Next Article in Journal
Nucleus Accumbens Functional Connectivity with the Frontoparietal Network Predicts Subsequent Change in Body Mass Index for American Children
Next Article in Special Issue
Exercise Intensity May Not Moderate the Acute Effects of Functional Circuit Training on Cognitive Function: A Randomized Crossover Trial
Previous Article in Journal
Association between Chronic Pain and Alterations in the Mesolimbic Dopaminergic System
Previous Article in Special Issue
Distinct Effects of Acute Aerobic Exercise on Declarative Memory and Procedural Memory Formation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Free-Weight Resistance Exercise Is More Effective in Enhancing Inhibitory Control than Machine-Based Training: A Randomized, Controlled Trial

Department of Sports Medicine, Goethe University, Ginnheimer Landstraße 39, 60487 Frankfurt am Main, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 10 September 2020 / Revised: 28 September 2020 / Accepted: 28 September 2020 / Published: 3 October 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Studying Brain Activity in Sports Performance)

Abstract

:
Resistance exercise has been demonstrated to improve brain function. However, the optimal workout characteristics are a matter of debate. This randomized, controlled trial aimed to elucidate differences between free-weight (REfree) and machine-based (REmach) training with regard to their ability to acutely enhance cognitive performance (CP). A total of n = 46 healthy individuals (27 ± 4 years, 26 men) performed a 45-min bout of REfree (military press, barbell squat, bench press) or REmach (shoulder press, leg press, chest press). Pre- and post-intervention, CP was examined using the Stroop test, Trail Making Test and Digit Span test. Mann–Whitney U tests did not reveal between-group differences for performance in the Digit Span test, Trail Making test and the color and word conditions of the Stroop test (p > 0.05). However, REfree was superior to REmach in the Stroop color-word condition (+6.3%, p = 0.02, R = 0.35). Additionally, REfree elicited pre-post changes in all parameters except for the Digit Span test and the word condition of the Stroop test while REmach only improved cognitive performance in part A of the Trail Making test. Using free weights seems to be the more effective RE method to acutely improve cognitive function (i.e., inhibitory control). The mechanisms of this finding merit further investigation.

1. Introduction

For millennia, resistance exercise (RE) has represented an essential type of physical training, evoking manifold benefits in a plethora of populations and conditions [1,2,3,4,5]. For instance, RE has been shown to counteract age-related sarcopenia [1], reduce arterial blood pressure [2], improve recovery from musculoskeletal disorders [3] and increase sports-related motor performance [4,5]. Most studies examining the effects of RE predominantly focused on peripheral adaptations in the soft tissue, i.e., the cross-sectional area of the skeletal muscles. However, beyond this, RE also appears to impact brain function. Pooling the results from 12 trials, a recent multilevel meta-analysis concluded that a single training bout acutely increases specific sub-domains such as inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility [6].
Despite the established knowledge about the general interaction between RE and brain function, the moderators driving the changes in cognitive performance (CP) are a matter of debate. Studies addressing this issue have yielded heterogeneous results and a high level of uncertainty regarding the influence of modifiable (e.g., intensity, duration) and non-modifiable (e.g., age) factors [6]. Besides these quantitative variables, Pesce [7] suggested to focus on qualitative characteristics. In fact, studies using electroencephalography or near-infrared spectroscopy have revealed that cortical activations patterns and hemodynamics vary as a function of task complexity (e.g., stable repetitive monotonous vs. variable or alternating, coordinatively challenging activities; [8,9,10,11]). The question arises if changes such as higher perfusion and/or a facilitation of specific sensorimotor cortices could be beneficial for cognitive performance.
Some researchers investigated the impact of varying task complexity on brain function. In adolescents attending an elite performance school, Budde et al. [12] found a 10-min, coordinatively challenging exercise session (e.g., bouncing one or two balls, alternatingly and/or simultaneously, concurrently passing balls with foot and hand) to acutely enhance executive function to a greater degree than a traditional intervention of identical duration (general moderate-intensity exercise without major coordinative demands). Gallotta et al. [13] made opposite findings. They instructed primary school children (8–11 years) to either participate in a normal school lesson lacking physical activity, a regular non-enriched physical education session (e.g., walking, running, skipping) or a coordinatively enriched physical education session (e.g., basketball mini-games with varying rules fostering decision-making). Interestingly, the coordinatively challenging session was least effective.
Hitherto, no study has examined the cognitive effects of task complexity in RE. However, in a pioneering trial, Carraro et al. [14] found free-weights training to more strongly increase arousal (a psychological state of being attentive) than machine-based training. As exercise-induced gains in arousal are linked to better CP [15], the objective of the present study was to test the hypothesis that free-weight RE would enhance cognition more effectively than a quantitatively matched intervention with machines.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Ethical Standards and Study Design

The study is nested within the COINS (COgnition and INjury in Sports) network project. A two-armed, randomized, controlled trial was performed. It was prospectively registered at the German Register of Clinical Trials (DRKS00022281) and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki including its recent modification of Fortaleza (2013). Ethical approval (ref. 2020-39) was obtained on 7 June 2020 from the ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Sports Sciences of Goethe University Frankfurt, and each volunteer provided written informed consent.
Participants were randomly allocated to two groups: (1) resistance training with free weights (REfree) or (2) resistance training with machines (REmach). Prior to and after the intervention, cognitive performance was assessed. All participants visited the laboratory twice with 5 to 7 days in-between. While the first session served for familiarization with the tests and the training equipment, the actual intervention was performed on the second appointment. Randomization was performed using the software package “BiAS for Windows”, version 9.05 (Goethe University, Frankfurt, Germany).

2.2. Participants

Healthy adults (n = 46, 27 ± 4 years, 26 men) were recruited between June and August 2020 by means of personal contact and poster advertising at the local university campus. They had to engage in a minimum of five sporting hours per week. Exclusion criteria were (a) severe orthopedic, cardiovascular, pulmonary, neurological, psychiatric or inflammatory rheumatic diseases; (b) pregnancy or nursing period; (c) analgesic intake during the trial or in the 48 h prior to study enrollment; (d) impairments in color vision and (e) history of surgery or trauma in the lower extremity.

2.3. Intervention

The interventions and related procedures have been validated in a previous trial assessing the impact of RE on arousal [14]. When allocated to REmach, the participants completed a resistance training session using conventional training machines (shoulder press, leg press and chest press). Exercises for individuals in REfree (military press, back squat, bench press) were executed with a barbell. Repetition numbers, set durations and relative intensities were identical in both groups. Participants performed four sets with the descending pyramid system (six, eight, ten, and twelve repetitions, weight progressively decreasing from set to set). To standardize movement velocity, the concentric phases lasted one second and the eccentric phases two seconds. Rest duration between-sets was 115 s. Weights were determined according to the individual 6-repetition maximums (6RM) of the respective exercises, which was determined during the familiarization session [14]. Prior to testing, participants performed two warm-up sets at 25% and 50% of the anticipated 1RM [16]. These sets, together with previous training records, were used to determine the starting weight. In the actual measurements, weight was increased once six correct repetitions had been performed. The interval between sets was 180 s [17]. Only assessments with a maximum of 5 attempts were considered valid [18]. In our sample, most participants required 1–3 sets (range 1–4 sets). All workouts were monitored by investigators holding an academic degree in Sports Sciences. Verbal feedback was continuously given if errors in movement execution were observed.

2.4. Outcomes

Immediately before and after the intervention, markers of CP were measured. To prevent practice effects, three strategies were used [19]. Firstly, in the familiarization session, all participants completed three repetitions of each test. Secondly, prior to initiating the actual assessments, one warm-up trial was performed. Finally, no identical tests forms (different color/number orders) were applied. Testing order was randomized and the delay between the end of the experimental condition and the start of the post-measurements was standardized amounting to 60 s.
Assessments included three tests. The Stroop task has three parts. In the first and second which both capture attention, the participants were required to name words written (Sw) or colors (Sc) displayed on a sheet as quickly as possible. The third section, a measure of inhibition control (Scw), consisted of color words presented incongruently (e.g., “green” written in red or “blue written in yellow). Here, the participants needed to name the color of the word while ignoring the letters. In all three parts, time until task completion was documented. The Stroop test exhibits high reliability (ICC: 0.82) and internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.93 to 0.97) [20].
The Trail Making test (TMT) has two parts. In part A, the participants were required to connect successive numbers using a pen at maximal possible speed (e.g., 1–4). In part B, numbers and letters (e.g., 1 to a to 2 to b) were to be linked in an alternating manner. Similar to the Stroop test, time needed for completion was recorded. The results are suggested to represent a measure of visual screening/attention (TMT-A) and cognitive flexibility/working memory (TMT-B). High reliability (ICC: 0.81 to 0.86) and construct validity of the TMT have been demonstrated [21,22].
In the Digit Span (DS) test, two conditions were performed. In the first, the participants had to recall and repeat increasing amounts of numbers read to them. Initially, four numbers were to be memorized. In case of success, five numbers were named. For each step, two repetitions were performed and one or zero points awarded depending on recall success. The test ends if both trials were failed. The second condition was identical to the first, but the numbers had to be repeated in reversed order (e.g., 2,4,7,9 becomes 9,7,4,2). Both test parts and the composite score were linked to short-term and working memory [23]. The DS test is reliable for repeated measurements (r = 0.73; [24]).
Prior to starting outcome assessments, subjective arousal (Likert scale from “0—not activated” to “6—highly activated”), concentration (10 cm Visual Analogue Scale, 0 = not concentrated at all to 10 = highly concentrated) and heart rate (heart rate monitor) were assessed. Additionally, after the interventions, the participants stated their rate of perceived exertion (6–20 RPE scale [25]) as well as enjoyment of the intervention (Likert scale from “0—not fun at all” to “6—most possible fun”).

2.5. Data Processing and Statistics

Kolmogorov–Smirnov analyses revealed violations of the normalcy assumption. To identify relative pre-post changes of CP within groups, we constructed parameter-free 95% confidence intervals [26] while between-group differences were detected by means of the Mann–Whitney U test. With regard to the latter, in case of significance, effect sizes (R = Z/sqr(n)) were computed according to Rosenthal [27] and interpreted as small (R = 0.1), moderate (R = 0.3), large (R = 0.5) or very large (R > 0.7). To reveal potential moderators of the intervention effect (age, sex, BMI, physical activity volume, arousal, exercise enjoyment, subjective exertion during exercise), we used Kendall’s tau correlation. p-values < 0.05 were considered to be significant in all calculations, the software used was “BiAS for Windows”, version 9.05 (Goethe University, Frankfurt, Germany).

3. Results

Both groups showed comparable cognitive performance at baseline and were not different regarding age, sex, BMI, physical activity and pre-exercise arousal (p > 0.05, Table 1). All participants completed the disposed interventions without the occurrence of adverse effects.

3.1. Exercise Effects on Cognitive Performance

No differences between groups were found for DS, TMT as well as Sc and Sw (p < 0.05, Table 2). However, analysis of the confidence intervals revealed that REfree increased TMT-A (+34.6%), TMT-B (+24.3%), Sw (+2.9%) and Sc (+4.2%) performance relative to baseline, while REmach only improved TMT-A (+23.5%, Figure 1 and Figure 2). Additionally, REfree improved Scw both pre to post according to the confidence intervals (+9.6%) and in comparison to REmach (+6.3%, p = 0.02, R = 0.35; moderate effect size, Figure 2).

3.2. Potential Moderators

Exhaustion, enjoyment as well as changes in arousal and heart rate were not different between groups (p < 0.05). Additionally, none of these variables correlated with changes in cognitive performance (p < 0.05).

4. Discussion

A wealth of evidence supports the beneficial short-term impact of resistance exercise on cognitive performance [6]. However, so far, research on the moderators of this effect predominantly focused on training intensity [28,29,30,31,32] while the relevance of exercise characteristics, i.e., the type of RE, had been scarcely examined. In detail, available studies mostly investigated machined-based (e.g., [33,34,35,36]) or a combination of machined-based and free-weight RE (e.g., [37,38,39,40,41,42]) but none provided a direct comparison of different regimes. The present trial reveals that the magnitude of exercise-induced CP improvements following free-weight RE is substantially larger when compared to machine-based training.
Several mechanisms potentially mediating short-term CP changes following RE have been discussed. A previous study demonstrated free-weight exercise to induce higher arousal levels when compared to machine-based training [14]. However, this finding could not be replicated in our trial as arousal was not different between groups. A potential reason for this could be that we used a 10-point Likert scale, while Carraro and colleagues had applied the Felt Arousal Scale with six points. Irrespectively, the impact of arousal as an effect trigger in different RE types requires further examination. Cerebral blood flow represents another factor suspected to explain activity-induced CP improvements. Experiments using aerobic exercise showed an intensity-dependent increase of brain perfusion until the ventilatory threshold [43]. Only few studies examined this association for RE, but initial evidence points towards the existence of blood flow fluctuations during exercise which may facilitate CP [43,44]. Together with or in absence of perfusion changes, altered cortical activation patterns may be triggered by RE. In both, younger adults and individuals with mild cognitive impairment, increased P3 amplitudes (linked to activity and cognitive functioning) were detected immediately following an exercise bout [45,46]. Finally, exercise generally modulates the production of cortisol and moderately elevated concentrations of the stress hormone can enhance working memory [47]. Evidence, however, is ambiguous for RE and both acute decreases [38] and increases [48] have been reported. While all these findings are intriguing, none of the mechanistic studies particularly addressed the question as to whether the magnitude of the metabolic, circulatory and electrophysiological changes is dependent on the RE characteristics (i.e., RE with free weights vs. RE with machines). Based on the available evidence showing an association of task complexity and cortical activation [8,9,10] and our data, we speculate that training with free weights may require higher levels of concentration, sensory processing and motor coordination and with this, higher or more complex brain activation levels than similar interventions using training machines.
RE with free-weights, in contrast to machine-based RE, improved Sc and TMT-B performance but no significant between-group differences were found. Some uncertainty therefore remains regarding a higher effectiveness of free-weight exercise in enhancing simple cognitive functions such as attention, processing speed, reaction time and visual scanning. Contrarily, a moderate effect indicating superiority of RE with free weights was found for inhibitory control (Scw). This finding seems plausible as exercising with a barbell requires constant fine and gross motor adaptations and corrections of the ongoing movement. Improvements of inhibitory control may be of relevance in a variety of contexts. Previous studies found that poor performance during response inhibition tasks is associated with the risk of future falls in community-dwelling older adults and individuals with neurological diseases [49,50,51,52]. Health professionals may hence consider incorporating free-weight exercises when designing exercise programs for seniors or patients. Beyond this, using free- or bodyweight resistance exercise could also be of value as a warm-up for athletes from both an injury-preventive and a performance-related perspective. Giesche et al. [53] examined the relation between inhibitory control and dynamic postural control during unplanned single-leg landings in healthy active individuals. Participants with low Scw values had higher center of pressure path lengths, which is indicative of a possible stability deficit. With regard to sporting success, several studies [54,55] have shown an association between inhibitory control and game performance sports.
Although machine-based paradigms are frequently used in studies examining the effects of RE on CP, Rmach did only improve TMT-A performance but, as said, had no effects on measures of executive function (TMT-B, Scw) or other tests capturing lower order cognitive skills. Compared to Sc and Sw, the TMT-A places higher demands to visual search [56], and hence, it may be speculated that REmach rather improves this skill than processing speed and reaction time in general.
Our results call for further research. (1) From a general point of view, gaining insight into the mechanisms explaining the higher effectiveness of Rfree in increasing CP should be a paramount objective of future trials. (2) In detail, we found a superiority of Rfree in inhibitory control, which is typically classified as a higher-order cognitive function. As the analysis of the confidence intervals also revealed pre-post improvements in lower-order functions (e.g., processing speed and visual scanning), which did not occur following machine-based training but failed statistical significance in the group comparison, we recommend follow-up experiments focusing on this area. Finally, it would be of interest to compare the traditional RE methods studied here with newer approaches such as high-intensity functional training, which mixes characteristics of endurance and resistance exercise.
Some limitations need to be discussed. Analysis of our data showed that it was non-normally distributed. As the choice of the sample size had been based on a biometric calculation for parametric testing and as non-parametric testing is more conservative, our trial may have had a slight lack of power preventing the detection of small-magnitude differences. When considering the confidence intervals, this, e.g., may relate to performance in the Trail Making test. Two other issues relate to the comparability of the interventions. Firstly, although we matched training intensity relative to individual maximal strength, exercising with free weights is known to achieve higher muscle activations [57]. Secondly, we did not use a linear encoder to control movement velocity. Minor differences may hence have occurred between the training regimes.

5. Conclusions

Resistance exercise performed with free weights is more effective in acutely increasing inhibitory control than the use of conventional training machines. It may hence be of interest for both, elderly individuals aiming to prevent falls or athletes seeking to improve performance.

Author Contributions

J.W.; data curation, J.W., V.S., S.U.; formal analysis, J.W.; investigation, J.W., V.S., S.U.; methodology, J.W.; project administration, J.W.; supervision, J.W.; visualization, J.W.; writing—original draft, J.W.; writing—review and editing, J.W., V.S., S.U. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Beckwée, D.; Delaere, A.; Aelbrecht, S.; Baert, V.; Beaudart, C.; Bruyère, O.; De Saint-Hubert, M.; Bautmans, I. Exercise Interventions for the Prevention and Treatment of Sarcopenia. A Systematic Umbrella Review. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2019, 23, 494–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Cornelissen, V.; Fagard, R.H.; Coeckelberghs, E.; Vanhees, L. Impact of Resistance Training on Blood Pressure and Other Cardiovascular Risk Factors. Hypertension 2011, 58, 950–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Kristensen, J.; Franklyn-Miller, A. Resistance training in musculoskeletal rehabilitation: A systematic review. Br. J. Sports Med. 2011, 46, 719–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Young, W.B. Transfer of Strength and Power Training to Sports Performance. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 2006, 1, 74–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Harries, S.K.; Lubans, D.R.; Callister, R. Resistance training to improve power and sports performance in adolescent athletes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2012, 15, 532–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wilke, J.; Giesche, F.; Klier, K.; Vogt, L.; Herrmann, E.; Banzer, W. Acute Effects of Resistance Exercise on Cognitive Function in Healthy Adults: A Systematic Review with Multilevel Meta-Analysis. Sports Med. 2019, 49, 905–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Pesce, C. Shifting the Focus from Quantitative to Qualitative Exercise Characteristics in Exercise and Cognition Research. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2012, 34, 766–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Jäncke, L.; Himmelbach, M.; Shah, N.J.; Zilles, K. The Effect of Switching between Sequential and Repetitive Movements on Cortical Activation. NeuroImage 2000, 12, 528–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Verstynen, T.V.; Diedrichsen, J.; Albert, N.; Aparicio, P.; Ivry, R.B. Ipsilateral Motor Cortex Activity During Unimanual Hand Movements Relates to Task Complexity. J. Neurophysiol. 2005, 93, 1209–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Holper, L.; Biallas, M.; Wolf, M. Task complexity relates to activation of cortical motor areas during uni- and bimanual performance: A functional NIRS study. NeuroImage 2009, 46, 1105–1113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Leff, D.R.; Orihuela-Espina, F.; Elwell, C.; Athanasiou, T.; Delpy, D.T.; Darzi, A.W.; Yang, G.Z. Assessment of the cerebral cortex during motor task behaviours in adults: A systematic review of functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) studies. NeuroImage 2011, 54, 2922–2936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Budde, H.; Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Pietraßyk-Kendziorra, S.; Ribeiro, P.; Tidow, G. Acute coordinative exercise improves attentional performance in adolescents. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 441, 219–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Gallotta, M.C.; Guidetti, L.; Franciosi, E.; Emerenziani, G.P.; Bonavolontà, V.; Baldari, C. Effects of Varying Type of Exertion on Children’s Attention Capacity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2012, 44, 550–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Carraro, A.; Paoli, A.; Gobbi, E. Affective response to acute resistance exercise: A comparison among machines and free weights. Sport Sci. Health 2018, 14, 283–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lambourne, K.; Tomporowski, P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 2010, 1341, 12–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Saeterbakken, A.H.; Fimland, M.S. Electromyographic Activity and 6RM Strength in Bench Press on Stable and Unstable Surfaces. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2013, 27, 1101–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Drinkwater, E.J.; Lawton, T.W.; Lindsell, R.P.; Pyne, D.B.; Hunt, P.H.; McKenna, M.J. Training leading to repetition failure enhances bench press strength gains in elite junior athletes. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2005, 19, 382–388. [Google Scholar]
  18. Wilke, J.; Kaiser, S.; Niederer, D.; Kalo, K.; Engeroff, T.; Morath, C.; Vogt, L.; Banzer, W. Effects of high-intensity functional circuit training on motor function and sport motivation in healthy, inactive adults. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2018, 29, 144–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Hausknecht, J.P.; Halpert, J.A.; Di Paolo, N.T.; Gerrard, M.O.M. Retesting in selection: A meta-analysis of coaching and practice effects for tests of cognitive ability. J. Appl. Psychol. 2007, 92, 373–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Wöstmann, N.M.; Aichert, D.S.; Costa, A.; Rubia, K.; Möller, H.-J.; Ettinger, U. Reliability and plasticity of response inhibition and interference control. Brain Cogn. 2013, 81, 82–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Wagner, S.; Helmreich, I.; Dahmen, N.; Lieb, K.; Tadic, A. Reliability of Three Alternate Forms of the Trail Making Tests A and B. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2011, 26, 314–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Sánchez-Cubillo, I.; A Periañez, J.; Adrover-Roig, D.; Rodríguez-Sánchez, J.; Rios-Lago, M.; Tirapu, J.; Barcelo, F. Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: Role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009, 15, 438–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Unsworth, N.; Engle, R. On the division of short-term and working memory: An examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychol. Bull. 2007, 133, 1038–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Youngjohn, J.R.; Larrabee, G.J.; Crook, T.H. Test-retest reliability of computerized, everyday memory measures and traditional memory tests. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1992, 6, 276–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chen, M.; Fan, X.; Moe, S.T. Criterion-related validity of the Borg ratings of perceived exertion scale in healthy individuals: A meta-analysis. J. Sports Sci. 2002, 20, 873–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Sachs, L. Angewandte Statistik: Anwendung Statistischer Methoden, 10th ed.; Springer-Verlag GmbH: Heidelberg, Germany, 2002; p. 893. (In German) [Google Scholar]
  27. Rosenthal, R. Applied Social Research Methods Series. In Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research, Rev. ed.; SAGE Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1991; Volume 6. [Google Scholar]
  28. Brisswalter, J.; Collardeau, M.; René, A. Effects of Acute Physical Exercise Characteristics on Cognitive Performance. Sports Med. 2002, 32, 555–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. McMorris, T.; Sproule, J.; Turner, A.; Hale, B.J. Acute, intermediate intensity exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: A meta-analytical comparison of effects. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 102, 421–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Chang, Y.; Labban, J.; Gapin, J.; Etnier, J. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012, 1453, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Loprinzi, P.D. Intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory function: Considerations for the timing of exercise and the type of memory. Heal. Promot. Perspect. 2018, 8, 255–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Moreau, D.; Chou, E. The Acute Effect of High-Intensity Exercise on Executive Function: A Meta-Analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 14, 734–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Pontifex, M.B.; Hillman, C.H.; Fernhall, B.; Thompson, K.M.; Valentini, T.A. The Effect of Acute Aerobic and Resistance Exercise on Working Memory. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2009, 41, 927–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Hsieh, S.-S.; Chang, Y.-K.; Hung, T.-M.; Fang, C.-L. The effects of acute resistance exercise on young and older males’ working memory. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2016, 22, 286–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Tsukamoto, H.; Suga, T.; Takenaka, S.; Takeuchi, T.; Tanaka, D.; Hamaoka, T.; Hashimoto, T.; Isaka, T. An acute bout of localized resistance exercise can rapidly improve inhibitory control. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0184075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Dunsky, A.; Abu-Rukun, M.; Tsuk, S.; Dwolatzky, T.; Carasso, R.; Netz, Y. The effects of a resistance vs. an aerobic single session on attention and executive functioning in adults. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0176092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Alves, C.R.R.; Gualano, B.; Takao, P.P.; Avakian, P.; Fernandes, R.M.; Morine, D.; Takito, M.Y. Effects of Acute Physical Exercise on Executive Functions: A Comparison Between Aerobic and Strength Exercise. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2012, 34, 539–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Tsai, C.-L.; Wang, C.-H.; Pan, C.-Y.; Chen, F.-C.; Huang, T.-H.; Chou, F.-Y. Executive function and endocrinological responses to acute resistance exercise. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Chang, H.; Kim, K.; Jung, Y.-J.; Kato, M. Effects of acute high-Intensity resistance exercise on cognitive function and oxygenation in prefrontal cortex. J. Exerc. Nutr. Biochem. 2017, 21, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Chang, Y.-K.; Ku, P.-W.; Tomporowski, P.D.; Chen, F.-T.; Huang, C.-C. Effects of Acute Resistance Exercise on Late-Middle-Age Adults’ Goal Planning. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2012, 44, 1773–1779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Chang, Y.-K.; Tsai, C.-L.; Huang, C.-C.; Wang, C.-C.; Chu, I.-H. Effects of acute resistance exercise on cognition in late middle-aged adults: General or specific cognitive improvement? J. Sci. Med. Sport 2014, 17, 51–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Johnson, L.; Addamo, P.K.; Raj, I.S.; Borkoles, E.; Wyckelsma, V.L.; Cyarto, E.; Polman, R. An Acute Bout of Exercise Improves the Cognitive Performance of Older Adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 2016, 24, 591–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Smith, K.J.; Ainslie, P.-N. Regulation of cerebral blood flow and metabolism during exercise. Exper. Physiol. 2017, 102, 1356–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Edwards, M.R.; Martin, D.H.; Hughson, R.L. Cerebral hemodynamics and resistance exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2002, 34, 1207–1211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pedroso, R.V.; Fraga, F.J.; Pérez, C.A.; Carral, J.C.; Scarpari, L.; Santos-Galduróz, R.F. Effects of physical activity on the P300 component in elderly people: A systematic review. Psychogeriatrics 2017, 17, 479–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Herold, F.; Törpel, A.; Schega, L.; Mueller, N. Functional and/or structural brain changes in response to resistance exercises and resistance training lead to cognitive improvements—A systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 2019, 16, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Basso, J.C.; Suzuki, W.A. The Effects of Acute Exercise on Mood, Cognition, Neurophysiology, and Neurochemical Pathways: A Review. Brain Plast. 2017, 2, 127–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Arent, S.M.; Landers, D.M.; Matt, K.S.; Etnier, J.L. Dose-Response and Mechanistic Issues in the Resistance Training and Affect Relationship. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2005, 27, 92–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Chen, T.Y.; Peronto, C.L.; Edwards, J.D. Cognitive function as a prospective predictor of falls. J. Gerontol. 2012, 67, 720–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mirelman, A.; Herman, T.; Brozgol, M.; Dorfman, M.; Sprecher, E.; Schweiger, A.; Giladi, N.; Hausdorff, J.M. Executive Function and Falls in Older Adults: New Findings from a Five-Year Prospective Study Link Fall Risk to Cognition. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e40297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Nagamatsu, L.S.; Kam, J.W.Y.; Liu-Ambrose, T.; Chan, A.; Handy, T.C. Mind-wandering and falls risk in older adults. Psychol. Aging 2013, 28, 685–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Saverino, A.; Waller, D.; Rantell, K.; Parry, R.; Moriarty, E.; Playford, E.D. The Role of Cognitive Factors in Predicting Balance and Fall Risk in a Neuro-Rehabilitation Setting. PLoS ONE 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Giesche, F.; Wilke, J.; Engeroff, T.; Niederer, D.; Hohmann, H.; Vogt, L.; Banzer, W. Are biomechanical stability deficits during unplanned single-leg landings related to specific markers of cognitive function? J. Sci. Med. Sport 2020, 23, 82–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Verburgh, L.; Scherder, E.J.A.; Van Lange, P.A.M.; Oosterlaan, J. Executive Functioning in Highly Talented Soccer Players. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e91254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Huijgen, B.C.H.; Leemhuis, S.; Kok, N.M.; Verburgh, L.; Oosterlaan, J.; Elferink-Gemser, M.T.; Visscher, C. Cognitive Functions in Elite and Sub-Elite Youth Soccer Players Aged 13 to 17 Years. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0144580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Crowe, S.F. The differential contribution of mental tracking, cognitive flexibility, visual search, and motor speed to performance on parts A and B of the trail making test. J. Clin. Psychol. 1998, 54, 585–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Schick, E.E.; Coburn, J.W.; Brown, L.E.; Judelson, D.A.; Khamoui, A.V.; Tran, T.T.; Uribe, B.P. A comparison of muscle activation between a Smith Machine and free weight bench press. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2010, 24, 779–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Relative changes in the two parts (A,B) of the Trail Making test (TMT) following free-weight and machine-based resistance exercise. Displayed are medians and parameter-free 95% confidence intervals. * = difference to pre-intervention value according to 95% confidence intervals
Figure 1. Relative changes in the two parts (A,B) of the Trail Making test (TMT) following free-weight and machine-based resistance exercise. Displayed are medians and parameter-free 95% confidence intervals. * = difference to pre-intervention value according to 95% confidence intervals
Brainsci 10 00702 g001
Figure 2. Relative changes in the three conditions of the Stroop test following free-weight and machine-based resistance exercise. Displayed are medians and parameter-free 95% confidence intervals. * = difference to pre-intervention value according to 95% confidence intervals, # = significant difference of pre-post change when compared to the other group.
Figure 2. Relative changes in the three conditions of the Stroop test following free-weight and machine-based resistance exercise. Displayed are medians and parameter-free 95% confidence intervals. * = difference to pre-intervention value according to 95% confidence intervals, # = significant difference of pre-post change when compared to the other group.
Brainsci 10 00702 g002
Table 1. Characteristics of the two groups measured pre- and post-intervention.
Table 1. Characteristics of the two groups measured pre- and post-intervention.
REmachREfreeTotal PreTotal Post
PrePostPrePost
Age (yrs.)27.0 ± 4.3 27.3 ± 4.4 27.2 ± 4.3
Sex12♂, 11♀ 14♂, 9♀ 26♂, 20♀
Physical Activity (hrs./week)8.2 ± 3.8 7.8 ± 3.6 8.0 ± 3.7
BMI22.7 ± 2.5 23.8 ± 3.1 23.3 ± 2.8
Arousal (0–10)6.2 ± 1.67.1 ± 1.46.5 ± 1.57.0 ± 1.76.3 ± 1.57.1 ± 1.6
Heart rate (bpm)79.2 ± 15.5103.4 ± 17.677.3 ± 9.7104.9 ± 18.478.2 ± 12.8104.1 ± 17.8
Enjoyment (0–10) 6.8 ± 2.1 7.6 ± 2.0 7.2 ± 2.0
Subjective exertion (6–20) 15.2 ± 2.1 15.1 ± 2.2 15.2 ± 2.1
Table shows means and standard deviations. REmach = machine-based resistance training, REfree = free-weights resistance training, yrs= years, hrs= hours, bpm = beats per minute.
Table 2. Cognitive performance measured pre- and post-intervention in both groups.
Table 2. Cognitive performance measured pre- and post-intervention in both groups.
REmachREfree
PrePostPrePost
Stroop word (s)25.9 (24.6–28.1)25.9 (24.2–28.5)27.2 (24.0–30.6)25.7 (24.4–29.7)
Stroop color (s)31.2 (28.9–35.5)29.3 (26.8–33.1)33.1 (28.3–35.7)30.2 (27.5–34.9) *
Stroop color-word (s)45.9 (42.2–52.3)44.7 (40.9–48.1)49.7 (42.6–56.1)43.9 (38.3–49.0) *#
TMT-A (s)25.5 (21.2–34.3)19.5 (16.3–25.2) *28.3 (24.7–32.6)18.5 (16.6–23.2) *
TMT-B (s)34.3 (28.7–39.1)30.3 (23.5–38.6)33.7 (27.4–41.7)25.5 (21.7–38.1) *
Digit Span (pts.)11 (10–13)11 (8–12)13 (8–14)12 (10–14)
Table shows medians and (interquartile ranges). REmach = machine-based resistance training, REfree = free-weights resistance training, s = seconds, pts = points, * = difference to pre-intervention value according to 95% confidence intervals, # = significant difference of pre-post change when compared to the other group.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wilke, J.; Stricker, V.; Usedly, S. Free-Weight Resistance Exercise Is More Effective in Enhancing Inhibitory Control than Machine-Based Training: A Randomized, Controlled Trial. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 702. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10100702

AMA Style

Wilke J, Stricker V, Usedly S. Free-Weight Resistance Exercise Is More Effective in Enhancing Inhibitory Control than Machine-Based Training: A Randomized, Controlled Trial. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(10):702. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10100702

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wilke, Jan, Vanessa Stricker, and Susanne Usedly. 2020. "Free-Weight Resistance Exercise Is More Effective in Enhancing Inhibitory Control than Machine-Based Training: A Randomized, Controlled Trial" Brain Sciences 10, no. 10: 702. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10100702

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop