Next Article in Journal
Towards an Understanding of Control of Complex Rhythmical “Wavelike” Coordination in Humans
Next Article in Special Issue
Migraine Frequency Decrease Following Prolonged Medical Cannabis Treatment: A Cross-Sectional Study
Previous Article in Journal
Examination of Correlates to Health-Related Quality of Life in Individuals with Fragile X Syndrome
Previous Article in Special Issue
Efficacy of Dronabinol for Acute Pain Management in Adults with Traumatic Injury: Study Protocol of A Randomized Controlled Trial
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Do Adolescents Use Substances to Relieve Uncomfortable Sensations? A Preliminary Examination of Negative Reinforcement among Adolescent Cannabis and Alcohol Users

1
San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, CA 92120, USA
2
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, CA 92093, USA
3
Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK 74136, USA
4
Department of Community Medicine, University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK 74104, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 7 March 2020 / Revised: 1 April 2020 / Accepted: 2 April 2020 / Published: 5 April 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Cannabis: Neuropsychiatry and Its Effects on Brain and Behavior)

Abstract

:
Alcohol and cannabis use are highly prevalent among adolescents and associated with negative consequences. Understanding motivations behind substance use in youth is important for informing prevention and intervention efforts. The present study aims to examine negative reinforcement principles of substance use among adolescent cannabis and alcohol users by pairing a cue reactivity paradigm with an aversive interoceptive stimulus. Adolescents (ages 15–17), classified as controls (CTL; n = 18), cannabis and/or alcohol experimenters (CAN+ALC-EXP; n = 16), or individuals meeting clinical criteria for cannabis and/or alcohol use disorder (CAN+ALC-SUD; n = 13) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging during which they experienced an aversive interoceptive probe delivered via breathing load while simultaneously performing a cue reactivity paradigm. Participants also provided self-report ratings of how their substance use is positively or negatively reinforced. While experiencing the breathing load, CAN+ALC-SUD exhibited greater (p < 0.05) deactivation in the right amygdala, the left inferior frontal gyrus, and the left parahippocampal gyrus than CAN+ALC-EXP and CTL, who did not differ. Across all substance users, activation during the breathing load within the left parahippocampal gyrus negatively correlated with cannabis and alcohol lifetime use episodes and the left inferior frontal gyrus activity negatively correlated with lifetime alcohol use episodes. CAN+ALC-SUD reported experiencing more positive and negative reinforcement of using their substance of choice than CAN+ALC-EXP; both user groups reported higher levels of positive than negative reinforcement. Adolescents with a cannabis/alcohol use disorder demonstrate an altered response to interoceptive perturbations. However, adolescent cannabis/alcohol use does not appear to be driven by negative reinforcement, as viewing substance images did not dampen this response. Based on self-report data, the experience of positive reinforcement may be stronger for adolescents. Future studies should examine whether positive reinforcement contributes to adolescent substance use.

1. Introduction

Increased risk-taking behavior is characteristic of adolescence, a critical time period marked by significant physical, cognitive, and behavioral development [1]. A common risky behavior initiated in adolescence is the use of illicit substances. Among 12th graders, approximately 44% report having used cannabis and approximately 59% report having used alcohol in their lifetime [2]. Adolescent substance use can also evolve into a substance use disorder (SUD). For example, in 2018, 2.1% of adolescents aged 12–17 met criteria for cannabis use disorder, while 1.6% met criteria for alcohol use disorder [3]. Substance use during adolescence also increases future risk of experiencing adverse consequences related to use; early adolescent cannabis use may contribute to low educational or occupational attainment, as well as increased use and development of a use disorder in adulthood [4]. Similarly, youth who initiate drinking before age 15 are at increased risk of developing an alcohol use disorder within their lifetime compared to youth who remain abstinent until age 21 [5,6]. Given the increased risks associated with adolescent substance use, it is important to improve our understanding of the motivations behind these behaviors in order to inform SUD prevention and intervention efforts.
Altered interoceptive-related neural processing has been implicated in SUD in combination with emotion dysregulation and decision-making deficits, resulting in suboptimal behavioral adjustments and the propensity to continue drug use despite negative consequences [7,8,9,10,11,12]. To date, examination of the brain mechanisms involved in interoception and negative reinforcement has focused on adult SUD and little research has examined these concepts among adolescent substance users [13,14,15].
Interoception is a biological and psychological process by which somatosensory information from inside and outside of the body is filtered and integrated within the brain to produce an overall representation of the bodily state [16]. Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), thalamus, frontal regions, and insular cortex (IC) are components of brain circuitry essential for processing and integrating bodily afferents to generate an overall representation of the body [16,17,18]. Afferent signals pass through thalamocortical pathways to IC to be integrated with sensorimotor activity and emotional information delivered by ACC and frontal regions such as inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) [16]. This process results in complex interoceptive feeling states or emotional awareness [18] and may lead to a bodily prediction error if the experienced state differs from the expected state [19,20,21,22]. Body prediction errors motivate individuals to engage in goal-directed behavior (e.g., substance use) and either approach or avoid stimuli (e.g., substance-related stimuli) with the aim of reestablishing equilibrium [23].
Among non-substance-using individuals, frontocingulate regions, including IFG and ACC, are thought to act as a regulatory system of behavioral reactions in response to aversive stimuli [24,25]. However, among individuals with SUD this regulatory system appears altered. For example, IFG and ACC blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal reductions in response to negative interoceptive stimuli have been found to characterize young adults transitioning to stimulant use disorders [8,26] while adolescent substance users have also demonstrated an increased IFG BOLD signal in response to a negative interoceptive stimuli [27]. In general, differing patterns of ACC, IFG, and IC activation have distinguished substance users from healthy individuals [28]. These frontocingulate deficits may be linked to reduced motivation to engage in behavioral changes to reestablish equilibrium despite feeling or sensing consequences of aversive bodily stimulation [29]. In addition to interoceptive processing, poor emotion regulation, an inability to effectively reduce arousal and cope with negative emotions has been implicated in adolescent substance use and requires similar brain regions [30]. The IFG and amygdala comprise a brain circuit involved in determining the emotional significance of an external stimulus and signaling the physiological, behavioral, cognitive, and affective responses necessary to minimize the impact of unpleasant stimuli [25,30,31,32,33].
One conceptualization of SUD, based on negative reinforcement principles, posits that individuals use drugs in order to alleviate uncomfortable feelings in general (e.g., emotional dysregulation, uncomfortable interoceptive states) [34,35]. For example, dysfunctional interoceptive processing may result in substance users seeking out and consuming drugs in order to reduce uncomfortable interoceptive states. Neuroimaging research suggests that drug cues activate brain regions similar to those activated by aversive interoceptive stimuli; cannabis cues elicit activation in parahippocampal gyri and various frontal regions among non-treatment-seeking cannabis-using adolescents [36]. Adolescents who primarily use alcohol also demonstrate an exaggerated neural response within frontal regions including IFG, parahippocampus, amygdala, and posterior cingulate in response to cue images [37]. Accordingly, the present study pairs an aversive interoceptive stimulus with a cannabis and alcohol cue reactivity task during functional magnetic resonance imaging. This pairing is viewed as a proxy for negative reinforcement, allowing for the examination of whether the rewarding effects of substance images dampen the negative experience of the breathing load. Specifically, we posit that viewing rewarding drug-relevant cues will dampen the interoceptive BOLD response observed in adolescent substance users while experiencing an aversive interoceptive stimulus.
An inspiratory breathing load can be used as an aversive stimulus to induce a negative interoceptive state [38] and has previously been tested among young adult [8,39], adult [40], and adolescent substance users [27] as well as matched controls. While experiencing the breathing load, young adults with problem stimulant use show lower IFG, IC, and ACC activation compared to individuals who no longer use stimulants as well as non-using controls [8,39]. Similarly, adults with a significant history of methamphetamine use currently meeting criteria for a methamphetamine use disorder also show lower IC and ACC during the breathing load [40]. Despite these differences in brain activation, groups did not differ in their subjective ratings of the breathing load experience. Overall, the reduced activation seen in regions implicated in interoceptive processing is conceptualized as an overall diminished ability to regulate when one does not feel well, and that this inability contributes to continued substance use despite negative consequences. To date, only one study has utilized an inspiratory breathing load with adolescent substance users; these results revealed an overactivation in interoceptive regions. This inconsistent finding suggests that alterations in interoceptive processing may differ as a function of age, type of substance used, or amount of substance used.
The current study is the first to pair an aversive interoceptive stimulus with a cue reactivity paradigm to examine the role of negative reinforcement in substance use. In addition, the sample of the present study includes adolescents (ages 15–17) who report cannabis and alcohol use with and without use disorders. This will allow for the examination of negative reinforcement and interoceptive-related neural responses within diagnostically subthreshold adolescent substance users to investigate whether altered processing is simply a consequence of use or unique to adolescents experiencing functional impairments related to use (i.e., adolescents with use disorder diagnoses).
Participants included adolescents meeting criteria for either cannabis and/or alcohol use disorder, adolescents who use cannabis and alcohol but do not meet diagnostic criteria (experimenters) and healthy comparison participants. On the basis of prior work, it was hypothesized that substance users meeting diagnostic criteria compared to controls would show: (1) increased neural activation in response to the breathing load across all conditions of the cue task in brain regions involved in interoceptive processing, such as IC, ACC, and IFG, as well as regions implicated in emotion regulation, including amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus [27]; (2) increased striatal response while viewing substance images across all breathing load conditions, reflecting heightened reward responsivity to substance cues [41,42]; and (3) a blunted interoceptive neural response to the breathing load when paired with substance images (cannabis and alcohol images) suggesting exposure to a conditioned drug stimulus may help modulate reactions to internal and aversive states similar to negative reinforcement principals of drug use behavior. Additionally, adolescent substance users who did not meet criteria for SUD, referred to as “experimenters”, were included to explore whether neural differences are more pronounced in adolescent substance users who endorse substance use-related functional impairment (i.e., adolescents meeting criteria for SUD) than those who do not. Therefore, it was hypothesized that experimenters would demonstrate a neural response more similar to controls than those meeting substance use disorder criteria, suggesting that impaired brain responses are a consequence of more severe use symptomatology.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Adolescent participants (n = 47, ages 15–17) were recruited through local high schools by flyers that advertised an adolescent neuroimaging research study consisting of a clinical interview and neuroimaging session. The University of California San Diego Human Research Protections Program approved the study protocol. Adolescent participants provided assent and informed consent was obtained from one parent or legal guardian prior to study enrollment. Participants were excluded if they endorsed any of the following: (1) lifetime Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5) of Mental Disorders psychiatric disorder (other than substance use disorder, SUD); (2) current use of psychoactive medications; (3) history of major neurological or medical disorder; (4) head injuries or loss of consciousness > 5 min; (5) irremovable metal in body; (6) pregnancy; (7) non-correctable vision or hearing problems; (8) premature birth or prenatal alcohol/drug exposure; (9) left handedness; or (10) claustrophobia. Eligible participants received financial compensation for their participation.
The final sample consisted of 18 controls with very minimal histories of substance use (CTL; cannabis/alcohol maximum lifetime use episodes of 3 each, nicotine maximum lifetime use episodes of 10; 13M, 5F), 16 cannabis and alcohol experimenters (CAN+ALC-EXP; 12M, 4F), and 13 who met criteria for cannabis and/or alcohol use disorder (CAN+ALC-SUD; 9M, 4F). SUD group classification required a report of cannabis or alcohol use within the past three months, current endorsement of 2 or more DSM-5 SUD criteria for either cannabis or alcohol, and fewer than 15 lifetime uses of other drugs except for nicotine (see Table 1 for diagnostic details). On average, CAN+ALC-SUD participants reported 467 lifetime cannabis uses and 131 lifetime alcohol uses. CAN+ALC-EXP group classification required a report of no substance use history other than alcohol, cannabis, or nicotine, and no current or lifetime endorsement of DSM-5 SUD criteria. CAN+ALC-EXP reported significantly less cannabis (t(12.48) = −5.31, p < 0.001) and alcohol use (t(12.28) = −3.12, p < 0.009) than SUD but significantly more use of these substances than CTL (cannabis: t(15) = −3.46, p = 0.003; alcohol: t(15.06) = −4.29, p = 0.001) (see Table 1).

2.2. Clinical Interview

The clinical interview consisted of the Semi-Structured Assessment for Drug Dependence and Alcoholism (SSADDA; [43]) to assess for the presence of SUD and the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR) [44] to capture quantity of lifetime substance use, age of first use, and last substance use. Participants provided demographic information and a battery of self-report measures to assess characteristics related to SUD including the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale [45], the Multi-Dimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) [46], and the Michigan Nicotine Reinforcement Questionnaire (MNRQ) [47]. The MNRQ was modified to assess negative reinforcement principles related to users’ substance of choice rather than nicotine. Each participant was asked to indicate their drug of choice (cannabis, alcohol) and answer the MNRQ questions regarding their experiences with that drug rather than nicotine. The specific questions and scale were not altered.

2.3. Neuroimaging Procedures

Participants were asked to abstain from substance use for at least 72 h prior to their fMRI session as confirmed by combination of self-report, breathalyzer, and urine toxicology screens. A positive result for any substance other than cannabis excluded individuals from the study. Acute cannabis use is difficult to determine by examination of urinary metabolites and therefore use within the past 72 h is possible; however, all participants self-reported abstaining for the 72 h prior to the appointment and only 5 (4 CAN+ALC-SUD, 1 CAN+ALC-EXP) participants were positive for THC on the day of testing, which could reflect use from up to four weeks prior given the regularity of their use history.
The Cue Breathing fMRI paradigm paired a cue reactivity task with anticipation and experience of an unpleasant interoceptive stimulus, an inspiratory breathing load. Each participant received either a cannabis or alcohol version of the task, depending on their reported primary substance of choice. For the cue reactivity task, participants were presented with images of substances (cannabis or alcohol), comparison images consisting of closely matched objects resembling the substance images (e.g., dried leaves resembling cannabis, non-alcoholic beverages), or scrambled versions of the substance and comparison images where the object in the image was unidentifiable. CTL viewed the same version of the task (cannabis or alcohol) as an age-matched substance-using participant. While viewing each image, participants were asked to indicate whether they disliked, felt neutral, or liked the image. Participants provided ratings using the first three buttons of a four-button box and saw a red box appear on screen to confirm their selected answer.
Participants wore a nose clip and respired through a mouthpiece with a non-rebreathing valve (2600 series, Hans Rudolph). The breathing equipment was attached to the scanner head coil using Velcro straps to help hold the mouthpiece in position and eliminate the need for participants to contract their mouth muscles. The mouthpiece connected to a hose that allowed for an inspiratory resistance load of 40 cmH2O/L/s to be attached. This breathing load consisted of a Plexiglas tube with a sintered bronze disk inside that partially limited airflow thereby producing a resistance load. A breathing load of 40 cm H2O/L/s was selected based on previous research which has demonstrated that this load alters subjective symptoms without significantly affecting CO2 or O2 levels, and thereby does not impact the BOLD signal [48,49]. Prior to the scan, participants completed a training session during which they were introduced to the breathing equipment and practiced the task. Individuals experienced increasing levels of restriction up to the target load of 40 cm H2O/L/s. The breathing load was described as feeling like “you are breathing through a straw” and participants were instructed to continue to breathe normally while experiencing the restriction. While in the scanner, participants experienced the breathing load at various times throughout the task for approximately 40 s at a time. Each block of images began with one null trial that lasted for 6 s. During this time, participants saw either a yellow or grey fixation screen. Yellow indicated there was a 1 in 4 (25%) chance of experiencing the breathing load during the next block of images. Alternatively, a grey fixation screen indicated there would be no chance of experiencing the breathing restriction during the upcoming block of images. Each null trial was followed by 6 pictures of the same type (substance, neutral, or scrambled) presented one at a time for 4 s each.
There was a total of 9 task conditions: anticipation neutral images, anticipation substance images, anticipation scrambled images, breathing load neutral images, breathing load substance images, breathing load scrambled images, neutral images only, substance images only, and scrambled images only. Trials during which neutral or scrambled images were presented without the anticipation or experience of the breathing load were combined into a baseline condition. This resulted in 5 conditions of interest: (1) baseline: neutral and scrambled images with no anticipation or breathing restriction; (2) anticipation neutral images: blocks of neutral images preceded by a yellow fixation screen during which the participant did not actually experience the breathing load; (3) anticipation substance images: blocks of substance images preceded by a yellow fixation screen during which the participant did not actually experience the breathing load; (4) breathing load neutral images: blocks of neutral images preceded by a yellow fixation screen during which the participant did experience the breathing load; (5) breathing load substance images: blocks of substance images preceded by a yellow fixation screen during which the participant did experience the breathing load (see Figure 1).
Prior to the scan, participants underwent a training session to learn the task and become familiar with the breathing equipment. This ensured that participants would be able to complete the task within the scanner. Immediately after the scan, participants provided ratings of their in-scanner experience with the breathing load using a Visual Analog Scale (VAS). Participants rated the breathing load for pleasantness, unpleasantness, and intensity using a 10 cm scale ranging from ‘not at all’ to ‘extremely’. After the scan, participants used the same VAS to rate their in-scanner experience of the breathing load.

2.4. Neuroimaging Data Acquisition

The cue reactivity paradigm was presented during one fMRI scan sensitive to blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast using a Signa EXCITE (GE Healthcare, Chicago, IL, USA) 3.0 Tesla scanner (T2*-weighted echo planar imaging (EPI) scans, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 24 cm (squared), 64 × 64 × 40 matrix, forty 3.0 mm axial slices with an in-plane resolution of 3.75 × 3.75 × 3 mm, flip angle = 90 degrees, 420 whole-brain acquisitions). For anatomical reference, a high-resolution T1-weighted image (spoiled gradient recalled [SPGR], TR = 8 ms, TE = 3 ms, slices = 172, FOV = 25 cm approximately 1 mm3 voxels) was obtained.

2.5. Neuroimaging Data Analysis

2.5.1. Individual-Level Processing

All neuroimaging data was processed using the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software package [50]. Following data acquisition, GE slices were reconstructed into AFNI BRIK format. Baseline volume for 3D registration was constructed using the largest temporal region containing the fewest voxel-wise outliers. Data was aligned to the baseline image using all other time points in dx, dy, dz, and roll, pitch, and yaw directions. The functional EPI underwent automatic coregistration to the high-resolution anatomical image and each alignment was manually inspected for each dataset. New outliers were generated for the volume-registered dataset based on whether a given time point greatly exceeded the mean number of voxel outliers for the time series. Six motion regressors (dx, dy, dz and roll, pitch, and yaw), a baseline and linear drift regressor, and nine task-related regressors (trials for anticipation neutral images, anticipation substance images, anticipation scrambled images, breathing load neutral images, breathing load substance images, breathing load scrambled images, neutral images only, substance images only, and scrambled images only) were convolved with a modified hemodynamic response function. The baseline condition, during which there was no cue or experience of the breathing load, served as the baseline for this analysis. A Gaussian Spatial Filter (6 mm full width-half maximum) was used to spatially blur data to account for anatomical differences. Automated transformations were applied to anatomical images and EPIs were subsequently transformed into Montreal neurological institute (MNI) space. Percent signal change (PSC) was determined by dividing each regressor of interest (anticipation neutral images, anticipation substance images, breathing load neutral images, breathing load substance images) by the baseline regressor and multiplying by 100.

2.5.2. Group-Level Analysis

A linear mixed-effects (LME) analysis (r-project.org) was performed to examine group differences in brain activation. Participants were treated as random effects, while group (CAN+ALC-SUD, CAN+ALC-EXP, CTL), interoceptive condition (no breathing load [anticipation], breathing load), and image type (neutral, substance) were treated as fixed effects. PSC from baseline (trials consisting of neutral and scrambled images and no chance or experience of the breathing load) was the dependent variable. The group main effect was examined to identify differences between CAN+ALC-SUD, CAN+ALC-EXP, and CTL across breathing load and cue image type conditions. The group by image type interaction was conducted to examine group differences while viewing substance images across all interoceptive conditions. The group by interoceptive condition interaction was examined to test hypotheses involving anticipation and receipt of the aversive interoceptive breathing load in CAN+ALC-SUD and CTL. The group by interoception by image type interaction was of interest because it allowed for examination of whether substance users show a blunted response to the aversive interoceptive stimuli when paired with the rewarding substance images. To guard against identifying false-positive areas of activation, a threshold adjustment method was applied using AFNI programs 3dFWHMx and 3dClustSim with the auto-correlation function (acf). The 3dClustSim identified a minimum cluster volume of 1280 µL (20 contiguous voxels) corresponding to a per-voxel p-value of 0.002 (bi-sided, NN = 3) to result in a voxel-wise probability of p < 0.05 (two-sided) corrected for multiple comparisons.

3. Results

3.1. Subject Characteristics

Groups did not differ in terms of demographics, including age (F(2, 44) = 1.27, p = 0.290), education (F(2,43) = 0.956, p = 0.392), racial (χ2(8) = 9.043, p = 0.339) and ethnic (χ2(2) = 0.10, p = 0.953) makeup, and gender distribution (χ2(2) = 0.37, p = 0.830); each group had more males than females. Moreover, there was no difference in subjective self-reported unpleasantness (F(2,44) = 0.432, p = 0.652) or intensity (F(2,44) = 2.68, p = 0.08) of the breathing load and the groups did not differ on self-reported interoceptive awareness and impulsivity. However, CAN+ALC-SUD compared to CAN+ALC-EXP reported higher levels of positive and negative reinforcement on the MNRQ. Additionally, both user groups reported higher levels of positive reinforcement than negative reinforcement on the MNRQ (see Table 2).

3.2. Neuroimaging Results

No clusters met the thresholding requirement of 20 voxels for the main effect of group, the group by image type interaction, or the three-way group by interoceptive condition by cue image type interaction.

3.2.1. The Group by interoception interaction

Four brain regions survived thresholding: the right amygdala, the left IFG, the right posterior cingulate, and the left parahippocampal gyrus. (see Table 3). All interactions remained significant after controlling for lifetime nicotine use.
The right amygdala. A significant interaction within the right amygdala (F(2,44) = 7.58, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.256) was examined. Here, groups significantly differed in activation for the anticipation only condition (F(2, 44) = 4.28, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.16) with CAN+ALC-SUD showing significantly greater activation than CAN+ALC-EXP (M = 0.16, SE = 0.06, p = 0.02). Groups also significantly differed during the breathing load condition (F(2, 44) = 4.59, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.17) with CAN+ALC-SUD showing lower activation than CAN+ALC-EXP (M = 0.54, SE = 0.18, p = 0.004). CTL did not significantly differ from either user group during either condition (see Figure 2).
The left inferior frontal gyrus. Within the left IFG (F(2,44) = 5.66, p = 0.006, partial η2 = 0.21), groups did not differ during anticipation (p = 0.28) but did during the breathing load (F(2,44) = 4.62, p = 0.015, partial η2 = 0.17). CAN+ALC-SUD exhibited lower activation than both CAN+ALC-EXP (M = 0.27, SE = 0.11, p = 0.049) and CTL (M = 0.27, SE = 0.11, p = 0.049), who did not differ from one another (see Figure 2).
The right posterior cingulate. An interaction within the right posterior cingulate (F(2,44) = 4.11, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.16) was driven by a significant effect of condition for CTL only (F(1,17) = 11.22, p = 0.004, partial η2 = 0.39) with greater deactivation while experiencing the breathing load; no simple main effect for group was seen in this region.
The left parahippocampal gyrus. Within the left parahippocampal gyrus (F(2,44) = 6.14, p = 0.004, partial η2 = 0.22), groups significantly differed during the anticipation condition (F(2,44) = 3.98, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.15) and during the breathing load trials (F(2,44) = 4.23, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.16). Specifically, during anticipation only trials, CTL exhibited significantly greater activation than CAN+ALC-EXP (M = 0.12, SE = 0.05, p = 0.03), and CAN+ALC-SUD did not differ from either group. For the breathing load, CAN+ALC-SUD showed significantly lower activation than both CTL (M = 0.35, SE = 0.15, p = 0.05) and CAN+ALC-EXP (M = 0.40, SE = 0.15, p = 0.03; see Figure 2).

3.2.2. Follow-Up Correlations

Follow-up correlations were conducted within CAN+ALC-SUD and CAN+ALC-EXP between activation in significant regions and lifetime episodes of cannabis and alcohol use. Within the left IFG, activation during the breathing load condition negatively correlated with lifetime episodes of alcohol use (r = −0.546, p = 0.002, R2 = 0.298). Within CAN+ALC-SUD and CAN+ALC-EXP, PHG activation during the breathing load condition negatively correlated with lifetime episodes of cannabis (r = −0.570, p = 0.001, R2 = 0.325) and alcohol use (r = −0.473, p = 0.009, R2 = 0.224; see Figure 3).

4. Discussion

The present investigation aimed to examine the role of negative reinforcement in adolescent substance use by pairing a cue reactivity paradigm with an aversive interoceptive probe. It was hypothesized that viewing rewarding substance images would dampen the exaggerated interoceptive response to an aversive probe that has previously been observed in adolescent substance users [27]. Specifically, CAN+ALC-SUD compared to CTL was hypothesized to show: (1) heightened neural activation during the breathing load experience in brain regions involved in interoceptive processing and emotion regulation; (2) heightened neural reward responsivity to substance images; and (3) a decreased interoceptive neural response to the breathing load when paired with substance images. It was also hypothesized that, overall, CAN+ALC-EXP would demonstrate a neural response more similar to CTL than CAN+ALC-SUD.
The hypotheses were partially supported. In relation to hypothesis one, a consistent pattern was observed within the left IFG and the left parahippocampal gyrus, wherein CAN+ALC-SUD exhibited a differential BOLD response to the breathing load compared to CAN+ALC-EXP and CTL (Figure 3; Table 3). Based on previous work demonstrating that adolescent SUD showed an increased response to the breathing load [27], it was hypothesized that CAN+ALC-SUD in the present investigation would also exhibit an increased BOLD response. However, compared to CAN+ALC-EXP and CTL, CAN+ALC-SUD showed greater deactivation during the breathing load. Although this result is inconsistent with previous findings among adolescent substance users [27], it is consistent with previous findings among young adults transitioning from recreational to problematic substance use [39]. A similar pattern was observed in the right amygdala, with CAN+ALC-SUD demonstrating greater deactivation than CAN+ALC-EXP. However, CTL did not differ from either group. Hypothesis two was not supported, as CAN+ALC-SUD did not show a differential reward response to substance images compared to CTL. In line with the lack of an exaggerated reward response to the substance images within CAN+ALC-SUD, viewing these cues did not attenuate the exaggerated interoceptive response exhibited by CAN+ALC-SUD (hypothesis 3). Lastly, it was hypothesized that CAN+ALC-EXP would demonstrate brain responses more similar to CTL than CAN+ALC-SUD; this was partially supported. During the anticipation only condition, CAN+ALC-EXP showed an inconsistent pattern. However, during the breathing load condition, CAN+ALC-EXP did not differ from CTL in the right amygdala, the left IFG, or the left parahippocampal gyrus.
The overall findings suggest that CAN+ALC-SUD experience the aversive breathing load differently than CTL and CAN+ALC-EXP in brain regions implicated in interoception and emotion regulation. However, this observation is in the opposite direction of previous findings. Adolescent SUD has previously shown exaggerated activation rather than deactivation in interoceptive regions when experiencing an aversive breathing load [27]. Additionally, viewing images of alcohol and cannabis did not appear to dampen the blunted interoceptive response seen among CAN+ALC-SUD. This finding would suggest that substance use may not be negatively reinforced by dampening uncomfortable sensations. The pattern of use demonstrated by adolescent substance users (non-treatment-seeking users meeting diagnostic criteria) may not be substantial enough to invoke withdrawal-related symptoms compared to adults who have used heavily for years and/or treatment seekers. Therefore, using in order to relieve uncomfortable sensations may be less common among adolescent users or individuals with less significant use patterns. Future studies should collect subjective ratings of how ‘unpleasant’ and ‘aversive’ participants found the breathing load to be while viewing substance and neutral images separately, as this would provide a clearer understanding of whether or not viewing substance images can contribute to an overall reduction in the aversiveness of the breathing load. Lastly, adolescents with SUD showed amygdala deactivation while experiencing the breathing load but increased activation when anticipating the upcoming load. Previous research has demonstrated that cannabis users exhibit deactivation in the amygdala while viewing emotional images, indicative of altered emotion regulation. This may suggest that the observed group differences in the present study are due to differences in emotion regulation. Although, emotion regulation was not directly assessed in this study, this is a potential avenue for future research.
Interestingly, there were also no significant findings within the insular cortex despite its central role in interoception. This contradicts previous research demonstrating that adolescents meeting criteria for SUD exhibit an increased insular response to the breathing load [27]. It is possible that this lack of insular cortex findings is due to the more stringent thresholding methods employed in the present investigation based on current methodological recommendations for the analysis of fMRI data, as insular activation was present at lower thresholds [51]. Overall, this could suggest that experiencing the breathing load within the context of an experimental manipulation may not be significant enough to elicit a strong insular response among adolescents. Future research should examine whether there is an age-related difference in response to aversive interoceptive perturbations.
The lack of evidence demonstrating any negative reinforcement-related neural response may also be because CAN+ALC-SUD did not find the images rewarding enough, given that an exaggerated reward response was not observed. Altered reward responsivity to substance cues is an established finding among adult substance users [52]. It is possible, given that adolescents with CAN+ALC-SUD typically have significantly less use history than adults with CAN+ALC-SUD, that adolescent reward networks have not yet been altered to show an exaggerated response to substance images. This would suggest that altered reward responsivity is not a predisposition among CAN+ALC-SUD but rather a consequence of use. However, an exaggerated neural response to alcohol and cannabis images in limbic regions has been observed among alcohol-using adolescents and young adults [36,37,53]. A possible reason for our discrepant finding could be differences in characteristics defining each sample. For example, participants in the present study used both alcohol and cannabis. Reward circuitry among alcohol and cannabis users may differ from individuals who only consume alcohol and/or cannabis like those in the previously mentioned investigation [36,37]. Future examination of reward circuitry in single- and multi-substance users with a larger sample could help to elucidate this question.
Clinically, our findings suggest that interventions aiming to improve coping through emotion regulation may not be the most effective for adolescent substance users given the lack of evidence that substance use is driven by negative reinforcement. Alternatively, adolescent substance use may be driven more by positive reinforcement; CAN+ALC-SUD self-reported significantly more motivations for use related to positive, as opposed to negative, reinforcement than CAN+ALC-EXP (Table 1). This aligns with the neurobiological imbalance model, which posits that the development of cognitive control regions is more protracted from childhood to young adulthood, while reward regions follow a curvilinear path of development, with a peak in reward responsivity during adolescence [1,54]. This heightened reward response during adolescence can be seen in reward-processing brain regions (i.e., striatum, insula, anterior cingulate cortex) [55,56,57,58] when anticipating and receiving various types of rewards [59,60]. Behaviorally, this imbalance may contribute to an increase in reward-seeking behaviors, including drug and alcohol experimentation [59] and increased susceptibility to the motivational properties of these substances. This may suggest that interventions aimed at helping adolescents learn alternative ways of experiencing reward may be more effective than those aimed at reducing uncomfortable sensations [61].
Although adolescent substance users report negative reinforcement of substance use, this was not observed using a functional imaging paradigm. As reported above, groups also did not differ in their neural responsivity to the substance images, but this finding may be due to a limitation of study design. The substance images used in the cue reactivity paradigm may not be potent or personally relevant enough to elicit a sufficient neural response to overcome the undesirable impact of the breathing load trials [61,62,63]. In daily life, adolescents may experience uncomfortable interoceptive signals on par with the breathing load experienced within the scanner while the rewarding effects of actual substance use may not be comparable to viewing images. Experimentally administering alcohol and drugs in conjunction with fMRI is an increasingly popular research method that may be more powerful for detecting neural changes related to negative reinforcement [64,65]. Alternatively, creating personalized cue reactivity paradigms using substance-related images from adolescents’ social media accounts may be an alternative method of increasing the valence of the substance cues. Future researchers investigating negative reinforcement principles within adolescent substance users should consider these methods to determine whether a more robust substance cue can elicit neural differences.
An additional limitation of the present study may be the categorization of adolescents based on meeting criteria for CAN+ALC-SUD. The observed correlations between substance use and neural response suggest that future examinations of adolescent substance users may be improved using a dimensional, rather than categorical, approach. Although significant differences in BOLD response to interoceptive stimulation have been observed among adult substance users with and without CAN+ALC-SUD [14,66,67], amount of substance use may be a more differentiating factor than reported CAN+ALC-SUD criteria in young users with comparatively little substance use experience. The small sample size of 13 CAN+ALC-SUD, 16 CAN+ALC-EXP, and 18 CTL also limits the conclusions that can be drawn from the current study and the ability to look at substance-use groups individually (e.g., cannabis vs. alcohol) although comorbid cannabis and alcohol use is common among adolescents [68]. Inclusion of more substance-using adolescents in future studies could help better differentiate between youth who experiment with drugs and alcohol and those who experience more negative consequences related to their use. Lastly, CAN+ALC-SUD and CAN+ALC-EXP significantly differed in the amount of time reported since their last cannabis use. Given that cannabis metabolites can remain in the body for up to three weeks after regular use, it is possible that the differences observed between groups could be due to residual effects in the CAN+ALC-SUD group. Therefore, it is possible that the reported findings are more reflective of the effects of current use and that these differences may resolve with continued abstinence, highlighting another potential avenue for future research.
Despite these limitations, the present study contributes preliminary findings to our overall understanding of substance use in adolescence. The findings further support the hypothesis that interoceptive processing may be altered in substance users. Further, the results suggest that adolescents may not seek substances to reduce negative or uncomfortable sensations, rather use may be driven more by increased sensation-seeking and reward responsivity in adolescence. Examining positive reinforcement in adolescent substance use is an important avenue for future research.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.C.M., J.J., J.L.S., M.P.P. and S.F.T.; data curation, A.C.M. and J.L.S. formal analysis, A.C.M., J.L.S. and A.N.S.; funding acquisition, A.C.M., J.L.S., M.P.P. and S.F.T.; investigation, A.C.M. and J.L.S.; methodology, A.C.M., J.J., J.L.S., A.N.S., M.P.P. and S.F.T.; project administration, A.C.M. and J.L.S.; resources, M.P.P. and S.F.T.; software, A.N.S. and M.P.P.; supervision, J.J., J.L.S., A.N.S., M.P.P. and S.F.T.; validation, A.N.S., M.P.P. and S.F.T.; visualization, A.C.M., J.J. and J.L.S.; writing—original draft, A.C.M.; writing—review and editing, J.J., J.L.S., A.N.S., M.P.P. and S.F.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research has been supported in part by NIDA 5P20DA027843, NIH/NIDA U01DA041089, NIH/NIAAA T32AA013525, NIAAA F31AA027169, NIDA R21DA047953, NIGMS 1P20GM121312, California Tobacco-Related Disease Research Grants Program Office of the University of California Grant 580264, and The William K. Warren Foundation. Financial Disclosures: Paulus is an advisor to Spring Care, Inc., a behavioral health startup, he has received royalties for an article about methamphetamine in UpToDate.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Casey, B.J.; Jones, R.M. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: Implications for substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 49, 1189–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Johnston, L.D.; Miech, R.A.; O’Malley, P.M.; Bachman, J.G.; Schulenberg, J.E.; Patrick, M.E. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use 1975–2019: Overview, Key Findings on Adolescent Drug Use; Education Resources Information Center: Washington, DC, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  3. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Key Substance Use and Mental Health Indicators in the United States: Results from the 2018 National Survey on Drug Use and Health, HHS; HHS Publ. No. PEP19-5068, NSDUH Ser. H-54; Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration: Rockville, MD, USA, 2019. [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Hasin, D.S. US Epidemiology of Cannabis Use and Associated Problems. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018, 43, 195–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Grant, B.F.; Dawson, D.A. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: Results from the national longitudinal alcohol epidemiologic survey. J. Subst. Abuse 1997, 9, 103–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2013 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National Findings; NSDUH Series H-48; HHS Publication No. (SMA) 14-4863; Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration: Rockville, MD, USA, 2014.
  7. London, E.D.; Ernst, M.; Grant, S.; Bonson, K.; Weinstein, A. Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: Functional imaging. Cereb. Cortex 2000, 10, 334–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Stewart, J.L.; Juavinett, A.L.; May, A.C.; Davenport, P.W.; Paulus, M.P. Do you feel alright? Attenuated neural processing of aversive interoceptive stimuli in current stimulant users. Psychophysiology 2015, 52, 249–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Bechara, A.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 2000, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Nestor, L.J.; Ghahremani, D.G.; Monterosso, J.; London, E.D. Prefrontal hypoactivation during cognitive control in early abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2011, 194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Paulus, M.P.; Feinstein, J.S.; Tapert, S.F.; Liu, T.T. Trend detection via temporal difference model predicts inferior prefrontal cortex activation during acquisition of advantageous action selection. Neuroimage 2004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Verdejo-Garcia, A.; Chong, T.T.J.; Stout, J.C.; Yücel, M.; London, E.D. Stages of dysfunctional decision-making in addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Sönmez, M.B.; Kahyacı Kılıç, E.; Ateş Çöl, I.; Görgülü, Y.; Köse Çınar, R. Decreased interoceptive awareness in patients with substance use disorders. J. Subst. Use 2017, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. May, A.C.; Stewart, J.L.; Migliorini, R.; Tapert, S.F.; Paulus, M.P. Methamphetamine Dependent Individuals Show Attenuated Brain Response to Pleasant Interoceptive Stimuli. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013, 131, 238–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Stewart, S.H.; Zvolensky, M.J.; Eifert, G.H. Negative-reinforcement drinking motives mediate the relation between anxiety sensitivity and increased drinking behavior. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Craig, A.D. How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Pollatos, O.; Gramann, K.; Schandry, R. Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Craig, A.D. Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2003, 13, 500–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Barrett, L.F.; Simmons, W.K. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Paulus, M.P.; Tapert, S.F.; Schulteis, G. The role of interoception and alliesthesia in addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Verdejo-Garcia, A.; Clark, L.; Dunn, B.D. The role of interoception in addiction: A critical review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Naqvi, N.H.; Bechara, A. The Insula: A Critical Neural Substrate for Drug Seeking under Conflict and Risk. Wiley Handb. Cogn. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Paulus, M.P.; Stewart, J.L. Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology 2014, 76, 342–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Critchley, H.D.; Wiens, S.; Rotshtein, P.; Öhman, A.; Dolan, R.J. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Etkin, A.; Buechel, C.; Gross, J.J. The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Stewart, J.L.; Flagan, T.M.; May, A.C.; Reske, M.; Simmons, A.N.; Paulus, M.P. Young adults at risk for stimulant dependence show reward dysfunction during reinforcement-based decision making. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 73, 235–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Berk, L.; Stewart, J.L.; May, A.C.; Wiers, R.W.; Davenport, P.W.; Paulus, M.P.; Tapert, S.F. Under pressure: Adolescent substance users show exaggerated neural processing of aversive interoceptive stimuli. Addiction 2015, 110, 2025–2036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Stewart, J.L.; Butt, M.; May, A.C.; Tapert, S.F.; Paulus, M.P. Insular and cingulate attenuation during decision making is associated with future transition to stimulant use disorder. Addiction 2017, 112, 1567–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Verdejo-García, A.; Bechara, A. A somatic marker theory of addiction. Neuropharmacology 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Wills, T.A.; Pokhrel, P.; Morehouse, E.; Fenster, B. Behavioral and Emotional Regulation and Adolescent Substance Use Problems: A Test of Moderation Effects in a Dual-Process Model. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2011, 25, 279–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  31. Payer, D.; Lieberman, M.; London, E. Neural correlates of affect processing and aggression in methamphetamine dependence. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2011, 68, 271–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Veinante, P.; Yalcin, I.; Barrot, M. The amygdala between sensation and affect: A role in pain. J. Mol. Psychiatry 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Morawetz, C.; Bode, S.; Derntl, B.; Heekeren, H.R. The effect of strategies, goals and stimulus material on the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Conrod, P.; Nikolaou, K. Annual Research Review: On the developmental neuropsychology of substance use disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Wolitzky-Taylor, K.; McBeth, J.; Guillot, C.R.; Stone, M.D.; Kirkpatrick, M.G.; Zvolensky, M.J.; Buckner, J.D.; Leventhal, A.M. Transdiagnostic processes linking anxiety symptoms and substance use problems among adolescents. J. Addict. Dis. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Karoly, H.C.; Schacht, J.P.; Meredith, L.R.; Jacobus, J.; Tapert, S.F.; Gray, K.M.; Squeglia, L.M. Investigating a novel fMRI cannabis cue reactivity task in youth. Addict. Behav. 2019, 89, 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Tapert, S.F.; Cheung, E.H.; Brown, G.G.; Frank, L.R.; Paulus, M.P.; Schweinsburg, A.D.; Meloy, M.J.; Brown, S.A. Neural Response to Alcohol Stimuli in Adolescents with Alcohol Use Disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2003, 60, 727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Lopata, M.; La Fata, J.; Evanich, M.J.; Lourenço, R.V. Effects of flow-resistive loading on mouth occlusion pressure during CO2 rebreathing. Am. Rev. Respir. Dis. 1977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Stewart, J.L.; Parnass, J.M.; May, A.C.; Davenport, P.W.; Paulus, M.P. Altered frontocingulate activation during aversive interoceptive processing in young adults transitioning to problem stimulant use. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Stewart, J.L.; May, A.C.; Poppa, T.; Davenport, P.W.; Tapert, S.F.; Paulus, M.P. You are the danger: Attenuated insula response in methamphetamine users during aversive interoceptive decision-making. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014, 142, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Schacht, J.P.; Anton, R.F.; Myrick, H. Functional neuroimaging studies of alcohol cue reactivity: A quantitative meta-analysis and systematic review. Addict. Biol. 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Zhou, X.; Zimmermann, K.; Xin, F.; Zhao, W.; Derckx, R.T.; Sassmannshausen, A.; Scheele, D.; Hurlemann, R.; Weber, B.; Kendrick, K.M.; et al. Cue Reactivity in the Ventral Striatum Characterizes Heavy Cannabis Use, Whereas Reactivity in the Dorsal Striatum Mediates Dependent Use. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Pierucci-Lagha, A.; Gelernter, J.; Feinn, R.; Cubells, J.F.; Pearson, D.; Pollastri, A.; Farrer, L.; Kranzler, H.R. Diagnostic reliability of the semi-structured assessment for drug dependence and alcoholism (SSADDA). Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Brown, S.A.; Myers, M.G.; Lippke, L.; Tapert, S.F.; Stewart, D.G.; Vik, P.W. Psychometric evaluation of the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (CDDR): A measure of adolescent alcohol and drug involvement. J. Stud. Alcohol 1998, 59, 427–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Whiteside, S.P.; Lynam, D.R. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2001, 30, 669–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Mehling, W.E.; Price, C.; Daubenmier, J.J.; Acree, M.; Bartmess, E.; Stewart, A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Pomerleau, O.F.; Fagerström, K.O.; Marks, J.L.; Tate, J.C.; Pomerleau, C.S. Development and validation of a self-rating scale for positive- and negative-reinforcement smoking: The Michigan nicotine reinforcement questionnaire. Nicotine Tob. Res. 2003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Paulus, M.P.; Flagan, T.; Simmons, A.N.; Gillis, K.; Kotturi, S.; Thom, N.; Johnson, D.C.; Van Orden, K.F.; Davenport, P.W.; Swain, J.L. Subjecting elite athletes to inspiratory breathing load reveals behavioral and neural signatures of optimal performers in extreme environments. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e29394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Davenport, P.W.; Vovk, A. Cortical and subcortical central neural pathways in respiratory sensations. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Cox, R.W. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Eklund, A.; Nichols, T.E.; Knutsson, H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Luijten, M.; Schellekens, A.F.; Kühn, S.; Machielse, M.W.J.; Sescousse, G. Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction. JAMA Psychiatry 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Dager, A.D.; Anderson, B.M.; Rosen, R.; Khadka, S.; Sawyer, B.; Jiantonio-Kelly, R.E.; Austad, C.S.; Raskin, S.A.; Tennen, H.; Wood, R.M.; et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) response to alcohol pictures predicts subsequent transition to heavy drinking in college students. Addiction 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Somerville, L.H.; Jones, R.M.; Casey, B.J. A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cogn. 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Delgado, M.R.; Locke, H.M.; Stenger, V.A.; Fiez, J.A. Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: Effects of valence and magnitude manipulations. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2003, 3, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Ernst, M.; Nelson, E.E.; Jazbec, S.; McClure, E.B.; Monk, C.S.; Leibenluft, E.; Blair, J.; Pine, D.S. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. Neuroimage 2005, 25, 1279–1291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Elliott, R.; Newman, J.L.; Longe, O.A.; Deakin, J.F.W. Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex to financial reward in humans: A parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 303–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Seymour, B.; Daw, N.; Dayan, P.; Singer, T.; Dolan, R. Differential Encoding of Losses and Gains in the Human Striatum. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 4826–4831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Hardin, M.G.; Ernst, M. Functional brain imaging of development-related risk and vulnerability for substance use in adolescents. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2016, 11, 361–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. May, A.C.; Stewart, J.L.; Tapert, S.F.; Paulus, M.P. The effect of age on neural processing of pleasant soft touch stimuli. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Aguinaldo, L.D.; Squeglia, L.M.; Gray, K.M.; Coronado, C.; Lees, B.; Tomko, R.L.; Jacobus, J. Behavioral Treatments for Adolescent Cannabis Use Disorder: A Rationale for Cognitive Retraining. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Fatseas, M.; Serre, F.; Alexandre, J.M.; Debrabant, R.; Auriacombe, M.; Swendsen, J. Craving and substance use among patients with alcohol, tobacco, cannabis or heroin addiction: A comparison of substance- and person-specific cues. Addiction 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Karoly, H.C.; Schacht, J.P.; Jacobus, J.; Meredith, L.R.; Taylor, C.T.; Tapert, S.F.; Gray, K.M.; Squeglia, L.M. Preliminary evidence that computerized approach avoidance training is not associated with changes in fMRI cannabis cue reactivity in non-treatment-seeking adolescent cannabis users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Filbey, F.M.; Claus, E.; Audette, A.R.; Niculescu, M.; Banich, M.T.; Tanabe, J.; Du, Y.P.; Hutchison, K.E. Exposure to the taste of alcohol elicits activation of the mesocorticolimbic neurocircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Strang, N.M.; Claus, E.D.; Ramchandani, V.A.; Graff-Guerrero, A.; Boileau, I.; Hendershot, C.S. Dose-dependent effects of intravenous alcohol administration on cerebral blood flow in young adults. Psychopharmacology 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Bjork, J.M.; Smith, A.R.; Hommer, D.W. Striatal sensitivity to reward deliveries and omissions in substance dependent patients. Neuroimage 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Gowin, J.L.; Stewart, J.L.; May, A.C.; Ball, T.M.; Wittmann, M.; Tapert, S.F.; Paulus, M.P. Altered cingulate and insular cortex activation during risk-taking in methamphetamine dependence: Losses lose impact. Addiction 2014, 109, 237–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Mason, W.A.; Chmelka, M.B.; Howard, B.K.; Thompson, R.W. Comorbid alcohol and cannabis use disorders among high-risk youth at intake into residential care. J. Adolesc. Health 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Depiction of the cue reactivity paradigm paired with interoceptive breathing load. (A) A yellow fixation screen is presented to the participant, indicating that there is a 1 in 4 chance they will experience the breathing load during the upcoming block of pictures. The fixation screen is immediately followed by 6 images—in this case, alcohol-related cue images. (B) A grey fixation screen is presented to the participant indicating that there is no chance they will experience the breathing load during the upcoming block of pictures. The fixation screen is immediately followed by 6 images—in this case, substance-matched comparison images. (C) Each participant wears the breathing apparatus while in the fMRI machine. They wear a nose clip to ensure they breathe through the tube only and a breathing manifold is attached at the end of the tube for periods of 40 s as indicated by the paired cue reactivity task.
Figure 1. Depiction of the cue reactivity paradigm paired with interoceptive breathing load. (A) A yellow fixation screen is presented to the participant, indicating that there is a 1 in 4 chance they will experience the breathing load during the upcoming block of pictures. The fixation screen is immediately followed by 6 images—in this case, alcohol-related cue images. (B) A grey fixation screen is presented to the participant indicating that there is no chance they will experience the breathing load during the upcoming block of pictures. The fixation screen is immediately followed by 6 images—in this case, substance-matched comparison images. (C) Each participant wears the breathing apparatus while in the fMRI machine. They wear a nose clip to ensure they breathe through the tube only and a breathing manifold is attached at the end of the tube for periods of 40 s as indicated by the paired cue reactivity task.
Brainsci 10 00214 g001
Figure 2. Neuroimaging results from the group by interoception condition interaction in (a) the right amygdala; (b) the left inferior frontal gyrus; (c) the right posterior cingulate; and (d) the left parahippocampal gyrus. * indicates significant differences.
Figure 2. Neuroimaging results from the group by interoception condition interaction in (a) the right amygdala; (b) the left inferior frontal gyrus; (c) the right posterior cingulate; and (d) the left parahippocampal gyrus. * indicates significant differences.
Brainsci 10 00214 g002
Figure 3. Follow-up correlations between activation in significant regions of interest and reported lifetime episodes of cannabis and alcohol use across all substance users.
Figure 3. Follow-up correlations between activation in significant regions of interest and reported lifetime episodes of cannabis and alcohol use across all substance users.
Brainsci 10 00214 g003
Table 1. Characteristics of Substance Use.
Table 1. Characteristics of Substance Use.
CAN+ALC-SUDGroup Description% Meeting Diagnostic CriteriaDiagnostic Criteria Endorsed
M(SD)MinMax
THC Use Disorder92.313.42 (1.38)26
Alcohol Use Disorder61.542.63 (.74)24
Substance UseCAN+ALC-SUD
Cannabis/Alcohol Substance Use Disorder
CAN+ALC-EXP
Cannabis/Alcohol Experimenter
CTL
Little to No Substance Use
dftp
Lifetime Cannabis Use467.85 (288.05)39.38 (45.15)0.17 (0.514)12.48−5.31<0.001
Days Since Last THC Use18.69 (33.34)71.69 (82.25)46.11 (160.19)20.632.350.029
Lifetime Alcohol Use131.92 (131.55)17.44 (15.87)0.22 (0.73)12.28−3.120.009
Days Since Last Alcohol16.46 (11.67)45.38 (98.99)22.22 (66.12)271.040.306
Lifetime Alcohol Binge Episode92.83 (71.90)7.87 (7.97)0.11 (0.47)11.22−4.070.002
Days Since Last Binge24.70 (24.83)90.93 (135. 77)240 (--)15.381.840.085
Lifetime Hallucinogen Use2.69 (3.88)0.13 (0.50)--12.32−2.370.035
Days Since Last Hallucinogen82.31 (93.58)9.81 (39.25)--15.42−2.610.019
Lifetime Sedative Use0.77 (1.36)----12.00−2.030.065
Days Since Last Sedative Use179.15 (330.97)----12.00−1.950.075
Lifetime Amphetamine Use0.31 (1.11)----12.00−1.000.337
Days Since Last Amphetamine Use14.46 (52.14)----12−1.000.337
Lifetime Rx Stimulant Use1.92 (5.48)0.06 (0.25)--12.04−1.220.245
Days Since Last Rx Stimulant Use148.23 (297.47)17.94 (71.75)--13.14−1.540.147
Lifetime Cocaine Use0.92 (1.50)----12.00−2.220.046
Days Since Last Cocaine Use55.00 (91.33)----12.00−2.170.051
Lifetime Ecstasy Use14.85 (27.65)----12.00−1.940.077
Days Since Last Ecstasy Use293.62 (333.72)----12.00−3.170.008
Lifetime Opiate Use0.92 (2.75)1.94 (7.49)--270.4620.647
Days Since Last Opiate Use139.31 (277.92)26.56 (73.13)--13.35−1.420.178
Lifetime Inhalant Use2.38 (8.30)----12.00−1.040.321
Days Since Last Inhalant Use106.00 (259.42)----12.00−1.470.166
Lifetime Nicotine Use232.00 (409.19)4.19 (6.73)0.56 (2.36)12.00−2.010.068
Days Since Last Nicotine Use92.69 (108.66)130.69 (157.63)21.94 (93.10)26.390.7660.451
Table 2. Sample Characteristics.
Table 2. Sample Characteristics.
CAN+ALC-SUD
Cannabis/Alcohol Substance Use Disorder
CAN+ALC-EXP
Cannabis/Alcohol Experimenter
CTL
Little to No Substance Use
DemographicsM(SD)M(SD)M(SD)dfFp
Age (in years)16.62 (0.51)16.69 (0.70)16.33 (0.77)2,441.270.290
Education (in years)10.46 (0.78)10.47 (0.83)10.11 (0.90)2,430.9560.392
WRAT 4 Verbal IQ107.31 (14.29)106.75 (12.37)112.00 (13.82)2,440.7700.469
VAS RatingsM(SD)M(SD)M(SD)dfFp
Unpleasant5.69 (3.29)4.63 (2.64)5.34 (3.49)2,440.4320.652
Intensity4.08 (3.47)2.13 (2.77)4.41 (2.89)2,442.680.08
QuestionnairesM(SD)M(SD)M(SD)dfF/tp
MAIA
Noticing2.83 (1.52)2.75 (1.03)2.78 (1.19)2,430.0160.984
Not Distracting2.14 (0.50)2.37 (1.12)2.52 (1.30)2,430.4430.645
Not Worrying2.89 (1.43)2.81 (1.42)2.70 (1.05)2,430.0790.925
Attention Regulation3.17 (.95)3.45 (0.74)3.14 (1.15)2,430.4940.613
Emotional Awareness3.18 (1.43)3.04 (1.31)3.07 (.93)2,430.0540.948
Self-Regulation3.10 (1.05)3.23 (0.90)3.01 (1.05)2,430.2070.814
Body Listening1.36 (1.16)1.96 (1.44)1.79 (1.05)2,430.8440.437
Trusting3.47 (1.40)3.73 (1.08)3.72 (0.92)2,430.2310.795
UPPS
Lack of Premeditation2.08 (0.39)2.18 (0.49)1.89 (0.42)2,441.920.159
Urgency2.30 (0.66)2.17 (0.59)2.06 (0.51)2,440.6720.516
Sensation Seeking3.18 (0.29)3.09 (0.48)3.03 (0.44)2,440.5280.594
Lack of Perseverance2.03 (0.59)2.13 (0.58)1.83 (0.34)2,441.530.229
MNRQ
Negative Reinforcement2.85 (2.38)0.875 (1.63)--20.522.550.019
Positive Reinforcement11.38 (2.53)7.25 (3.45)--273.590.001
Table 3. fMRI results and between-group comparisons (SUD = CAN+ALC-SUD; EXP = CAN+ALC-EXP).
Table 3. fMRI results and between-group comparisons (SUD = CAN+ALC-SUD; EXP = CAN+ALC-EXP).
GROUP BY INTEROCEPTIVE CONDITION INTERACTION
R/LVoxelsVolumeXYZBAAnticipationLoad
AmygdalaR33211228−9−3028SUD > EXPEXP > SUD
Inferior Frontal GyrusL281792−1324−2011--EXP = CTL > SUD
Posterior CingulateR25160013−651631--EXP = SUD > CTL
Parahippocampal GyrusL211344−24−7−1935CTL > EXPCTL = EXP > SUD
MAIN EFFECT OF INTEROCEPTIVE CONDITION
R/LVoxelsVolumeXYZBACondition Effect
Cingulate GyrusR31412010248−62324Load > Anticipation
Fusiform GyrusR6634243240−12−2420Anticipation > Load
Superior Frontal GyrusR3342137614576Load > Anticipation
Cingulate GyrusL1318384−2−253731Load > Anticipation
CuneusR112716818−852818Load > Anticipation
ThalamusR6440966−184 Load > Anticipation
DecliveL613904−15−63−20 Load > Anticipation
Middle Frontal GyrusL483072−3637289Anticipation > Load
Middle Occipital GyrusR43275234−83919Load > Anticipation
Anterior CingulateL392496−6311524Load > Anticipation
PrecuneusR3623045−43437Load > Anticipation
Precentral GyrusR29185618−26644Load > Anticipation
Precentral GyrusL241536−18−29634Anticipation > Load
MAIN EFFECT OF CUE STIMULUS TYPE
R/LVoxelsVolumeXYZBAStimulus Effect
Medial Frontal GyrusR432752144309Substance > Comparison
Anterior CingulateL231472−146832Substance > Comparison

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

May, A.C.; Jacobus, J.; Stewart, J.L.; Simmons, A.N.; Paulus, M.P.; Tapert, S.F. Do Adolescents Use Substances to Relieve Uncomfortable Sensations? A Preliminary Examination of Negative Reinforcement among Adolescent Cannabis and Alcohol Users. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 214. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10040214

AMA Style

May AC, Jacobus J, Stewart JL, Simmons AN, Paulus MP, Tapert SF. Do Adolescents Use Substances to Relieve Uncomfortable Sensations? A Preliminary Examination of Negative Reinforcement among Adolescent Cannabis and Alcohol Users. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(4):214. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10040214

Chicago/Turabian Style

May, April C., Joanna Jacobus, Jennifer L. Stewart, Alan N. Simmons, Martin P. Paulus, and Susan F. Tapert. 2020. "Do Adolescents Use Substances to Relieve Uncomfortable Sensations? A Preliminary Examination of Negative Reinforcement among Adolescent Cannabis and Alcohol Users" Brain Sciences 10, no. 4: 214. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10040214

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop