake”, “Eating Habit”, “Child”, “Pre-adolescence”, “Adolescence”, and “School Child”.
The selection of articles was independently made by two researchers (A.M. and F.F.), and the disagreements were solved by a supervisor (M.C.). The software Zotero (AGPL v.3 License; CHNM, Fairfax City, VR, USA) was adopted to facilitate the selection process. An initial deletion of the studies considering the title and abstract content was carried out according to the adopted inclusion and exclusion criteria.
The last search was carried out on 15 December 2019. No time limit was considered, and all studies published up to the date of research were considered.
Table 1 shows the scripts adopted for the systematic search in databases. In Supplement 1, there is a detail of the search strategies adopted in each database and the number of records identified.
The Zotero software was used to remove the duplicates. According to PICOS [56], the information extracted by each study was: authors, year of publication, country, sample information (N, age, sex, type of population), assessment tools, results on the relation between the variables of interest (see Table 2 and Table 3).

2.2. Eligibility Criteria

All the studies focused on the relationship between emotional regulation/emotional intelligence and overeating behaviors in children and adolescents and published in international peer-review journals were selected. No cultural or geographical limits to the selection of studies were considered. Therefore, doctorate dissertations that discussed the relationship of interest were also included.
The following inclusion criteria were adopted: (1) studies focused on general populations of children and adolescents (age range: 7–20 years) without clinical conditions; (2) studies on groups of participants presenting overweight or obesity conditions in the absence of organic metabolic causes or eating disorders; (3) studies that adopted standardized self-report instruments for the assessment of emotional variables: (4) studies that analyzed eating behavior, dietary preferences, or weight status to determine the condition related to overeating.
The following exclusion criteria were adopted: (1) studies not written in English; (2) studies focused on samples that did not include children or adolescents; (3) studies that adopted qualitative assessment or cognitive-affective tasks to measure emotional variables; (4) reviews, commentary, or brief reports; (5) studies that included participants presenting medical (e.g., diabetes, metabolic syndrome, hypertension) or psychopathological (e.g., eating disorders, depression, anxiety) conditions; (6) studies that considered the effectiveness of intervention focused on eating behavior or emotional regulation. Although studies evaluating the effectiveness of interventions on eating behavior or emotional regulation might offer some valuable insights, they have been excluded to prevent some confounding variables from hindering a good analysis of interest constructs.
Only studies meeting all inclusion and exclusion criteria were considered eligible for this systematic review.

2.3. Quality Assessment

According to the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Review [83], adapted to the main aims of this study, a quality assessment was carried out.
The dimensions considered for the assessment were: (1) Selection bias (I): the use of standardized tools for the classification of eating behavior or weight status; (2) Selection bias (II): the controlling of confounding variables during the selection of the sample: (3) Detection bias: the use of standardized instruments for the assessment of the emotional dimensions considered; (4) Attrition bias: incomplete outcomes; (5) Reporting bias: selective results discussed; (6) Other sources of biases. These six dimensions were described for each study as characterized by low (“0”), medium (“1”), or high risk of bias (“2”). After calculating the mean score and multiplying it by 100, each study was categorized as at low risk of bias (lower than 75%) or high risk of bias (higher than 75%) (as in [18]). Then, an analysis of the quality of each item of the assessment was reported.

3. Results

3.1. Studies Selection

The systematic search produced 480 articles. Other 4 articles were identified through other sources such as key journals for the topic or citing papers from key relevant studies. After the exclusion of 279 duplicates, 201 papers were screened. At the end of the selection process, 26 studies were reviewed. Figure 1 shows the process of the selection of the studies.

3.2. Qualitative Assessment Results

Figure 2 shows the quality assessment results, considering the percentage of the studies obtaining, for each considered dimension, a high, medium, or low quality. Specifically, no study reported a high risk of bias (>75%), a medium-high quality of the studies was found. However, the dimension characterized by the higher risk of bias was the “attrition bias”, typifying studies (11.5%) that reported missing or incomplete outcomes. A lower risk of bias was reported for the items characterizing the “detection bias”, with 25 studies that reported using appropriate instruments to assess emotional variables.

3.3. Cross-Sectional Studies

The systematic review identified 20 cross-sectional studies (see Table 2). Sixteen studies were focused on adolescents (age range: 12–20 years), one on children (around 10 years of age), and three studies considered both children and adolescents. Of these lasts, two reported a division considering the mean age, and one indicated only the range of age of the sample (between 11 and 17 years of age) (see Table 2).
Of the 20 studies, one reported a higher percentage of males, seven showed a similar percentage (between 45% and 55%) of males and females; ten studies presented a higher percentage of females; one study considered only females. A study did not report information about the percentage of males and females (see Table 2).
Sixteen studies focused their attention on studying the general population of children or adolescents (see Table 2), and only two studies adopted a classification of participants considering body mass index. Vandewalle et al. [76] analyzed the characteristics of a sample affected by severe obesity, while other authors compared the differences between a group with severe obesity and a group of control with normal weight [59,64,73].
The studies are heterogeneous in the classification of weight status. Wong et al. [77] used the classification suggested by Chen and colleagues [84]; Book and Berant [58] adopted the growth charts of Cole et al. [85] and De Onis et al. [86]; Percinel et al. [73] considered the suggestions of Neyzi and colleagues [87]; the study of Tan et al. [75] adopted the guidelines of the Group of China Obesity Task Force [88]; Gouveia et al. [64] used the WHO Child Growth Standards [89].

3.3.1. Emotional Regulation and Overeating Behavior in Cross-Sectional Studies

A high number of data reported a significant association between emotional regulation and overeating behaviors, regardless of the cultural context where the samples were recruited (see Table 2). However, there is a heterogeneity of the instruments for the assessment of emotional regulation used. The questionnaire most adopted was the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS), used in five studies [58,64,66,70,73]. Three studies [61,63,76] analyzed emotional regulation with the Questionnaire to Assess Children’s and Adolescents’ Emotion Regulation Strategies [90], and three studies [62,67,69] used the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; [91]). However, other self-report instruments were adopted (see Table 2). The study of Isasi and colleagues [65] used three different scales for analyzing the relationship between overeating and emotional regulation: Soothability [92], Sadness management [93], and Anger management [93] (see Table 2).
For the assessment of eating behavior, the study of Tan et al. [75] adopted BMI as an index of eating behavior; the other studies used self-report measures. Fifteen studies selected a unique self-report instrument [59,60,61,62,63,64,66,67,68,70,71,76,77] one study used two questionnaires [69], and two studies proposed an evaluation with three [74] or four [65] scales.
For the overeating assessment, various instruments were also adopted (see Table 2).
The correlational analysis results (see Table 2) highlighted significant associations between emotional regulation and overeating behaviors [58,61]. Some authors [65] found that higher emotional competencies were associated with higher self-efficacy and adaptive eating habits. Both emotional dysregulation and low emotional intelligence levels were related to a high risk of developing maladaptive eating behaviors [60,64,68,70,71]. Two studies supported a positive correlation between binge eating symptomatology and alexithymia [72], emotional dysregulation, and the absence of emotional awareness [66].
Some authors showed a strong negative association between emotional regulation and emotional eating [76]. Specifically, Lu et al. [69] identified a positive correlation between expressive suppression and emotional eating, and Laghi et al. [67] highlighted an association between emotional expression inhibition and overeating symptoms.
Only Wong et al. [77] reported opposite results than the other studies of this systematic review. Individuals with maladaptive eating behaviors showed higher awareness, expression, and adaptive use of their own emotions. Twelve studies used regression analysis focused on the predictive role of emotional regulation toward eating behaviors (see Table 2). Some studies reported a predictive role of emotional dysregulation [65,66,71], emotional awareness [66], and alexithymia [74] for maladaptive eating behaviors, overeating, and excessive intake of hypercaloric food. Other authors [60,68] identified the negative role of emotional intelligence in predicting the risk of the onset of eating behaviors, bulimic symptoms, and body dissatisfaction.
Five studies [62,67,69,75,76] observed that emotional regulation strategies predict negatively emotional eating [62,76], with the high consumption of hedonic foods [62]. Cognitive reappraisal was the main negative predictor of overeating [67], and expressive suppression was the main positive predictor of excessive and uncontrolled assumption of food in the presence of negative emotions [67,69]. Book and Berant [59] confirmed these results, highlighting a relationship between difficulty identifying and describing feelings and maladaptive eating behaviors associated with the body-weight increase.
Five studies analyzed the differences between groups [61,63,64,73,75]. Two studies compared a clinical group with severe obesity and a group with normal weight [64,73], but only one [73] reported significant differences between groups in emotional regulation. Three studies analyzed the differences between groups with different BMI [61,63,75], showing significant differences between participants with normal weight and overweight or obesity in emotional regulation strategies; furthermore, excessive body weight was associated with overeating behaviors and worst emotional strategies.

3.3.2. Discussion of Cross-Sectional Studies

Despite the heterogeneity of the measures, cross-sectional studies confirmed a relationship between emotional regulation difficulties and overeating behavior. The only results that did not confirm this association [64] may have been influenced by having included only participants overweight or with obesity.
Almost all the studies have focused on adolescence. This period of life has been described as a critical developmental period, characterized by reduced emotional coping [94] and enhanced autonomy from parents’ eating habits. These characteristics could explain some of the observed results.
Various hypotheses have been advanced to explain the relationship between emotional regulation and overeating. Some authors [68,77] have proposed a cultural interpretation bringing back the results to the social idea of “beauty is thinness”. According to this view, overeating would generate social unease and shame for their own body in individuals overweight or with obesity, especially in adolescents. Furthermore, chronic stress would cause emotion regulation difficulties [33,35,95], leading to unhealthy eating behaviors.
Overeating has also been described as an outcome of a maladaptive approach with negative and not-mentalized emotions, particularly anxiety, preceding uncontrolled food intake episodes in individuals that are overweight [61,64]. In the model of Pink et al. [27], it was suggested that alexithymia, which is only indirectly predictive of variation in BMI, would predispose people to higher vulnerability to depressive and anxious symptoms, which would determine impulsive eating behaviors.
Another explanation is proposed by the Escape Theory [96]. This theory suggests that the loss of control in food intake can also be described as the insane attempt to escape from a self-appreciating and ruminative self-awareness [63,96]. Emotional dysregulation, strongly related to depressive symptoms, could inhibit self-efficacy and could facilitate obesogenic behaviors such as sedentary lifestyles [65].
The food-related loss of control could also originate from the absence of mature emotional coping mechanisms, such as distraction, humor, and problem-solving [63]. Several authors [69,71,76] identified a more consistent role of maladaptive emotional coping strategies than functional mechanisms of emotional regulation in determining the overeating behavior. In a study investigating the emotional regulation associated with specific diet patterns [69], a significant relationship was found between emotional eating and emotional suppression, even in individuals in which cognitive reappraisal was not compromised. According to an integrative hypothesis, other results (i.e., [66], in line with an integrative hypothesis, highlighted how both the tendency to adopt emotional suppression and the inability to use cognitive reappraisal could contribute to the development of binge eating symptomatology.
A widely accepted hypothesis suggests the role of attachment style and parents’ characteristics as risk factors for developing maladaptive eating behaviors. A widely accepted hypothesis suggests the role of attachment style and parents’ characteristics as risk factors for developing maladaptive eating behaviors. Some results [59] indicated that the parents’ excessive use of emotional inhibition would reduce the social and affective quality of the relationship. Difficulties in emotional attunement with primary caregivers, especially during the separation-individuation process, could trigger the tendency to resort to maladaptive eating behaviors, such as overeating, to express their emotions [59].
According to this view, distancing [76] and emotional neglect [72] in the parent-child relationship seem associated with affective dysregulation and greater alexithymia. The difficulty in differentiating between the emotional activation and the interoceptive signals of hunger and satiety [97,98] can predispose alexithymic individuals to develop overeating episodes [74]. When maternal control is intrusive, guilty, and rejecting, it could also worsen the children’s need for eating independence [76].
Although the topics of this review have not been addressed in Oceania, South America, and Africa, it is useful to underline that the relationship between maladaptive eating behaviors and emotional functioning seems to persist regardless of the geographical area. Although samples were recruited in different countries with different socio-cultural backgrounds (America, Asia, Europe), all studies showed that the inability to organize one’s emotional world corresponds to a growing risk of being involved in unhealthy eating habits (see Table 2). Assuming that the cultures define what is masculine and feminine, further studies are needed to explore how the gender variable influences the relationship between these constructs. Evidence from both samples in China and Spain agree in suggesting significant differences between males and females, showing how girls presented a higher desire to be thinner, intense preoccupation with weight, body dissatisfaction, and finally more frequent episodes of eating in response to emotional upset [60,69]. Furthermore, it was found in the female group (and not in the male one) an indirect path from suppression to energy-rich food consumption through emotional eating [69]. Compared to the specificity of Western and Eastern contexts, the predictive role of gender on emotional eating could be stronger than that of cultural issues.

3.4. Longitudinal Studies

Six studies focused on the relationship between emotional strategies and overeating behaviors adopted a longitudinal design (see Table 3). Five studies were focused on adolescents (age range: 12–20 years) and one on children (age range: 7–9 years).
Three studies included a similar percentage (between 45% and 55%) of females and males; two studies reported a higher percentage of females than males, one study considered only females (see Table 3).
While five studies analyzed the relationship in children or adolescents’ general populations, one study analyzed participants from low-income urban areas.
Only one study [80] reported a BMI classification, using the criteria suggested by Okorodudu et al. [99]. However, these studies did not report an analysis focused on dissimilarities between groups with different BMI.

3.4.1. Emotional Regulation and Overeating Behavior in Longitudinal Studies

Different instruments for assessing emotional regulation were adopted by the longitudinal studies (see Table 3). Moreover, various measures were used to evaluate eating behaviors (see Table 3).
All the studies reported a negative relationship between emotional functioning and overeating, confirming a link between these variables over time.
Five studies [77,78,79,80,82] confirmed a relationship between emotional regulation and overeating behaviors. Specifically, Goldschmidt et al. [77] reported a relationship over time between low emotional awareness and loss of control in eating behavior. Harrist et al. [78] identified a role of maladaptive anger management and emotional inhibition in the onset of emotional and external eating [78], while van Strien et al. [81] highlighted the role of emotional dysregulation in predicting the onset of emotional eating. Orihuela and colleagues [79] focused the attention on the motivational aspects related to eating behaviors. Shriver and colleagues [80] reported a negative correlation between emotional control and emotional eating in adolescents and reported a high risk of overweight occurrence over time in the presence of poor emotional control.
However, some follow-up studies [81,82] did not report a significant correlation between emotional regulation and eating behavior over time.
Regression analysis identified the role of both maladaptive emotional regulation strategies and low emotional awareness in predicting overeating events [77,78]. Finally, Orihuela and colleagues [79] confirmed that emotional regulation influences maladaptive eating behaviors over time. In their study, participants who reported difficulty identifying and expressing emotion had a higher probability of experiencing emotional eating within one year from the first assessment.

3.4.2. Discussion of Longitudinal Studies

Although the presence of mixed results, longitudinal studies confirmed the negative relationship between emotional regulation and overeating and, generally, reported its persistence over time. In longitudinal studies, as in the cross-sectional ones, the main age target was adolescence, described as a critical developmental period in which there is an asymmetry between negative affectivity and available affective regulation strategies [77]. The results add to the growing literature suggesting the role of recurrent use of reactivity and emotional inhibition in the etiology and maintenance of emotional and external eating [78]. Moreover, both the difficulty in understanding and regulating negative emotions were considered causal factors of onset and maintaining dysregulated eating behaviors over time [77].
Vandewalle et al. [81] have analyzed the role of parents in determining emotional eating behaviors. The authors have also reported a predictive role of the mother-child relationship for emotional dysregulation and emotional eating. However, these results were not confirmed by the follow-up assessment. In the transition from childhood to adolescence, there is progressive emancipation from parents [100] and increased social pressure from peers [101]. These aspects allow suggesting a role of social conformism in the prediction of overeating [79].

4. General Discussion

Previous reviews have analyzed the association between emotional regulation and overeating behavior [35,102,103,104,105,106]. However, none of these have investigated this relationship in a healthy general population, especially considering childhood and adolescence. Some evidence suggested that compulsive eating behavior and obesity originate from a long asymptomatic history of hypercaloric food consumption [107].
This systematic review analyzed the studies that focused exclusively on non-clinical populations of children and adolescents. Differentiating the analysis of the cross-sectional studies from that of the longitudinal ones allowed reflecting better on the quality and persistence over time of the relationship between emotional competence and maladaptive eating habits during childhood and adolescence, supporting the use of interventions focused on improving one’s understanding and management of negative emotions.
Regarding this review hypothesis, both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies confirmed a correlational and predictive association between reduced emotional competencies and overeating [60,64,66,77]. In particular, distinct domains of emotional regulation and emotional intelligence, such as difficulty describing emotions, lack of emotional awareness, and expressive suppression were described as predictive factors of maladaptive eating behaviors. Finally, significant differences in the emotional regulation ability between groups with different BMI were also found [60,62,74].
Most of the studies provided control of some confounding variables, such as sex, age, and BMI, highlighting the possibility to consider the relationship between emotional abilities and overeating as a complex phenomenon. In fact, this relationship appears to be influenced over time by a plurality of physiological, psychological, and social factors [59,61,77,78,80,81,108]. Future research should clarify the role of many other variables compared to those evaluated in this study.

5. Limits

This systematic review presents some limitations. The low number of studies directly focused on the relationship between emotional regulation and overeating restricts the generalizability of these results. Furthermore, the small sample size does not allow generalizing the results of both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. The higher prevalence of females in the analyzed studies represents another restraint. Some results [59,68] identified significant differences between males and females that should be further investigated. Additionally, the exclusion criteria about the English language and the exclusion of so-called “gray literature” (unpublished works) could have resulted in the exclusion of articles useful in further clarifying the nature of the relationship.
An additional constraint concerns the characteristics of longitudinal studies. Only one study [80] considered an extended four-year follow-up [80]. Other studies have not investigated the persistence of the relationship between these variables over a long time, so it has not been possible to make empirical inferences on how these dimensions change over time. Another limitation is the reliability of emotional regulation measures in children, although studies have generally used validated tests. This limit prompts caution in the interpretation of data concerning children. Lastly, this review has not considered other psychological variables that could modulate the relationship between emotional regulation and excessive body weight. It would be a limitation that makes it difficult to find a univocal interpretation of the results.

6. Conclusions

The studies analyzed in the present review has highlighted consistent results on the association between emotional regulation and overeating behaviors in non-clinical populations of children and adolescents. However, some doubts about the temporal persistence of the relationship between effective competence and overeating persist. Further studies are needed to point out the periods of childhood and adolescence that are crucial for the consolidation of maladaptive eating behaviors and to confirm the relationship between emotional dysregulation, overeating, and overweight/obesity during the lifespan.
This review could help further research to achieve new conclusions that can help promote interventions focused on diminishing overeating, overweight, and obesity in the first phases of the development. Early interventions and prevention programs in childhood and adolescence could be important for reducing the ongoing increase in the prevalence of these diseases in the general population. These interventions, characterized by an integrated approach, should consider the emotional world and the individual’s affective regulation abilities, taking into account the different patterns of association that emerge in the development and according to the main characteristics reported by this review. This approach may promote greater effectiveness of the intervention.
Moreover, it would be useful a meta-analysis of the studies that could contribute to a more reliable interpretation of the results.
Although several scientific analyses emphasize the role of other variables, the interdependence between emotional regulation and overeating seems evident. These topics should be further explored through researches that should consider a longitudinal experimental design involving children and adolescents.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.C.; methodology, F.F., A.M., M.C.; formal analysis, A.M., F.F., M.C.; investigation, A.M., F.F.; resources, M.C.; data curation, F.F., A.M., M.C.; writing—original draft preparation, F.F., A.M., M.C.; writing—review and editing, F.F., A.M., M.C.; supervision, M.C.; funding acquisition, M.C., F.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Doctorate in Psychology and Cognitive Science found.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not Applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not Applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

ACCAcceptance
aERsAdaptive Emotion Regulation Strategies
AERSAdolescents’ Emotion Regulation Skills
BEBinge Eating
B-EDI-3Bulimia-EDI-3
BD-EDI-3Body Dissatisfaction-EDI-3
BESBinge Eating Scale
BES-TSBinge Eating Scale-Total Score
BMIBody Mass Index
CEMConformity Eating Motive
CERQCognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire
ChEDEQEating Disorder Examination Questionnaire for Children
CPSCognitive Problem Solving
DDEDifficulty in Describing Emotions
DEaDisordered Eating
DEBQDutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire
DEBQKChildren’s Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire
DERDifficulties in Emotional Regulation
DERSDifficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale
DERS-SFDifficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale-Short Form
DERS-TSDifficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale-Total Score
DISDistraction
DT-EDI-3Drive for Thinness-EDI-3
EAEmotional Awareness
EaDSEating Disorder Symptoms
EaDREating Disorder Risk
EAT-2626-item Eating Attitude Test
EAT-26-TS26-item Eating Attitude Test- Total Score
EBEating Behaviors
ECEMEndorsement of Coping Eating Motives
EDEmotion Dysregulation
EDI-3Eating Disorder Inventory-3
EDI-3-TSEating Disorder Inventory-3- Total Score
EEEmotional Expression
EIEmotional Intelligence
EMEating Motives
EmEEmotional Eating
EOExogenous Obesity
EREmotion Regulation
E-RDPEnergy-Rich Dietary Pattern
ERICAEmotional Regulation Index for Children and Adolescents
ERQEmotion Regulation Questionnaire
ERQ-CSCognitive Reappraisal
ERQ-ESExpressive Suppression
EUEmotional Utilization
ExEExternal Eating
F&VFruits and Vegetables
FEEL-KJQuestionnaire to Assess Children’s and Adolescents’ Emotion Regulation Strategies
FEEL-KJ-TSQuestionnaire to Assess Children’s and Adolescents’ Emotion Regulation Strategies-Total Score
FFQFood Frequency Questionnaire
HCHealthy Controls
HFHedonic Foods
K-PEMS19-item Kids Palatable Eating Motive Scale
LCLean Controls
LEALack of Emotional Awareness
LOCLoss of Control
mERsMaladaptive Emotion Regulation Strategies
NCTNeed to Control Thoughts
N-DDPNutrient-Dense Dietary Pattern
NEGNeglect
NMRThe Generalized Expectancy for Negative Mood Regulation Scale
NWNormal weight
OAAdolescents with obesity
OWAAdolescents with overweight
PIGHPut into Good Humor
P-OAProblem-Oriented Action
P-REAPositive Reappraisal
P-REFPositive Refocusing
REQRegulation of Emotion Questionnaire
RNEEBRegulating Negative Emotional Experience in a Behavioral Manner
RUMRumination
S-BSelf-Blame
S-DEVSelf-Devaluation
S-EBPASelf-Efficacy for Being Physically Active
S-EHFCSelf-Efficacy for Healthy Food Choices
TAS-2020-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale
TEIQue-ASFTrait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire—Adolescent Short Form
UWunderweight
YAQShort version of the Youth/Adolescent Questionnaire
YRBSYouth Risk Behavior Survey
WITWithdrawal
WLEISWong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale
BFBody Fat
CEMCoping Eating Motives
CoEMConform Eating Motives
DERSDifficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale
DRDietary Restraint
EAT-2626-item Eating Attitude Test
EEMEnhance Eating Motives
EIEmotion Inhibition
EMEating Motives
EmEEmotional Eating
EREmotion Regulation
EReaEmotion Reactivity
ExEExternal Eating
HWHealthy Weight
LEALack of Emotional Awareness
LOCefLoss of Control in Eating Frequency
mERsMaladaptive Emotion Regulation Strategies
OBObesity
OWOverweighed

References

  1. Yumuk, V.; Tsigos, C.; Fried, M.; Schindler, K.; Busetto, L.; Micic, D. Obesity management task force of the european association for the study of obesity. European guidelines for obesity management in adults. Obes. Facts 2015, 8, 402–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Rothman, K.J. BMI-related errors in the measurement of obesity. Int. J. Obes. 2008, 32, 3–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Ricca, V.; Mannucci, E.; Moretti, S.; Di Bernardo, M.; Zucchi, T.; Cabras, P.L.; Rotella, C.M. Screening for binge eating disorder in obese outpatients. Compr. Psychiatry 2000, 41, 111–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. James, W.P.T.; Jackson-Leach, R.; Mhurchu, C.N.; Kalamara, E.; Shayeghi, M.; Rigby, N.J.; Nishida, C.; Rodgers, A. Overweight and obesity (high body mass index). In Comparative Quantification of Health Risks: Global and Regional Burden of Disease Attribution to Selected Major Risk Factors; Ezzati, M., Lopez, A.D., Rodgers, A.A., Murray, C.J.L., Eds.; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2004; pp. 497–596. [Google Scholar]
  5. World Health Organization. Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  6. World Health Organization. Obesity and Overweight. Growth Reference 5–19 Years. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight (accessed on 20 May 2020).
  7. Gargiulo, L.; Bologna, E.; Iannucci, L. Epidemiologia dell’obesità in Italia e alcuni aspetti della qualità della vita. In 8° Rapporto sull’Obesità in Italia; Il Pensiero Scientifico Editore: Rome, Italy, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  8. Reilly, J.J.; Methven, E.; McDowell, Z.C.; Hacking, B.; Alexander, D.; Stewart, L.; Kelnar, C.J.H. Health consequences of obesity. Arch. Dis. Child 2003, 88, 748–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Serdula, M.K.; Ivery, D.; Coates, R.J.; Freedman, D.S.; Williamson, D.F.; Byers, T. Do obese children become obese adults? A review of literature. Prev. Med. 1993, 22, 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. World Health Organization. Global Action Plan for the Prevention and Control of Noncommunicable Diseases (2013–2020); WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  11. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Casagrande, M. The executive functions in overweight and obesity: A systematic review of neuropsychological cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Front. Psych. 2019, 10, 2126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Booth, K.M.; Pinkston, M.M.; Poston, W.S.C. Obesity and the built environment. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2005, 105, 110–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Lieberman, L.S. Evolutionary and anthropological perspectives on optimal foraging in obesogenic environments. Appetite 2006, 47, 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Speakman, J.R. Obesity: The integrated roles of environment and genetics. J. Nutr. 2004, 134, S2090–S2105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Belin, D.; Belin-Rauscent, A.; Murray, J.E.; Everitt, B.J. Addiction: Failure of control over maladaptive incentive habits. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013, 23, 564–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Everitt, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosc. 2005, 8, 1481–1489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Smith, K.S.; Graybiel, A.M. Habit formation. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2016, 18, 33–43. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  18. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Marotta, A.; Casagrande, M. Food-related attentional bias in individuals with normal weight and overweight: A study with a flicker task. Nutrients 2020, 12, 492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Halmi, K.A. Perplexities of treatment resistance in eating disorders. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13, 292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Moore, C.F.; Sabino, V.; Koob, G.F.; Cottone, P. Pathological overeating: Emerging evidence for a compulsivity construct. Neuropsychopharmacology 2017, 42, 1375–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Heras, P.; Kritikos, K.; Hatzopoulos AKritikos, N.; Mitsibounas, D. Psychological consequences of obesity. Endocrinologist 2010, 20, 27–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Malnick, S.D.H.; Knobler, H. The medical complications of obesity. QJM 2006, 99, 565–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Dash, S. Causes of severe obesity: Genes to environment. In Psychiatric Care in Severe Obesity; Sockalingam, S., Hawa, R., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2017; pp. 21–36. [Google Scholar]
  24. Murphy, J.; Brewer, R.; Hobson, H.; Catmur, C.; Bird, G. Is alexithymia characterised by impairedinteroception? Further evidence, the importance of control variables, and the problems with the heartbeat counting task. Biol. Psych. 2018, 136, 189–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Van Strien, T. Causes of emotional eating and matched treatment of obesity. Curr. Diab. Rep. 2018, 18, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Casagrande, M.; Boncompagni, I.; Forte, G.; Guarino, A.; Favieri, F. Emotion and overeating behavior: Effects of alexithymia and emotional regulation on overweight and obesity. Eat. Weight Disord. 2019, 25, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Pink, A.E.; Lee, M.; Price, M.; Williams, C. A serial mediation model of the relationship between alexithymia and BMI: The role of negative affect, negative urgency and emotional eating. Appetite 2018, 133, 270–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Baldaro, B.; Rossi, N.; Caterina, R.; Codispoti, M.; Balsamo, A.; Trombini, G. Deficit in the discrimination of nonverbal emotions in children with obesity and their mothers. Int. J. Obes. 2003, 27, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Elfhag, K.; Lundh, L.G. TAS-20 alexithymia in obesity, and its links to personality. Scand. J. Psychol. 2007, 48, 391–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Fukunishi, I.; Kaji, N. Externally oriented thinking of obese men and women. Psychol. Rep. 1997, 80, 219–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kerin, J.L.; Webb, H.J.; Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J. Resisting the temptation of food: Regulating overeating and associations with emotion regulation, mindfulness, and eating pathology. Aust. J. Psychol. 2017, 70, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Crockett, A.C.; Myhre, S.K.; Rokke, P.D. Boredom proneness and emotion regulation predict emotional eating. J. Health Psychol. 2015, 20, 670–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Zysberg, L.; Rubanov, A. Emotional intelligence and emotional eating patterns: A new insight into the antecedents of eating disorders? J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2010, 42, 345–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Paone, E.; Pierro, L.; Damico, A.; Aceto, P.; Campanle, F.C.; Silecchia, G.; Lai, C. Alexithymia and weight loss in obese patients underwent laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy. Eat. Weight Disord. 2019, 24, 129–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Leehr, E.J.; Krohmer, K.; Schag, K.; Dresler, T.; Zipfel, S.; Giel, K.E. Emotion regulation model in binge eating disorder and obesity—A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 49, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pinaquy, S.; Chabrol, H.; Simon, C.; Louvet, J.P.; Barbe, P. Emotional eating, alexithymia, and binge-eating disorder in obese women. Obes. Res. 2003, 11, 195–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Gianini, L.M.; White, M.A.; Masheb, R.M. Eating pathology, emotion regulation, and emotional overeating in obese adults with binge eating disorder. Eat. Behav. 2013, 14, 309–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Zijlstra, H.; Van Middendorp, H.; Devaere, L.; Larsen, J.K.; Van Ramshorst, B.; Geenen, R. Emotion processing and regulation in women with morbid obesity who apply for bariatric surgery. Psycol. Health 2017, 27, 1375–1387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Willem, C.; Gandolphe, M.-C.; Roussel, M.; Verkindt, H.; Pattou, F.; Nandrino, J.L. Difficulties in emotion regulation and deficits in interoceptive awareness in moderate and severe obesity. Eat. Weight Dis. 2019, 24, 633–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Critchley, H.D.; Garfnkel, S.N. Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 17, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Garfnkel, S.N.; Seth, A.K.; Barrett, A.B.; Suzuki, K.; Critchley, H.D. Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psych. 2015, 104, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Oswald, A.; Chapman, J.; Wilson, C. Do interoceptive awareness and interoceptive responsiveness mediate the relationship between body appreciation and intuitive eating in young women? Appetite 2017, 109, 66–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Young, H.A.; Williams, C.; Pink, A.E.; Freegard, G.; Owens, A.; Benton, D. Getting to the heart of the matter: Does aberrant interoceptive processing contribute towards emotional eating? PLoS ONE 2017, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Alberga, A.; Sigal, R.; Goldfield, G.; Prud’Homme, D.; Kenny, G. Overweight ad obese teenagers. Why is adolescence a critical period? Pediatr. Obes. 2017, 7, 261–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mikkilä, V.; Räsänen, L.; Raitakari, O.; Pietinen, P.; Viikari, J. Consistent dietary patterns identified from childhood to adulthood: The cardiovascular risk in young Finns study. Br. J. Nutr. 2005, 93, 923–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Belsky, J. The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Dev. 1984, 55, 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Farrow, C.V. Do parental feeding practices moderate the relationships between impulsivity and eating in children? Eat. Behav. 2012, 13, 150–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Farrow, C.V.; Haycraft, E.; Blissett, J.M. Teaching our children when to eat: How parental feeding practices inform the development of emotional eating—A longitudinal experimental design. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2015, 101, 908–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Powell, F.C.; Farrow, C.; Meyer, C. Food avoidance in children: The influence of maternal feeding practices and behaviours. Appetite 2011, 57, 683–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Zeman, J.; Cassano, M.; Perry-Parrish, C.; Stegall, S. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2006, 27, 155–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gnepp, J.; Hess, D.L.R. Children’s understanding of verbal and facial display rules. Dev. Psychol. 1986, 22, 103–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Jaffe, M.; Gullone, E.; Hughes, E.K. The roles of temperamental dispositions and perceived parenting behaviours in the use of two emotion regulation strategies in late childhood. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 1, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Loman, M.M.; Gunnar, M.R. Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulation in children. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 6, 867–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Morris, A.S.; Silk, J.S.; Steinberg, L.; Myers, S.S.; Robinson, L.R. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc. Dev. 2007, 16, 361–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; Mulrow, C.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Ioannidis, J.P.A.; Clarke, M.; Devereaux, P.J.; Kleijnen, J. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Liberati, A.; Altman, D.G.; Tetzlaff, J.; Mulrow, C.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Ioannidis JP, A.; Clarke, M.; Devereaux, P.J.; Kleijnen, J.; Moher, D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2009, 62, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Bayraktar, F. Fathers’ Parenting Attitudes and Turkish Adolescents’ Eating Behaviors and Emotion Regulation Skills. Ph.D. Thesis, Walden University, Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  58. Book, A.; Berant, E. The contribution of difficulties in separation-individuation and emotional regulation to obesity among adolescent girls. Asia Pac. J. Couns. Psychother. 2013, 5, 28–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Cuesta-Zamora, C.; González-Martí, I.; García-López, L.M. The role of trait emotional intelligence in body dissatisfaction and eating disorder symptoms in preadolescents and adolescents. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 126, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Czaja, J.; Rief, W.; Hilbert, A. Emotion regulation and binge eating in children. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2009, 42, 356–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Ferrer, R.A.; Green, P.A.; Oh, A.Y.; Hennessy, E.; Dwyer, L.A. Emotion suppression, emotional eating, and eating behavior among parent-adolescent dyads. Emotion 2017, 17, 1052–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Goossens, L.; Van Malderen, E.; Van Durme, K.; Braet, C. Loss of control eating in adolescents: Associations with adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies. Eat. Behav. 2016, 22, 156–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Gouveia, M.J.; Canavarro, M.C.; Moreira, H. Associations between Mindfulness, self-compassion, difficulties in emotion regulation and emotional eating among adolescents with overweight/obesity. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2018, 28, 273–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Isasi, C.R.; Ostrovsky, N.W.; Wills, T.A. The association of emotion regulation with lifestyle behaviours in inner-city adolescents. Eat. Behav. 2013, 14, 518–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Laghi, F.; Bianchi, D.; Pompili, S.; Lonigro, A.; Baiocco, R. Metacognition, emotional functioning and binge eating in adolescence: The moderation role of need to control thoughts. Eat. Weight Disord. 2018, 23, 861–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Laghi, F.; Liga, F.; Pompili, S. Adolescents who binge eat and drink: The role of emotion regulation. J. Addict. Dis. 2019, 37, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Li, Y. Social anxiety and eating disorder risk among chinese adolescents: The role of emotional intelligence. Sch. Ment. Health 2018, 10, 264–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Lu, Q.; Tao, F.; Hou, F.; Zhang, Z.; Ren, L. Emotion regulation, emotional eating and the energy-rich dietary pattern. A population-based study in Chinese adolescents. Appetite 2016, 99, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. McEwen, C.; Flouri, E. Fathers’ parenting, adverse life events, and adolescents’ emotional and eating disorder symptoms: The role of emotion regulation. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 18, 206–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Mills, P.; Newman, E.F.; Cossa, J.; Murray, G. Emotional maltreatment and disordered eating in adolescents: Testing the mediating role of emotion regulation. Child Abuse Negl. 2014, 39, 156–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Minnich, A.M.; Gordon, K.H. Examining the mediating role of alexithymia in the association between childhood neglect and disordered eating behaviors in men and women. Psychol. Men Masc. 2017, 17, 414–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Percinel, I.; Ozbaran, B.; Kose, S.; Simsek, D.G.; Darcan, S. Increased deficits in emotion recognition and regulation in children and adolescents with exogenous obesity. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2016, 19, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Shank, L.M.; Tanofsky-Kraff, M.; Kelly, N.R.; Jaramillo, M.; Rubin, S.G.; Altman, D.R.; Byrne, M.E.; LeMay-Russell, S.; Schvey, N.A.; Broadney, M.M.; et al. The association between alexithymia and eating behavior in children and adolescents. Appetite 2019, 142, 104381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Tan, Y.; Xin, X.; Wang, X.; Yao, S. Cognitive emotion regulation strategies in chinese adolescents with overweight and obesity. Child Obes. 2017, 14, 26–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Vandewalle, J.; Moens, E.; Braet, C. Comprehending emotional eating in obese youngsters: The role of parental rejection and emotion regulation. Int. J. Obes. 2014, 38, 525–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Wong, Y.; Lin, J.S.; Chang, Y.J. Body satisfaction, emotional intelligence and the development of disturbed eating: A survey of Taiwanese students. Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 23, 651–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Goldschmidt, A.B.; Lavender, J.M.; Hipwell, A.E.; Stepp, S.D.; Keenan, K. Emotion regulation and loss of control eating in community-based adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2016, 45, 183–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Harrist, A.W.; Hubbs-Tait, L.; Topham, G.L.; Shriver, L.H.; Page, M.C. Emotion regulation is related to children’s emotional and external eating. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2013, 34, 557–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Orihuela, C.A.; Mrug, S.; Boggiano, M.M. Reciprocal relationships between emotion regulation and motives for eating palatable foods in African American adolescents. Appetite 2017, 117, 303–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Shriver, L.H.; Dollar, J.M.; Lawless, M.; Calkins, S.D.; Keane, S.P.; Shanahan, L.; Wideman, L. Longitudinal associations between emotion regulation and adiposity in late adolescence: Indirect efects through eating behaviors. Nutrients 2019, 11, 517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Vandewalle, J.; Moens, E.; Braet, C. Can we link emotional eating with the emotion regulation skills of adolescents? Psychol. Health 2016, 31, 857–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Van Strien, T.; Beijers, R.; Smeekens, S.; Winkens, L.H.; Konttinen, H. Parenting quality in infancy and emotional eating in adolescence: Mediation through emotion suppression and alexithymia. Appetite 2019, 141, 104339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Higgins, J.P.T.; Green, S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions; Version 5.1.0; The Cochrane Collaboration: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  84. Chen, W.; Lin, C.C.; Peng, C.T.; Li, C.I.; Wu, H.C.; Chiang, J. Approaching healthy body mass index norms for children and adolescents from health-related physical fitness. Obes. Rev. 2002, 3, 225–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Cole, T.J.; Bellizzi, M.C.; Flegal, M.; Diedz, W.H. Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: International survey. Br. Med. J. 2000, 320, 1240–1245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. De Onis, M.; Onyango, A.W.; Borghu, E.; Siyam, A.; Nishida, C.; Siekmann, J. Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescence. Bull. World Health 2007, 85, 660–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Neyzi, O.; Gunoz, H.; Furman, A.; Ruveide, B. Weight, height, head circumference, and bory mass index references for Turkish children. Çocuk Sağlığı ve Hastalıkları Dergisi 2008, 51, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Group of China Obesity Task Force. Body mass index reference norm for screening overweight and obesity in Chinese children and adolescents. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi 2004, 25, 97–102. [Google Scholar]
  89. World Health Organization. WHO Multicenter Growth Reference Study Group. WHO Child Growth Standards: Lenght/Height-for-Age, Weight-for-Age, Weight-for-Lenght, Weight-for-Height and Body Mass Index-for-Age: Methods and Development; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  90. Cracco, E.; Van Durme, K.; Braet, C. Validation of the FEEL-KJ: An instrument to measure emotion regulation strategies in children and adolescents. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0137080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  91. Gross, J.J.; John, O.P. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 348–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Kendall, P.C.; Williams, C.L. Epidemiology of depressive mood in adolescents: An empyrical study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1982, 39, 1205–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Shipman, K.; Zeman, J.; Penza, S.; Champion, K. Emotion management skills in sexually maltreated and non maltreated girls: A developmental psychopatology perspective. Dev. Psychopat. 2000, 12, 47–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Patterson, J.M.; McCubbin, H.I. Adolescent coping style and behaviors: Conceptualization and measurement. J. Adolesc. 1987, 10, 163–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Green, M.A.; Scott, N.A.; Cross, S.E.; Liao, K.Y.H.; Hallengren, J.J.; Davids, C.M.; Carter, L.P.; Kugler, D.W.; Read, K.E.; Jepson, A.J. Eating disorder behaviors and depression: A minimal relationship beyond social comparison, self-esteem, and body dissatisfaction. J. Clin. Psychol. 2009, 65, 989–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Heatherton, T.F.; Baumeister, R.F. Binge eating as escape from self awareness. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 110, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Murphy, J.; Brewer, R.; Catmur, C.; Bird, G. Interoception and psychopathology: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 23, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. O’Reilly, G.A.; Cook, L.; Spruijt-Metz, D.; Black, D.S. Mindfulness-based interventions for obesity-related eating behaviours: A literature review. Obes. Rev. 2014, 15, 453–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Okorodudu, D.O.; Jumean, M.F.; Montori, V.M.; Romero-Corral, A.; Somers, V.K.; Erwin, P.J.; Lopez-Jimenez, F. Diagnostic performance of body mass index to identify obesity as defined by body adiposity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Obes. 2010, 34, 791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Larson, R.W.; Moneta, G.; Richards, M.H.; Holmbeck, G.; Duckett, E. Changes in adolescents’ daily interactions with their families from ages 10 to 18: Disengagement and transformation. Dev. Psychol. 1996, 32, 744–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Haye, K.; Robins, G.; Mohr, P.; Wilson, C. Adolescents’ intake of junk food: Processes and mechanisms driving consumption similarities among friends. J. Res. Adolesc. 2013, 23, 524–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Godfrey, K.M.; Gallo, L.C.; Afari, N. Mindfulness-based interventions for binge eating: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Behav. Med. 2015, 38, 348–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Kittel, R.; Brauhardt, A.; Hilbert, A. Cognitive and emotional functioning in binge-eating disorder: A systematic review. Int. J. Eat. Dis. 2015, 48, 535–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Nicholls, W.; Devonport, T.J.; Blake, M. The association between emotions and eating behaviour in an obese population with binge eating disorder. Obes. Rev. 2015, 17, 30–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Brownell, K.D.; Horgan, K.B. Food Fight: The Inside Story of the Food Industry, America’s Obesity Crisis and What We Can Do About It; McGraw Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  106. Dingemans, A.; Danner, U.; Parks, M. Emotion regulation in binge eating disorder: A review. Nutrients 2017, 9, 1274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Di Segni, M.; Patrono, E.; Patella, L.; Puglisi-Allegra, S.; Ventura, R. Animal models of compulsive eating behavior. Nutrients 2014, 6, 4591–4609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Sahoo, K.; Sahoo, B.; Kumar Choudhury, A.; Sofi, N.Y.; Kumar, R.; Bhadoria, A.S. Childhood obesity: Causes and consequences. J. Fam. Med. Prim. Care 2015, 4, 187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram.
Figure 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram.
Behavsci 11 00011 g001
Figure 2. Percentage of studies which presents high/medium/low quality for each dimension assessed. (1) Selection bias (I); (2) Selection bias (II); (3) Detection bias; (4) Attrition bias; (5) Reporting bias; (6) Other sources of biases.
Figure 2. Percentage of studies which presents high/medium/low quality for each dimension assessed. (1) Selection bias (I); (2) Selection bias (II); (3) Detection bias; (4) Attrition bias; (5) Reporting bias; (6) Other sources of biases.
Behavsci 11 00011 g002
Table 1. Databases, scripts, and the number of records for each database.
Table 1. Databases, scripts, and the number of records for each database.
DatabasesScriptsRecords
MedlineSince EBSCOHost does not allow the filters TI (Title) and AB (Abstract) to be used simultaneously for the same script, it was necessary to conduct two separate searches. While in the first one, all the mini-scripts were searched with filter TI title; in the second one, the first two mini-scripts were found with filter AB and the third one only with the filter TI. Finally, the two searches were combined through the command “Search with OR”.101
PsycArticles6
PsycInfo132
Pubmed(((alexithym* [Title/Abstract] OR “emotion* regulation” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotion* disregulation” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotion* self-monitor*” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotion recognition” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotional intelligence” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotional empathy” [Title/Abstract] OR “Toronto alexithymia scale” [Title/Abstract] OR “alexithymia questionnaire for children” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotional regulation questionnaire” [Title/Abstract])) AND ((“eating behav*” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotional* eating” [Title/Abstract] OR “external* eating” [Title/Abstract] OR “binge eating” [Title/Abstract] OR overeating [Title/Abstract] OR obesity [Title/Abstract] OR overweight [Title/Abstract] OR “mindless eating” [Title/Abstract] OR “emotional hunger” [Title/Abstract] OR “absence of hunger” [Title/Abstract] OR “eat* to cope” [Title/Abstract] OR “unhealthy food intake” [Title/Abstract] OR “eating habit*” [Title/Abstract])) AND ((child* [Title/Abstract] OR “pre-adolescen*” [Title/Abstract] OR adolescen* [Title/Abstract] OR “school child*” [Title/Abstract]))).
The script was completed with the following filters: “Full text”, “Humans”, “English”, “Child: birth-20 years”
65
ScopusIt was used the script in Table 1.152
Web of ScienceSince Web of Sciences does not allow the filter “TITLE-ABS-KEY” to be used simultaneously for the same script, it was necessary to conduct two separate searches. The first two mini-scripts (emotional regulation and eating behaviors) were found with filter TOPIC, and the third one was found with filter TITLE.226
Table 2. Cross-sectional studies investigating the relationship between Overeating Behavior and Emotion Regulation.
Table 2. Cross-sectional studies investigating the relationship between Overeating Behavior and Emotion Regulation.
Author (Year)CountrySampleInstrument 1 (Overeating Behavior)Instrument 2 (Emotion Regulation)Relation Between Variables
NAge (Years)
M (SD)
Gender (Males)Typology of PopulationCorrelation and RegressionDifferences between Groups
Bayraktar, 2015 [57]Turkey401Range of age:
18–19
122
(30.4%)
AdolescentsEAT-26DERSAERS and EB presented a significant positive correlation
Book & Berant, 2013 [58]Israel90Patients
16.70
(1.16)
/
Controls
16.82
(1.30)
0 (0%)45 HC (BMI < 25) and 45 OWA (BMI ≥ 25) or OA (BMI ≥ 30)EAT-26NMRDDE predicted the ability to RNEEB that predicted higher susceptibility to obesity
Cuesta-Zamora, González-Martíb, & García-Lópeza, 2018 [59]Spain382 preadolescents
/
380
adolescents
Preadolescents
10.55
(0.60)
/
Adolescents
13.53
(1.25)
Preadolescents
184
(48.2%)
/
Adolescents 198
(52%)
Normal populationEDI-3TEIQue-ASFTEIQue-ASF presented a significant negative correlation both with EDI-3 and its subscales
(BD-EDI-3, DT-EDI-3 and B-EDI-3).
EI emerged as a significant predictor of BD-EDI-3, B-EDI-3 and EDI-3-TS both in boys and girls
Czaja, Rief, & Hilbert, 2009 [60]Germany127 (65 LOC+ and 62 LOC-)LOC+
10.62
(1.46)
/
LOC−
10.92
(1.47)
25 LOC+ (41.7%)
/
27 LOC− (45%)
Normal population DEBQKFEEL-KJr FEEL-KJ and DEBQ-K (ExE and EmE) presented a significant positive correlationLOC+ children reported higher use of mERs for anxiety, anger, and sadness than LOC-
Ferrer, Green, & Oh, 2017 [61]USA155614.45
(1.62)
778
(50%)
Adolescents27-item dietary screenerFour-item version of the ERQERQ-ES was a predictor of EmE, lower F&V consumption frequency and greater HF consumption frequency
Goossens, Van Malderen, Varn Durme, et al., 2016 [62]Belgium52815.08
(1.59)
155
(29.4 %)
Normal adolescentsChEDE-Q
FEEL-KJ LOC+ vs. LOC−
Regarding mERs, LOC+ adolescents reported more NEG, S-DEV, and RUM, WIT than LOC− ones.
Regarding aERs, LOC− used, compared to LOC+, reported more use of P-OA, DIS, PIGH, ACC, CPS
Gouveia, Canavarro, Moreira, 2018 [63]Portugal24514.49
(1.71)
Normal adolescents 62
(51.7%)
/
Adolescents in nutritional treatment
62
(49.6%)
136 OWA and 109 OA
/
125 (51%) patients and 120 (49%) from the community
DEBQDERS-SFDER resulted to be positively correlated with EmENo significant differences were found for difficulties in emotion regulation, or emotional eating
Isasi, Ostrovsky, Wills, 2013 [64]USA60212.7
(0.8)
253
(42%)
StudentsYAQ (Dietary assessment)
/
YRBS (Physical Activity)
/
A 6-item scale for S-EHFC
/
A 6-item scale for S-EBPA
An instrument to assess ER, including: a scale for soothability
/
A 5-item scale for sadness management
/
A 4-item scale for anger management)
ER was positively correlated to S-EBPA and S-EHFC that was related to F&V intake
ER was a direct predictive factor of snack/junk food intake
Laghi, Bianchi. Pompili, 2018 [65]Italy80417.45
(1.02)
404
(50.3 %)
StudentsBESDERS (Scale “Lack of emotional awareness” was considered as an independent measure)ED and LEA resulted to be not only positively correlated to BE, but also its significant predictors. Regarding LEA, it is true only at high levels of NCT
Laghi, Liga, Pompili, 2019 [66]Italy100417.9
(0.8)
395
(39.34%)
StudentsBESERQBES-TS and ERQ-ES were positively correlated.
ERQ-CR and ERQ-ES were negatively and positively, respectively, predictors of BE
Li, 2018 [67]China78417.12
(1.32)
382
(48.72%)
StudentsEAT-26 (Chinese version)WLEISEAT-26 and WLEIS were positively correlated.
EI was a negative predictor of EaDR.
Lu, Tao, Hou, 2016 [68]China4316Range of age:
11–17
-StudentsFFQ
/
DEBQ
ERQERQ-CR
In boys, it was found a positive correlation with N-DDP. In girls, a positive correlation with N-DDP and a negative one with E-RDP.
ERQ-ES
In boys, it was found a positive correlation with EmE. In girls, a positive correlation with both EmE
and E-RDP and a negative one with N-DDP
In girls, E-RDP was predicted by:
ERQ-ES → EmE → E-RDP
McEwen, Flouri, 2008 [69]England20314.04
(1.91)
78
(38.4%)
StudentsEAT-26DERSDER and EaDS were positively correlated
Mills, Newman, Cossar et al., 2014 [70]United Kingdom22215.38
(1.05)
123
(55.4%)
StudentsEAT-2621 items-REQDER and DEa were positively correlated
DER was a predictor of DEa
Minnich, Gordon, 2017 [71]USA134418.97
(1.24)
481
(35%)
StudentsBESTAS-20TAS-20 and BES were positive correlated
Percinel, Ozbaran, Kose et al., 2016 [72]Turkey6030 patients
14.57
(2.07)
/
30 controls
14.73
(1.85)
Patients
4
(13.3%)
/
Controls
4
(13.3%)
30 patients EO
/
30 Controls
DERS DERS-TS and all its subscale were significantly higher in EO group compared to HC
Shank, Tanofsky-Kraff, Kelly et al., 2019 [73]USA20013.1
(2.8)
92
(46%)
Community sampleEDE interview
/
Emotional Eating Scale for Children and Adolescents
/
Eating in the Absence of Hunger Questionnaire
AQCAlexithymia was a predictive factor of emotional eating, eating-related psychopathology, and eating in the absence of hunger.
Tan, Xin, Wang, 2017 [74]China2042OWA
15.06 (1.95)
/
OA
14.50 (1.93)
/
HC
14.92 (1.80)
OWA
500
(71.43%)
/
OA
230
(71.65%)
/
HC
730
(71.50%)
Students
OWA: 700 (34.28%)
/
OA: 321
(15.71%)
/
HC: 1021
(50%)
CERQGreater S-B and RUM were predictors of higher BMI, while greater ACC and “P-REF were predictors of lower BMI.OA obtained the highest scores on both S-B and RUM scales, and OWA obtained higher scores than HC.
HC obtained the highest scores on ACC, P-REF, and P-REA
Vandewalle, Moens, Braet, 2014 [75]Belgium11013.59
(1.64)
47
(42.7%)
Children and adolescents with obesityDEBQ-child versionFEEL-KJmERs and EmE were positively correlated
mERs and Adjusted BMI were negatively correlated
mERs, but not aERs, was a significant predictor of EmE
Wong, Ling, Chang, 2014 [76]Taiwan102816.1
(0.7)
420
(40.86%)
Students divided into 4 groups: UW, NW, OW and OAEAT-26Adolescent Emotional Intelligence ScaleEAT-26-TS was positively correlated with EI and its subscales EA, EE and EU
All the abbreviations can be find in the Abbreviations/Nomenclature section at the end of the manuscript.
Table 3. Longitudinal studies investigating the relationship between Overeating Behavior and Emotion Regulation.
Table 3. Longitudinal studies investigating the relationship between Overeating Behavior and Emotion Regulation.
Author
(Year)
CountrySampleInstrument 1 (Overeating Behavior)Instrument 2 (Emotion Regulation)Relation between Variables
NAge (Years)
M (SD)
Gender (Males)Typology of PopulationCorrelation and RegressionDifferences between Groups
Goldschmidt, Lavender, Hipwell et al., 2016 [77]USA5883 assessment
16.5
(0.4)
/
17.3
(0.4)
/
18.3
(0.3)
0
(0%)
Normal sample EAT-26Two DERS-subscales:
EA and ERS
mERs and LEA were positively correlated with LOCef at every year of age.
mERs and LEA in adolescents aged 17 and 18 independently predicted LOCef at 18 age
Harrist, Hubbs-Tait, Topham, 2013 [78]USA740Range of age
7–9 years old
375
(50.7%)
StudentsDEBQCEMSRegarding Anger and Worry, each ERea and EI measures were significantly or marginally correlated with ExE and EmE.
Regarding Anger and Worry, each measure of ERea and EI were predictive of change in ExE and EmE
Orihuela, Mrug, Boggiano, 2017 [79]USA75Waves 2
15
(0.97)
Waves 3
16
(1.11)
37
(47%)
Students K-PEMSERICAWave 3-ER were negatively correlated with CEM, CoEM at Wave 2, and CEM and EEM at Wave 3.
Poorer Wave 2-ER predicted CoEM and CEM at Wave 3
Shriver, Dollars, Lawless et al., 2019 [80]USA1533 assessment
15
/
16
/
19
67
(44%)
Normal sample
68.8% HW
/
15.6% OW
/
15.6% OB
TFEQERCAER at 15-year was positively correlated with DR at 16-year and negatively correlated with EmE at 16-year. Furthermore, DR at 16-year and EmE at 16-year were both positively correlated with BF at 19-year.
ER at 15-year was a positive predictor of DR at 16-year and a negative predictor of EmE at 16-year. EmE was a positive predictor of BF at 19 years.
Vandewalle, Moens, Beyers et al., 2016 [81]Belgium81First assessment
12.86
(1.65)
/
Follow up
4 months later
36
(44.4%)
StudentsDEBQFEEL-KJRegarding the “Level”, mERs was positively related to EmE, but concerning the “Change”, mERs was not related to EmE.
van Strien, Beijers, Smeekens et al., 2019 [82]Netherlands and Finland105Wave 1
12.38
(0.28)
Wave 2
16.27
(0.30)
52
(50%)
Community sampleDEBQ-CERQ
/
TAS-20
EmE at 12-year was correlated with TAS-20 DIF and tas-20 DDF
All the abbreviations can be find in the Abbreviations/Nomenclature section at the end of the manuscript.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Favieri, F.; Marini, A.; Casagrande, M. Emotional Regulation and Overeating Behaviors in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/bs11010011

AMA Style

Favieri F, Marini A, Casagrande M. Emotional Regulation and Overeating Behaviors in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Behavioral Sciences. 2021; 11(1):11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/bs11010011

Chicago/Turabian Style

Favieri, Francesca, Andrea Marini, and Maria Casagrande. 2021. "Emotional Regulation and Overeating Behaviors in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review" Behavioral Sciences 11, no. 1: 11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/bs11010011

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop