Next Article in Journal
Overview of Traumatic Brain Injury: An Immunological Context
Next Article in Special Issue
Modeling the Development of Audiovisual Cue Integration in Speech Perception
Previous Article in Journal
Single Electrode Deep Brain Stimulation with Dual Targeting at Dual Frequency for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: A Case Series and Review of the Literature
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Contributions of Letter-Speech Sound Learning and Visual Print Tuning to Reading Improvement: Evidence from Brain Potential and Dyslexia Training Studies

by
Gorka Fraga González
1,2,*,†,
Gojko Žarić
3,4,†,
Jurgen Tijms
1,2,5,
Milene Bonte
3,4 and
Maurits W. Van der Molen
1,6
1
Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1018 WS, The Netherlands
2
Rudolf Berlin Center, Amsterdam 1018 WS, The Netherlands
3
Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht 6200 MD, The Netherlands
4
Maastricht Brain Imaging Center, Maastricht University, Maastricht 6200 MD, The Netherlands
5
IWAL Institute, Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1001 EW, The Netherlands
6
Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1018 WT, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Submission received: 21 November 2016 / Revised: 20 December 2016 / Accepted: 9 January 2017 / Published: 18 January 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Audiovisual Integration in Early Language Development)

Abstract

:
We use a neurocognitive perspective to discuss the contribution of learning letter-speech sound (L-SS) associations and visual specialization in the initial phases of reading in dyslexic children. We review findings from associative learning studies on related cognitive skills important for establishing and consolidating L-SS associations. Then we review brain potential studies, including our own, that yielded two markers associated with reading fluency. Here we show that the marker related to visual specialization (N170) predicts word and pseudoword reading fluency in children who received additional practice in the processing of morphological word structure. Conversely, L-SS integration (indexed by mismatch negativity (MMN)) may only remain important when direct orthography to semantic conversion is not possible, such as in pseudoword reading. In addition, the correlation between these two markers supports the notion that multisensory integration facilitates visual specialization. Finally, we review the role of implicit learning and executive functions in audiovisual learning in dyslexia. Implications for remedial research are discussed and suggestions for future studies are presented.

1. Introduction

Alphabetic written languages consist of symbols of arbitrary form or letters, which are associated by social agreement to specific speech sounds, i.e., perceptually distinct units of spoken language that differentiate between the words (e.g., bat, cat, hat). A broader definition of alphabetic scripts includes abjad scripts (e.g., Hebrew) in which commonly only the consonants are written, syllabic scripts (Japanese katakana) in which single characters represent syllables, and abugida scripts (e.g., Ethiopic Ge’ez) in which vowels do not have their own symbol but are represented by changes in the symbols of the consonants. Languages using alphabetic scripts can vary in the consistency of the connection between grapheme and phoneme from highly transparent (shallow) orthographies (e.g., Finnish, Italian) with high regularity in the letter-speech sound connections [1], to opaque (deep) orthographies (e.g., English, Danish) in which the letter-speech sound correspondences are complex and ambiguous [2,3]. Thus, the first task of a beginning reader in an alphabetic orthography is to match the distinctive visual symbols (graphemes) to the units of speech sound (phonemes). Efficiently establishing letter-speech sound (L-SS) associations, together with the ability to distinguish separate phonemes, so called phonemic awareness, is essential to connect the spelling of written words to their pronunciation and meaning. This ultimately enables sight word reading, that is, automatic and accurate word reading from memory [4]. The majority of the population easily attains the cognitive steps needed for fluent reading with reading instruction and training. However, a small, but significant, percentage of the population diagnosed with developmental dyslexia fails to develop typical levels of reading abilities in the absence of other cognitive or neurological impairments [5]. Developmental dyslexia affects around 5%–10% of children and is characterized by dysfluent and inaccurate word recognition, spelling, and phonological decoding [5,6,7]. The lack of reading fluency is its most persistent symptom. It is hardly remediated by current treatments [8,9,10], although there are inter-individual differences in the level of reading (dys)fluency among dyslexic readers [11,12]. The present review is concerned with impairments in L-SS learning and visual specialization during the initial phases of reading in dyslexic children. Before these deficits are considered, let us first introduce in more detail the key cognitive processes involved in the typical development of fluent reading.
We consider the acquisition of fluent reading skills as a multisensory problem in which a novice reader has to combine a visual element (letter) with an auditory element (speech sound) into a new audiovisual unit. Rather than mapping orthography directly to existing phonological representations of speech sounds, there is an ongoing recoding of the relevant spoken language elements, as it is unlikely that the speech sounds have isolated representations before learning to read [13,14,15]. At the same time, the reader must also learn how to extract a specific speech sound from the otherwise continuous auditory input to create a “graphoneme”, i.e., a new representation of the speech sound that also includes its orthographic characteristic [16]. Ultimately, instead of exclusively connecting the orthographic forms to phonology, expert readers also connect orthographic forms to meaning [17,18]. This facilitates the reading of familiar words, especially if they contain irregular L-SS correspondences [19]. Thus, concurrently with the development of L-SS integration, cognitive changes related to processing print and whole words occur.
There are several lines of evidence showing that experienced readers perceive a visually presented word in a different manner than merely processing its parts separately in succession. For example, the time needed to recognize a word is not always influenced by its length [20,21] and recognition is almost perfect, even when a word is presented for a very brief period of time, such as 50 ms [22]. In addition, a letter is recognized faster if it is embedded in a word than in isolation or as part of a random letter string; this is called word superiority effect [23]. This effect extends to pseudowords, which suggests that it depends partly on prelexical processing and it can be easily generalized to novel stimuli [24]. Moreover, these effects are invariant to differences in size, case, and font, and are influenced by priming across such variations [24]. Importantly, the above findings indicate that there is a shift from primarily relying on phonological recoding strategies to predominantly sight word reading, depending on the familiarity of the word [23,24,25,26,27]. While familiar words are read without involvement of phonology, unfamiliar words and pseudowords would require orthographic-phonological decoding (e.g., [28,29]). Further, these cognitive strategies may depend on the level of orthographic transparency, with beginning readers adopting strategies based on L-SS conversion in more transparent orthographies, while relying more on direct visual recognition strategies in opaque orthographies [30].
The most prevalent view of dyslexia is the phonological deficit theory, based on the finding that dyslexic readers usually exhibit impaired phonological awareness and limited verbal working memory [31]. Within that framework, it is still a matter of debate whether they cannot build clear phonological representations or cannot efficiently use otherwise intact phonological representations [32]. Despite being a widely accepted theory, a purely phonological account of dyslexia does not explain some of the empirical results available. For example, an L-SS deficit is found to be present in the absence of obvious impairments in phonological awareness [33]. That study also showed that children who would later become impaired readers often did not show phonological awareness problems before the onset of reading instruction in kindergarten. Moreover, studies on integration of L-SS correspondences have shown impairments in dyslexics in behavioral [18,33,34], as well as in neural measures [35,36,37,38,39,40,41]. Additionally, a recent study in pre-reading children, at varying risks of dyslexia, revealed a relation between neural L-SS integration and the learning rate/achievement on an artificial L-SS correspondences training [42]. It should also be noted that interpreting the results from phonological tasks as reflecting purely phonological processing is not as straightforward as it may seem. Several studies have shown that orthographic influences can be observed in verbal phonological tasks [43,44,45,46]. This observation suggests that, even without presenting written stimuli, orthographic knowledge influences phonological judgments [47]. This point does not necessarily challenge the phonological deficit idea per se, but it stresses the interaction between written and spoken language representation with reading development. Based on the above and similar studies, some authors proposed a failure to successfully develop fluent and automated L-SS associations as a most proximal cause for dyslexia [27,47,48,49,50]. Accordingly, it is suggested that, although dyslexic children do possess letter knowledge already at the end of the first grade, their L-SS integration may not automatize, leading to slower and dysfluent reading [49]. This proposition does not exclude, however, the possibility of phonological deficit in dyslexic readers, but considers it as a more distal cause of reading failure [33] whose influence may be greater in opaque than in transparent languages [51].
From a neurobiological perspective, literacy is subserved by two evolutionary developed neural systems, predominantly located in the left hemisphere [52]: one for spoken language [53,54], and the other for visual object recognition [55]. Since reading is a recent cultural invention, these systems have to adapt to the newly-obtained reading skill [56,57,58]. More specifically, studies have delineated the networks of brain regions involved in L-SS integration and visual word recognition. Brain regions in the superior temporal cortex are involved in multisensory L-SS integration [49,59,60] while a ventral left occipito-temporal region, more specifically the left fusiform gyrus, is involved in fast visual word recognition [24,61]. Additionally, learning to read seems to enhance brain activation in a widespread network [57,58] including regions involved in phonological processing [59,62,63]. The proposed developmental trajectory of brain posterior systems for reading suggests an early specialization of multisensory integration regions that facilitates specialization of the visual occipito-temporal system for word recognition [64]. It is these two posterior brain systems that neuroimaging studies converge on reporting atypical activation in both dyslexic adults and children [9,37,65,66,67]. The findings of those studies are interpreted to indicate deficits in multisensory integration and fast visual word recognition. The relative contribution of each these two systems to reading deficits in dyslexia is addressed in our joint analyses of electrophysiological responses associated with L-SS integration and visual specialization (see Section 2).
In the first part of this paper we review and extend the work of our group regarding reading fluency and its improvements associated with training. The focus is predominantly on electrophysiological measures of L-SS integration and visual word recognition. We start by reviewing our previous findings concerned with a comparison between dyslexics and typically reading children [38,68]. Then, we review our longitudinal findings pertaining to the predictive power of our measures in relation to improvements associated with the first stages of a treatment program. The major focus here is on L-SS associations [40,69,70]. We will then review behavioral data obtained when children received extended practice enhancing their knowledge at the word level [71]. This is followed by examining the relation between electrophysiological measures of audiovisual integration and visual specialization. In the second part of our review, we consider the role of associative learning in establishing L-SS associations within the context of current training approaches. We present a concise review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies of dyslexia on implicit learning and executive functions that we consider relevant to associative learning. We discuss how neurocognitive measures derived from those studies may help to improve the learning of L-SS associations in dyslexics and can be used to predict treatment outcome. Finally, we will present our ongoing EEG research on the learning of artificial L-SS associations.

2. Electrophysiological Markers for Letter-Speech Sound Integration and Visual Specialization in Dyslexic Readers

Electroencephalography (EEG) has a high temporal resolution, which makes it a powerful tool to investigate the temporal dynamics of neural activity during fluent reading. Previously, we employed a classical event-related potential (ERP) analysis [38,68] and, more recently, connectivity measures derived from EEG oscillations [72,73], to examine differences between dyslexics and typical readers. In our ERP studies, we used two experimental paradigms to investigate multisensory integration and visual specialization for words, i.e., a crossmodal oddball paradigm [38,40] and a visual word recognition paradigm [68,69], respectively (see Figure 1). These two paradigms should allow a comprehensive picture of the differences between typical readers and dyslexic children and how these differences scale with the severity of the reading (dys)fluency.
The crossmodal oddball paradigm was used to assess mismatch negativity (MMN) and late negativity (LN) responses in relation to developmental dyslexia. The MMN is a fronto-central negativity elicited at around 100–200 ms by an infrequent sound (deviant, or oddball), that is presented within a sequence of a frequent, repeated (standard) sounds [74]. The LN is a later negativity within a broader time range that is usually found around 300–700 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus. We used a crossmodal version of the oddball paradigm, which involves an auditory-only (phoneme presentation) and an audiovisual condition (phoneme and grapheme presentation), as presented in Figure 1A. In the audiovisual condition, letter and speech sounds were presented either synchronously or the letter preceded the speech sound by 200 ms. The MMN response is believed to represent automatic change detection that is sensitive to deviation from established traces in auditory short-term memory [74,75]. The LN is hypothesized to reflect the more cognitive and/or attentional aspects of this integration, although its functional role is still unclear [40]. Previous studies showed that the simultaneous presentation of letter and speech sounds evoked an MMN but not an LN in adults, whereas it affected the LN in the beginning readers, and both the LN and the MMN in advanced school-aged children [76,77]. This developmental dissociation suggests that, even in a fairly transparent language like Dutch, it may take years to develop the integration of accurate letter-speech sound identification and discrimination at the neural level [38,76,77] while, at the behavioral level these associations are typically observed after only one year of reading instruction [78].
We used a visual word recognition paradigm to examine visual specialization for print. The paradigm consists of a one-back task in which words and strings of symbols are visually presented (see Figure 1B). The ERP component of interest in this paradigm is the visual N1/N170 (hereafter N170), an occipito-temporal component of negative polarity and peak latencies around 200 ms after stimuli. Previous studies found enhanced N170 amplitudes to orthographic compared to visually matched stimuli [79,80]. In a series of studies, Maurer and colleagues examined N170 responses to relative words vs. strings of icon-like symbols in dyslexics and typical readers at different stages of reading acquisition [81,82]. Typical readers showed a left-lateralized N170 amplitude to words vs. symbol strings that increased from kindergarten to 2nd grade, but leveled off between 2nd grade and 5th grade [82]. This was interpreted to suggest an inverted “U” model of development of perceptual visual expertise for print. In the same study, dyslexics showed reduced word-specific N170 amplitudes as compared to typical readers in 2nd grade, while in 5th grade this group difference was not significant, and even an opposite trend was observed at that stage [82]. This finding was interpreted to suggest a protracted developmental trajectory of visual expertise for words in dyslexics. However, other studies using a similar paradigm suggested that visual specialization deficits may remain in dyslexic pre-adolescents [83] and adults [84,85]. Finally, correlations between N170 amplitudes and reading abilities were reported in typical readers [86], and in a group including both dyslexic and typical readers [87,88].

2.1. Crossmodal MMN and Visual N170 Related to Reading Fluency in Dyslexic Children

In our study of L-SS integration in dyslexic and typically reading children [38], we used the same crossmodal paradigm (Figure 1A) as in the previous studies [35,76]. The goal of our study was to extend previous findings to an age group between complete beginners and more experienced children, i.e., nine-year-old typical and dyslexic children [38]. The focus of this study was on individual differences in reading fluency, thus, the dyslexic sample was divided into two groups based on their reading fluency score: severely dysfluent dyslexic children and moderately dysfluent dyslexic children. First, we found both MMN and LN effects in the typically reading children, which are consistent with the notion of a slow/protracted development and a high sensitivity to reading related stimuli (e.g., phonemes [89] and words [82,87]) in the first years of reading acquisition; Second, our results revealed a different pattern of integration deficiency depending on the level of reading fluency impairment. We observed a deficit in crossmodal MMN responses that was only present in severely dysfluent dyslexics, while the later crossmodal LN response was reduced in both dyslexic groups compared to typical readers. Importantly, reading fluency scores were correlated with the latency of the crossmodal MMN response when letters and speech sounds were presented simultaneously [38]. The results were interpreted in the framework of an unsuccessful representation of an audiovisual stimulus in severe dyslexics and unavailability of the represented stimulus for further manipulation in both dyslexic groups [37,90,91]. Further, the findings on crossmodal LN might point at deficits in cognitive, explicit associative, and/or attentional processes associated with L-SS integration, rather than an isolated pure bottom-up deficit [35,76,92,93]. This is supported by two recent ERP studies on L-SS integration in dyslexic children reporting abnormal responses suggestive of greater cognitive effort rather than automaticity [94] and a delay in using orthographic and phonological information during response selection [41].
In a companion study we examined electrophysiological responses in typical and dyslexic readers during a visual word recognition task [68]. We presented words vs. strings of meaningless letter-like symbols (Figure 1B) to examine the specificity of the N170 component in nine-year old children. Both typical and dyslexic readers showed a more pronounced N170 component for words compared to symbols. Interestingly, the N170 responses to words were reduced at the left occipito-temporal sites relative to the right hemisphere in typical readers but there was no hemisphere effect in the dyslexic group. We interpreted this finding to suggest facilitated lexical access or more efficient attention allocation in the group of typical readers. Interestingly, N170 amplitudes and reading fluency were positively correlated in the dyslexic group. We concluded that these results point to the functional role of N170 in visual word specialization [79,80] and its potential use to discriminate between typically reading and dyslexic children [82,95].

2.2. Crossmodal MMN and Visual N170 as Predictors of Treatment Response

The next question we addressed was whether the training we previously used to enhance reading fluency in dyslexic children [70] would also affect the brain potential responses that we observed to be associated with reading fluency. The training approach was inspired by the multisensory integration deficit account of dyslexia, proposing a deficit in automation of L-SS associations [49]. Most treatments for dyslexia have a strong focus on accurate learning of L-SS correspondences [96,97,98]. However, as previously indicated, this may constitute only a first step towards the automation that L-SS mappings require to be efficiently used for fluent reading [99]. Thus, traditional interventions may not account for the time demands of multisensory integration at the neural level, which seems to take place within a very brief time window in skilled readers [76,77]. Accordingly, we employed a remediation procedure that addresses these automation demands by massive and intentional repetitive training of L-SS correspondences [40,69,70]. The training in automaticity was combined with explicit instruction to establish a strong understanding of phonemic and orthographic regularities as well as decoding skills. These two treatment elements are proposed to enable dyslexics to take advantage of increasing reading experience, facilitating the neural tuning processes required for fluent word identification. Our previous randomized controlled trial study showed that, first, the training led to reading fluency gains; second, L-SS knowledge was not associated with these gains; and, thirdly, initial mapping fluency strongly limited the acquisition of reading speed in untrained dyslexics but not in trained dyslexics [70].
Our ERP training studies investigated whether nine-year old dyslexic children would exhibit changes in crossmodal MMN and LN responses and occipito-temporal N170 following the systematic training of L-SS integration, and whether these changes would relate to reading improvements [40,69]. Regarding crossmodal MMN, we observed moderate improvements in the neural integration of letters and speech sounds after training, particularly in the timing of the late response (LN window) [40]. The early integration response (MMN window) seems to be more treatment resistant as no changes were found after training. Interestingly, however, individual differences in crossmodal MMN timing prior to training related to reading fluency both at pretest and posttest, and predicted gains in reading fluency. In other words, children who exhibited a longer lasting MMN response at pretest read more fluently already before the start of the training and remained more fluent at the second measurement. Importantly, these children also benefitted more from the L-SS training [40]. In relation to visual specialization, we asked whether training would result in changes in the N170 response to words [69]. Interestingly, children that significantly improved in reading fluency showed a significant reduction in visual N170 amplitudes for words relative to the pretest, while this effect was absent in children that did not improve. Furthermore, we found a moderately positive relation between gains in reading fluency and the training-related reduction in N170 responses.

3. Current Results: Additional Analysis on MMN and N170

3.1. Prediction of Reading Gains after Extended Practice

The previous training was largely based on the initial stages of a longer remedial program implemented at the IWAL institute focusing on L-SS associations [71]. The subsequent phases of the program advanced towards more syllabic and word knowledge, and learning of morphological elements that are productive for reading and spelling of polysyllabic words in Dutch (for more details of the subsequent modules of the program see [10]). From the dyslexic sample of our previous ERP training studies, 15 participants continued with the full program. The mean (SD) age of this subset of participants was 9.07 (0.45) years old. They followed an average (SD; range) of 36 (6; 26–44) additional sessions with an intensity of one session per week. Thus, in total, participants completed 70 (6; 60–78) sessions including the initial letter-speech sound training. Participants did not finish the program at the fixed pace (same number of sessions) as the program goal is to achieve a mastery level for each element of the program (i.e., around 80% of correct answers; [71]). For the present review, we considered data pertaining to reading fluency after the completion of the program. Reading fluency was measured by word (high and low frequency) and pseudoword reading subtests of the 3DM, a standard Dutch battery of reading related tests [78]. Figure 2 shows gains in reading fluency in normative scores after the initial training on L-SS associations (T2) and moderate gains (not significant) after completion of the full reading remediation program (T3). The reading scores for each test and time point and their comparisons are included as supplementary material (see Table A1). The improvement in reading fluency was substantial after the experimental program (T1 to T2: t (14) = −4.42, p = 0.001; see [70] for the complete assessment). The second part of the program, with the focus on morphology, was followed by moderate improvements in word reading fluency (high and low frequency words combined, T2 to T3: t (14) = −2.35, p = 0.017), but not for pseudowords (T2 to T3: t (14) = −0.66, p = 0.260). The more marked improvements after the first part of the program are in line with the specific goals of each of the training parts [100], and with our expectations that the first part would be crucial to break through the initial ‘fluency barrier’ and facilitate further improvements on the trained material, i.e., words rather than pseudowords.
The next step was to examine the reading gains from the beginning until the completion of the remedial program (T1 to T3) using our two ERP markers. We used standardized scores of reading fluency (T scores; M = 50, SD = 10) to account for potential differences in age and provide with a more clinically meaningful estimation of the progress after the treatment. We performed partial correlations to include the number of sessions in addition to the pretest reading scores as control variables.
First, we looked at crossmodal MMN latency at the initial measurement as it was significantly related to the outcomes of the first part of the reading training [40]. Crossmodal MMN latency at T1 was not significantly related to the combined word and pseudoword reading fluency at the end of the remedial program (rpart = 0.254, p = 0.201). However, crossmodal MMN latency at T1 was significantly related to pseudoword reading fluency at T3 (rpart = 0.514, p = 0.036; Figure 3A), while it was not related to high and low frequency word reading fluency (rpart = 0.139, p = 0.325). It should be noted, however, that the correlation with pseudoword reading fluency was not significant after applying a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons for three tests (αbonf = 0.016). Thus, this result should interpret with caution. The results could be due to the fact that pseudowords (meaningless non-existent words following phonotactic restrictions of the Dutch language [78]) cannot be visually identified directly via sight word reading, as they are not stored in the visual word form vocabulary, but they can be read by L-SS translation [18]. In this regard, it is possible that the focus of the second part of the training on morphological knowledge [71,100] could orient children from L-SS conversion strategies towards sight word reading [4].
Second, as we previously found that gains in reading fluency are accompanied by decrease of N170 amplitude for words over left parieto-occipital sites [69], we tested if this initial change of N170 during letter-speech sound integration stage of the reading training could predict further gains and outcomes. This analysis yielded a significant result, both for the combined word and pseudoword final fluency outcome of the training (rpart = −0.642, p = 0.009), and for the word (rpart = −0.666, p = 0.006) and pseudoword (rpart = −0.631, p = 0.010) reading fluency separately, indicating that a stronger reduction in N170 amplitudes was associated with better outcome of the reading training. These correlations remained significant after correcting for multiple comparisons (αbonf = 0.016). These results extend our previous findings and support the role of visual specialization, i.e., print tuning reflected by N170 changes, in reading fluency. Importantly, this print tuning followed L-SS integration training. Thus, we further investigated relations between letter-speech sound integration and visual specialization.

3.2. Relation between Crossmodal MMN and Visual N170

In our data, initial MMN latency, regarded as an indicator of multisensory integration, was significantly correlated with reading fluency. In addition, our visual word study showed that a decrease in N170 amplitudes for words at left occipito-temporal sites from T1 to T2 was correlated with gains in reading fluency. The proposed developmental trajectory of brain networks for reading suggests that successful letter-speech sound integration in multisensory areas facilitates specialization of visual areas for print. In order to examine this, we correlated the MMN latencies at pretest and the post-pretest differences in N170 amplitudes for words (see Figure 4). This correlation was significant (r = 0.610, p = 0.016) indicating a larger reduction of N170 amplitudes after training in those individuals that showed longer MMN latencies at pretest, the latter reflecting more crossmodal enhancement (see Section 2.1). This result offers partial support for our hypothesis, as it could indicate that dyslexics with less marked integration deficits were more capable of modulating their visual responses after training.

3.3. Summary of Results

The findings from our previous ERP studies [38,40,68,69] support the relevance of L-SS integration and visual specialization at the early phases of learning how to read in dyslexic children. In two of those studies, crossmodal MMN latency was related to reading fluency [38] and to reading improvements after training focused on gaining automation in L-SS associations [40]. In the present review, we show that crossmodal MMN latency also correlates with visual tuning for print, as reflected by changes in N170 responses after training, which were previously associated with reading fluency and training outcomes. This is in line with the idea that development of multisensory integration areas facilitates specialization of visual areas for fast letter and word identification [101].
In addition, we evaluated the relation of these ERPs with reading fluency gains in the longer term after completion of the next parts of the program. The results showed a trend (significant at the uncorrected level) associating crossmodal MMN latency to pseudoword reading, but there was no longer a relation between MMN and gains in word reading. A potential explanation of this apparent discrepancy is that as the focus of reading instruction moved to the word level (morphological structure), children adopt different reading strategies, such as sight word reading [4]. That is, they connect orthography directly to meaning [17,18]. At that stage, the importance of a successful L-SS integration is likely to be mainly observed on tasks, such as pseudoword reading, which arguably require a strategy based upon L-SS translation [18]. Visual specialization, on the other hand, would continue to be an important predictor of reading fluency irrespective of the reading strategy, as suggested by the significant correlations between N170 and both word and pseudoword reading in the current paper. This interpretation is in line with previous studies showing that word superiority effect extends to pseudowords (e.g., [102]).

4. Enhancing Letter-Speech Sound Binding by Associative, Implicit Learning

We previously discussed the beneficial effects of explicitly addressing L-SS associations in combination with intensive practice, and exposure to gain automation [70]. Our longitudinal ERP studies and current results support a differential and interactive involvement of audiovisual integration and visual specialization in dyslexics’ reading deficits and their response to the L-SS intervention. Another important element we have not yet considered is how audiovisual associations are learned in the first place, before they become automated and temporally integrated. Insights from research on the learning mechanisms allowing for establishing and consolidating L-SS associations could be applied to more efficient training designs and outcome predictors, in particular, given the increasing number of computer-based implicit trainings. General accounts of skill acquisition postulate that the more controlled metacognitive processing in beginners is gradually replaced by more automatic associative processing [103,104]. Accordingly, the L-SS elements are typically taught explicitly and need to be repeated intensively so as to facilitate transition from accurate and controlled processing to associative and automatic processing. Indeed, some studies have suggested that children initially establish L-SS correspondences in an explicit manner but then continue to learn these associations implicitly [105,106]. In this regard, it should be emphasized that learning the alphabetic code (letter knowledge) and learning to associate letters and speech sounds are considered as two different processes. The reading deficits observed in dyslexics seem to be linked to the latter [33,37].
Computer-based trainings based on massive exposure and associative learning are becoming increasingly popular. The aim of these trainings is primarily to improve the persistent reading fluency deficits in dyslexia. Their effectiveness and clinical potential was discussed in a recent review [107]. It should be noted that the authors of this review opted for combining implicit training with initial explicit instruction, which also constitutes a key feature of the treatment discussed in Section 2.2 [70]. Explicit instruction is proposed to act as a bootstrapping mechanism to increase automaticity. This notion is supported by the finding that enhanced reading accuracy after initial stages of specialized treatment, typically explicit, leads to later development of fluency after subsequent treatment stages [108].
In two recent studies, our group examined the diagnostic and prognostic value of a computer-based training of an artificial script [109,110]. The training consisted of a game in which new L-SS associations were implicitly learned, that is, learning the artificial orthography was not the explicit goal of the game. The outcome of the training was used as a dynamic assessment tool. While in a conventional static assessment, the individual’s current knowledge and/or skills are evaluated, dynamic assessment is adaptive and focusing on learning potential and cognitive modifiability. Within this context, trainings typically have a short time frame and contain feedback elements. The scores from the artificial L-SS associations training were able to discriminate between dyslexics and typical readers [110]. Interestingly, these scores could also explain a significant proportion of variance in response to a validated reading intervention program for dyslexia [109]. These studies illustrate the usefulness of the dynamic assessment approach in the context of implicit learning of artificial associations to identify new moderators of response to intervention. In addition, and more closely related to the focus of the present review, a recent neuroimaging study investigated explicit learning of artificial symbol-speech sound correspondences in kindergartners at varying risk of dyslexia [42]. The study demonstrated that a small set of artificial L-SS correspondences could be successfully learned by pre-reading children in less than 30 min. Most importantly, in line with our previous findings, children in that study showed modulation of their brain responses at the posterior systems for reading (temporal and occipito-temporal areas) that were related to their ability to learn the new associations.
The above findings could be interpreted within the multisensory deficit account of dyslexia. It should be noted, however, that besides letter (grapheme) knowledge and phonological skills, there are other cognitive abilities that could contribute to inefficient learning of novel L-SS associations in dyslexics. In fact, several authors proposed a more general learning deficit that is not limited to phonological processing. These authors consider dyslexia as mainly arising from impairments in the procedural learning system for language [111]. In the following sections we present an overview of studies examining dyslexia vis-à-vis higher-order processes that are relevant to associative learning; that is, the detection of statistical regularities and performance monitoring mechanisms (error detection and feedback processing).

5. Associative, Implicit Learning and Other Cognitive Processes

In order to efficiently learn and establish L-SS associations, we require a set of cognitive mechanisms that are not limited to the language domain. Some of these mechanisms pertain to bottom-up processes, more involved in implicit learning, while others are related to top-down processes more relevant to explicit learning. As discussed in previous section, both types of learning are proposed to interact during the acquisition of reading skills. Below we review findings from dyslexia studies focusing on different cognitive mechanisms that enable associative learning. The evidence that is currently available points at deficits in general learning and executive functions that may be present in dyslexic readers.

5.1. Implicit Learning

Previous studies have suggested that non-linguistic cognitive processes, such as the recognition of statistical properties of stimuli, may be important to implicit associative learning of L-SS correspondences [112,113]. Implicit learning paradigms have widely used the serial reaction time task [114]. In its implicit version, participants are asked to rapidly press a button that corresponds to the location at which a visually presented stimulus appears on screen. Unknown to participants, stimuli are presented in a repeated sequence; the implicit learning of that structure is reflected in faster reaction times across experimental blocks, but subjects are unable to explicitly describe the sequence. Previous studies reported poor performance of dyslexics on implicit serial reaction time tasks [115,116,117]. One of these studies also reported a significant correlation between reading skills and task performance [116]. The findings were interpreted to indicate impaired statistical learning in dyslexic readers. Interestingly, Hedenius and colleagues observed a procedural learning deficit in dyslexic readers and in children with specific language impairment that was related to the consolidation or automatization of the procedural knowledge rather than initial learning [111,118]. This suggests that impairments in dyslexics may also extend to effective long-term learning. Another study found no differences in learning rate, but longer response times in dyslexics, together with a correlation with reading scores that was absent in the control group [119]. Other studies using the implicit serial reaction time paradigm, however, failed to obtain differences in the performance between dyslexics and typical readers [120,121]. These inconsistencies in the literature were discussed in a meta-analysis [122]. The results of that analysis made two important points. First, there is sufficient evidence for considering poor implicit learning abilities as a risk factor for reading problems, and that the interaction between age of participants and methodological characteristics may account for differences between studies. The authors proposed that those differences point to compensatory mechanisms of the declarative memory system. Further, the differences in processing demands for statistical learning in different domains, e.g., audio, visual, tactile, but also audiovisual, were also highlighted in a recent review [123].
Other studies examined implicit learning deficits in dyslexia using an artificial grammar learning (AGL) paradigm [124,125,126,127,128,129], but see [117]. In brief, the AGL paradigm consists of observing sequences of symbols that follow a set of rules (which are not made explicit) and classifying novel sequences as legal or not, according to those rules. Pothos and Kirk [124] used that paradigm with two tasks that presented geometric shapes either serially, to encourage perception of individual items, or embedded, to encourage perception of the stimuli as a whole. The results showed that adult dyslexics performed equally well on both tasks, while controls underperformed dyslexics on the serial task. This was interpreted to reflect interference from explicit attentional mechanisms on the serial task in controls but not in dyslexic readers. More specifically, the authors of the study suggested that difficulties in processing individual elements of stimuli could prevent dyslexics from adopting explicit efforts to “decode” the stimuli in an item-by-item fashion. The study highlights how the interplay between implicit and explicit mechanisms in learning may differ in dyslexics. This was further investigated in a series of studies by Pavlidou and colleagues [125,126,127] in 9–12 year old children. The first study [127] used an AGL task with either implicit instructions (i.e., asking merely to observe training items) or explicit instructions (i.e., asking to memorize as many items as possible). In that study, dyslexics performed worse than controls in both experimental conditions; although they showed some sensitivity to structural regularities of stimuli, they seemed to have difficulties in abstracting this knowledge. This finding was supported by a second study that further controlled for working memory load of the task [126]. However, a study in dyslexic adults found impaired performance only in an implicit AGL task but not when explicit instructions were provided, suggesting an specific deficit in implicit sequential processes [128]. An additional experiment by Pavlidou and colleagues included a transfer task, in which items of a different shape set than the trained ones were tested [125]. The results supported their previous findings, suggesting problems in abstracting higher-order information across stimuli and tasks in the dyslexic group. An important factor in artificial grammar learning studies, especially those in children, is that of task difficulty. A recent study used the same AGL task with an additional manipulation of complexity, as measured by topological entropy [129]. The results showed that decreasing grammar complexity resulted in comparable performance between dyslexics and controls.
Finally, studies used an MMN paradigm (see Section 2) in which the phonotactic probabilities of stimuli are manipulated; that is, the sounds presented differed in their likelihood of occurrence in spoken language [130,131]. The MMN responses in this paradigm are expected to be stronger for stimuli with higher phonotactic probability [132]. Dyslexics showed less sensitivity than typical readers to such enhancement of MMN responses, which was interpreted to reflect a subtle deficit in auditory cortical tuning for phonemic regularities in natural speech [130,131]. Interestingly, some studies in typical readers suggested that lexical knowledge [133] and orthographic information [134] could influence perceptual learning of speech, referred to as phonetic recalibration. The latter study indicated a rapid adjustment of phoneme boundaries induced by the presentation of print-speech pairs [134], while dyslexic readers seemed to be impaired specifically on the print-speech material and not in the more general audiovisual condition, including lip-read [135]. Further studies using a similar paradigm in the context of letter and speech sounds combinations could provide an interesting point of convergence for findings on both phonological and audiovisual learning deficits in dyslexia.

5.2. Performance Monitoring

The evaluation of errors and feedback are cognitive control mechanisms instrumental in adapting and optimizing performance in a given task. These mechanisms may, thus, be critical to a successful transition from explicit to implicit and automated, associative learning, when mapping letters and speech sounds. The following paragraphs review a series of dyslexia ERP studies, suggesting deficits in the mechanisms involved in performance monitoring.

5.2.1. Error Processing

The notion of an error detection mechanism is central to theories of executive and cognitive control [136,137]. The efficient detection of erroneous responses is an essential element in various processes important for associative learning, such as performance monitoring and response activation and inhibition. This brain mechanism for error detection is reflected in the error-related negativity (ERN). The ERN has a fronto-central topography and a negative polarity, and is elicited around the first 0–160 ms after erroneous/correct responses. The sources of the ERN seem to be in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in the prefrontal cortex [138]. There are different theories about the functional significance of the ERN: most of them converge on the idea that ERN reflects to some extent a comparison or mismatch between the neural representations of the correct responses and those of the actual response [139,140]. A more recent account includes elements of reinforcement learning and proposes that the ERN may reflect a negative reinforcement signal expressed in the ACC via the mesencephalic dopamine system [141]. Accordingly, this signal is used to optimize performance on a specific task.
There are a few neurophysiological studies reporting deficits in dyslexics in the error detection mechanism, as reflected by the ERN during a lexical decision task [142,143,144,145,146]. One of those studies also reported changes in the ERN responses in adult dyslexics after a reading training [142]. Another study found reduced ERN amplitudes in non-compensated dyslexics compared to compensated dyslexics (i.e., slow readers with relatively well-preserved decoding abilities) [146]. The authors suggested the potential use of ERPs associated with executive functions to evaluate effectiveness of intervention.

5.2.2. Feedback Learning

Related to the ERN, the feedback-related negativity (FRN) is a negative potential elicited between 200 and 300 ms after the presentation of a feedback stimulus, at fronto-central locations. The FRN has been proposed as part of the same reinforcement learning system above [141]. The FRN response has been examined in combination with the P300, a centro-parietal cue-locked positive component thought to reflect attentive processes [147,148]. The FRN-P300 complex has been proposed to index learning ability [149,150]. A recent study examined these responses in dyslexics and controls during the early and late phases of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, which relies on both learning and executive functioning [151]. The task performance results showed comparable accuracy in both groups, but slower reaction times in dyslexics compared to controls. The electrophysiological results showed that FRN and P300 amplitudes were smaller during the early phases of the task in dyslexics compared to controls. With regard to the FRN, the initial amplitudes in dyslexics became comparable to those of typical readers during the later phase of the task. Regarding P300, responses in dyslexics decreased during the task in contrast to those of the control group. Based on previous studies, however, both FRN and P300 responses were expected to decrease through task performance due to learning effects and less novelty, respectively [147,148]. Thus, the neurophysiological results in Kraus and Horowitz-Kraus were interpreted by the authors to reflect lower baseline performance that was rapidly adjusted in the dyslexic group.

5.3. Conclusions from Associative Learning Studies

The findings reviewed in this section suggest that dyslexics and typical readers may differ in executive functions important for several aspects of associative learning. With regard to implicit learning, we found reports of impairments in bottom-up processes, such as procedural learning, detection of statistical regularities, and phonetic recalibration. In relation to more top-down and explicit processes, there is evidence suggesting that dyslexics may exhibit impairment in error detection and feedback learning mechanisms. These difficulties may be relevant at several stages of fluent reading skills acquisition. For example, they may hinder the establishment and consolidation of L-SS associations, but they may also limit the subsequent use of that information to generate word-specific orthographic representations and to learn exceptions and irregularities (e.g., the self-teaching hypothesis [26]). Furthermore, a central aspect in these mechanisms, especially in those relying on top-down processes, is that of attentional control. This is emphasized in recent attention-based models of associative learning [152,153]. Importantly, there is evidence associating dyslexia with sensory deficits in visual and visuo-spatial attention systems (e.g., [154,155]). The above evidence on executive functioning, associative learning and attentional processes in dyslexia may help in explaining the heterogeneity of the dyslexic population and the high comorbidity with other cognitive disorders, in particular, ADHD [156,157]. Future studies should examine how these mechanisms (attentional and those enabling associative learning) interact to produce the reading deficits observed in dyslexic individuals.

6. General Discussion and Future Research

The main goal of this paper was to discuss the combined role of the posterior brain systems for audiovisual integration and visual specialization in dyslexia, and to review findings from associative learning studies that, applied in the context of audiovisual learning, could benefit treatment studies.
We first reviewed and extended the findings of our previous electrophysiological studies. Those studies found two measures that were correlated with reading fluency in dyslexics: the MMN latency in an audiovisual oddball paradigm and the N170 responses to visually-presented words. The continuation of those studies used a pretest-posttest design and a L-SS training, and revealed that those ERP responses also correlated to reading fluency improvements after training [40,69]). In the present review, we included behavioral measurements obtained at a third time point, after children finished the complete remedial program. We extended our previous findings by showing that changes in N170 after the initial part of the treatment also correlated with reading fluency gains program completion, while MMN may still be related to pseudoword reading. We interpreted this pattern of findings to reflect the increasing importance of visual specialization for word recognition as reading fluency develops. This interpretation is related to the notion of a growing reliance on sight word reading with increasing reading expertise [99].
Furthermore, we examined how our two ERP markers were related to each other, and found a significant correlation between initial MMN latencies and changes in N170. This result is supportive of the proposed trajectory of the reading network specialization, namely, that successful L-SS integration facilitates specialization of visual areas. Multisensory areas responsible for letter-speech integration are strongly engaged in the first years of instruction, and interact with the anterior systems that are involved in slow and effortful decoding of new words. Subsequently, as reading expertise increases, the specialization of the ventral system for fast visual print recognition becomes more relevant to fluent word decoding. With regard to the development of the VWFA, strong word selectivity was reported in nine-year-old children [158]. Visual word specialization seems to develop in a relatively short period of time after learning the alphabetic script. A study in six-year-old children showed that word specific VWFA responses could emerged after only a few months of grapheme-phoneme correspondence training [57]. Additionally, similar studies in adults show increased ventral occipito-temporal activations for symbols of artificial scripts after just a few days of training [159,160,161,162]. Importantly, an inverted “U” trajectory has been proposed for VWFA responses [163,164]. Accordingly, activation of visual areas for reading strongly increases at the initial learning stages, but activation declines with reading expertise once print processing becomes more automatic and overlearned. In sum, the mutual interactions and specialization of different functional systems are essential in the development of the reading network. We propose that electrophysiological measures accounting for the functional relation between multisensory integration and visual areas (e.g., functional connectivity studies; [73]) may provide with additional markers of reading (dys)fluency and training outcomes in dyslexia.
There are some limitations in the use of N170 as a marker of visual specialization that should be acknowledged. The use of implicit word recognition tasks may not sufficiently constrain allocation of attentional resources and cognitive strategies during tasks, which can influence early visual components. Indeed, in many of the studies examining occipito-temporal responses to print in dyslexics, the experimental tasks allow for phonological processing of stimuli in addition to orthographic processing (see review in [165]). This has been emphasized in a previous study which intended to restrict as much as possible top-down effects of phonological processing by combining short stimulus duration at high presentation rate with a low-level detection task [166]. The study showed that even restricting high-order processing, dyslexics showed a deficit in print sensitivity of the occipito-temporal areas. The authors interpreted the finding as supporting deficits at bottom-up orthographic processing stages in dyslexics. It is then important to consider task influences when interpreting ERP results based the functionality of VWFA.
The second goal of the present review was to discuss studies investigating cognitive mechanisms involved in associative learning. That type of learning is important in the automation of L-SS associations and it is the focus of a growing number of game-based implicit-learning training programs. There is sufficient evidence to consider deficits in probabilistic learning, error monitoring and feedback learning, as potential contributors to impaired learning of L-SS associations in dyslexia. Such cognitive skills are, thus, relevant to remedial research. The use of dynamic assessment measurements of associative learning and executive functions could provide with additional predictors of treatment outcome and aid in the characterization of dyslexics’ cognitive deficits. In this context, we propose a neurophysiological study using EEG in an experimental task that focuses on learning novel symbol-phoneme associations through feedback after response. The task is based on learning of artificial audiovisual associations as in the study of Aravena and colleagues [34], but in the present case, the learning of stimulus-response associations is explicit, similarly to the probability-learning tasks used in the ERP literature [141]. This paradigm, which is currently being tested in our lab, will allow examining ERPs associated with error/correct responses and positive/negative feedback in the different stages of learning (i.e., at the beginning and at the end of each experimental block). We propose a dynamic balance between feedback-related and error-related brain responses as an additional measure to assess the ability to learn new associations. Moreover, the trial structure, derived from Crone et al. [167], will allow for the evaluation of oscillatory activity (e.g., [168]) and functional connectivity network metrics during tasks [169].
To conclude, learning of audiovisual associations constitutes a crucial step towards reading expertise. The dynamic interplay between L-SS effective integration and visual specialization should be considered to further characterize and predict reading fluency deficits in dyslexics at the different phases of reading instruction, development and trainings. In addition to phonological skills, deficits in other learning mechanisms related to associative learning could hinder the effective consolidation of fluent audiovisual associations, thus having an effect on performance and response to implicit trainings, aimed at gaining fluency. Finally, most research on dyslexia centers around the idea of a primary underlying deficit and the traditional diagnosis excludes other cognitive or neurological dysfunctions. However, the views and evidence emerging in the literature (e.g., attention, executive functions, specific brain areas vs. connectivity) suggest a heterogeneous group characterized by a cluster of deficits, rather than a homogenous group with a single underlying deficit (e.g., [170]).

Acknowledgments

We dedicate this paper to Leo Blomert, who passed away on 25 November 2012. His contributions to the initial stages of the project prior to his untimely death were significant. This project is part of the research program “Fluent reading neurocognitively decomposed: The case of dyslexia—HCMI 10-59” funded by the Netherlands Initiative Brain and Cognition, a part of the Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) under grant number 056-14-015. We would like to express our gratitude to all the children and parents for participating in the study. We are grateful to Suzanne van Grieken, Jitka Annen, Marlena van Langevelde, Mandy Meijer, Gert-Jan Munneke, and Jorinde Wesseling for their essential collaboration during data collection. We also thank Jasper Wijnen for his technical assistance. Finally, we thank the group of Piers L. Cornelissen for their contribution with the symbol font used in this study.

Author Contributions

M.W.v.d.M. and M.B. conceived and designed the experiments; G.F.G., G.Ž. and J.T. performed the experiments; G.F.G. and G.Ž. analyzed the data; J.T. and M.B. contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools; G.F.G., G.Ž., M.B. and M.W.v.d.M. contribute to the writing of the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Reading scores and training gains.
Table A1. Reading scores and training gains.
Dyslexics (N = 15)Training Effects
T1T2T3T2-T1T3-T2
MSDMSDMSDt (14)p-Value (One-Tailed)t (14)p-Value (One-Tailed)
3DM Word reading—accuracy (%) a
High Frequency91.987.4896.257.8697.431.742.170.0240.600.280
Low Frequency81.0917.3789.9113.4996.095.322.900.0062.400.015
Pseudowords69.0017.0976.4420.2180.9911.161.340.1000.960.176
Total [T] b31.6712.7539.4714.8538.339.682.410.015−0.300.385
3DM Word reading—fluency [T]
High Frequency29.935.7334.276.9438.006.604.800.0003.870.001
Low Frequency29.936.3933.936.7234.076.834.370.0000.120.451
Pseudowords29.735.5131.676.9532.475.681.780.0490.660.260
Total29.075.6232.676.7233.406.324.410.0000.940.182
a Raw scores; b T scores (M = 50, SD = 10).

References

  1. Zoccolotti, P.; De Luca, M.; Di Pace, E.; Gasperini, F.; Judica, A.; Spinelli, D. Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain Lang. 2005, 93, 369–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Seymour, P.H.K.; Aro, M.; Erskine, J.M. Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 2003, 94, 143–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Schmalz, X.; Marinus, E.; Coltheart, M.; Castles, A. Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2015, 22, 1614–1629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Ehri, L.C. Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Sci. Stud. Read. 2005, 9, 167–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Snowling, M.J. Early identification and interventions for dyslexia: A contemporary view. J. Res. Spec. Educ. Needs 2013, 13, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Blomert, L. Dyslexie in Nederland; Uitgeverij Nieuwezijds: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  7. Lyon, G.R.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. A definition of dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2003, 53, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Benjamin, C.F.A.; Gaab, N. What’s the story? The tale of reading fluency told at speed. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012, 33, 2572–2585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Shaywitz, S.E.S.; Shaywitz, B.B.A. Paying attention to reading: The neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Dev. Psychopathol. 2008, 20, 1329–1349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Tijms, J.; Hoeks, J.J.W.M.; Paulussen-Hoogeboom, M.C.; Smolenaars, A.J. Long-term effects of a psycholinguistic treatment for dyslexia. J. Res. Read. 2003, 26, 121–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Katzir, T.; Kim, Y.-S.; Wolf, M.; Morris, R.; Lovett, M.W. The Varieties of Pathways to Dysfluent Reading: Comparing Subtypes of Children With Dyslexia at Letter, Word, and Connected Text Levels of Reading. J. Learn. Disabil. 2008, 41, 47–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Leinonen, S.; Müller, K.; Leppänen, P.H.T.; Aro, M.; Ahonen, T.; Lyytinen, H. Heterogeneity in adult dyslexic readers: Relating processing skills to the speed and accuracy of oral text reading. Read. Writ. 2001, 14, 265–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Blomert, L.; Froyen, D. Multi-sensory learning and learning to read. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2010, 77, 195–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Morais, J.; Alegria, J.; Content, A. The relationships between segmental analysis and alphabetic literacy: An interactive view. Cah. Psychol. Cogn. 1987, 7, 415–438. [Google Scholar]
  15. Wimmer, H.; Landerl, K.; Linortner, R.; Hummer, P. The relationship of phonemic awareness to reading acquisition: More consequence than precondition but still important. Cognition 1991, 40, 219–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Whitney, C.; Cornelissen, P. Letter-position encoding and dyslexia. J. Res. Read. 2005, 28, 274–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Nation, K. Form-meaning links in the development of visual word recognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 3665–3674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Snowling, M.J. The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1980, 29, 294–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Nation, K.; Cocksey, J. The relationship between knowing a word and reading it aloud in children’s word reading development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2009, 103, 296–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Nazir, T.A.; Jacobs, A.M.; O’Regan, J.K. Letter legibility and visual word recognition. Mem. Cognit. 1998, 26, 810–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. New, B.; Ferrand, L.; Pallier, C.; Brysbaert, M. Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2006, 13, 45–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Rayner, K.; Pollatsek, A.; Schotter, E.R. Reading: Word Identification and Eye Movements. In Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology; Healy, A., Ed.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; pp. 548–577. [Google Scholar]
  23. Reicher, G.M. Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. J. Exp. Psychol. 1969, 81, 275–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. McCandliss, B.D.; Cohen, L.; Dehaene, S. The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 293–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Vaessen, A.; Blomert, L. Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 105, 213–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Share, D.L. Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1999, 72, 95–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Share, D.L. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 1995, 55, 151–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ischebeck, A.; Indefrey, P.; Usui, N.; Nose, I.; Hellwig, F.; Taira, M. Reading in a regular orthography: An FMRI study investigating the role of visual familiarity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 727–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Carlisle, J.F.; Katz, L.A. Effects of word and morpheme familiarity on reading of derived words. Read. Writ. 2006, 19, 669–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Wimmer, H.; Goswami, U. The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition 1994, 51, 91–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Snowling, M.J. Dyslexia as a Phonological Deficit: Evidence and Implications. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Rev. 1998, 3, 4–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ramus, F.; Szenkovits, G. What phonological deficit? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2008, 61, 129–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Blomert, L.; Willems, G. Is there a causal link from a phonological awareness deficit to reading failure in children at familial risk for dyslexia? Dyslexia 2010, 16, 300–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Aravena, S.; Snellings, P.; Tijms, J.; van der Molen, M.W. A lab-controlled simulation of a letter–speech sound binding deficit in dyslexia. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 115, 691–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Froyen, D.; Willems, G.; Blomert, L. Evidence for a specific cross-modal association deficit in dyslexia: An electrophysiological study of letter-speech sound processing. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 635–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Blau, V.; van Atteveldt, N.; Ekkebus, M.; Goebel, R.; Blomert, L. Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds links phonological and reading deficits in adult dyslexia. Curr. Biol. 2009, 19, 503–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Blau, V.; Reithler, J.; van Atteveldt, N.; Seitz, J.; Gerretsen, P.; Goebel, R.; Blomert, L. Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain 2010, 133, 868–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Žarić, G.; Fraga González, G.; Tijms, J.; van der Molen, M.W.; Blomert, L.; Bonte, M. Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds in dyslexic children scales with individual differences in reading fluency. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e110337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Moll, K.; Hasko, S.; Groth, K.; Bartling, J.; Schulte-Körne, G. Letter-sound processing deficits in children with developmental dyslexia: An ERP study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2016, 127, 1989–2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Žarić, G.; Fraga González, G.; Tijms, J.; van der Molen, M.W.; Blomert, L.; Bonte, M. Crossmodal deficit in dyslexic children: Practice affects the neural timing of letter-speech sound integration. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Jones, M.W.; Kuipers, J.-R.; Thierry, G. ERPs Reveal the Time-Course of Aberrant Visual-Phonological Binding in Developmental Dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Karipidis, I.; Pleisch, G.; Röthlisberger, M.; Hofstetter, C.; Dornbierer, D.; Stämpfli, P.; Brem, S. Neural initialization of audiovisual integration in prereaders at varying risk for developmental dyslexia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Seidenberg, M.S.; Tanenhaus, M.K. Orthographic effects on rhyme monitoring. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1979, 5, 546–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Dijkstra, T.; Roelofs, A.; Fieuws, S. Orthographic effects on phoneme monitoring. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 1995, 49, 264–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Ziegler, J.C.; Ferrand, L. Orthography shapes the perception of speech: The consistency effect in auditory word recognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1998, 5, 683–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Cone, N.E.; Burman, D.D.; Bitan, T.; Bolger, D.J.; Booth, J.R. Developmental changes in brain regions involved in phonological and orthographic processing during spoken language processing. Neuroimage 2008, 41, 623–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Bishop, D.V.M. Dyslexia: What’s the problem? Dev. Sci. 2006, 9, 256–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Wimmer, H.; Schurz, M. Dyslexia in regular orthographies: Manifestation and causation. Dyslexia 2010, 16, 283–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Blomert, L. The neural signature of orthographic-phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. Neuroimage 2011, 57, 695–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Hahn, N.; Foxe, J.J.; Molholm, S. Impairments of multisensory integration and cross-sensory learning as pathways to dyslexia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 47, 384–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Mann, V.; Wimmer, H. Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Read. Writ. 2002, 15, 653–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Schlaggar, B.L.; McCandliss, B.D. Development of neural systems for reading. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 30, 475–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Liberman, I.Y.; Shankweiler, D.; Fischer, F.W.; Carter, B. Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1974, 18, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Pugh, K.R.; Mencl, W.E.; Jenner, A.R.; Lee, J.R.; Katz, L.; Frost, S.J.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. Neuroimaging studies of reading development and reading disability. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2001, 16, 240–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L.; Sigman, M.; Vinckier, F. The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 335–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Castro-Caldas, A.; Petersson, K.M.; Reis, A.; Stone-Elander, S.; Ingvar, M. The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain 1998, 121, 1053–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Brem, S.; Bach, S.; Kucian, K.; Guttorm, T.K.; Martin, E.; Lyytinen, H.; Brandeis, D.; Richardson, U. Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter-speech sound correspondences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 7939–7944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Dehaene, S.; Pegado, F.; Braga, L.W.; Ventura, P.; Nunes Filho, G.; Jobert, A.; Kolinsky, R.; Morais, J.; Cohen, L. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 2010, 330, 1359–1364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Van Atteveldt, N.; Ansari, D. How symbols transform brain function: A review in memory of Leo Blomert. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2014, 3, 44–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Simos, P.G.; Breier, J.I.; Fletcher, J.M.; Foorman, B.R.; Castillo, E.M.; Papanicolaou, A.C. Brain mechanisms for reading words and pseudowords: An integrated approach. Cereb. Cortex 2002, 12, 297–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Jobard, G.; Crivello, F.; Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: A metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage 2003, 20, 693–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Brennan, C.; Cao, F.; Pedroarena-Leal, N.; McNorgan, C.; Booth, J.R. Reading acquisition reorganizes the phonological awareness network only in alphabetic writing systems. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34, 3354–3368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Monzalvo, K.; Dehaene-Lambertz, G. How reading acquisition changes children’s spoken language network. Brain Lang. 2013, 127, 356–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Sandak, R.; Mencl, W.; Frost, S.J.; Pugh, K.R. The neurobiological basis of skilled and impaired reading: Recent findings and new directions. Sci. Stud. Read. 2004, 8, 273–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Brunswick, N.; McCrory, E.; Price, C.J.; Frith, C.D.; Frith, U. Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics A search for Wernicke’s Wortschatz? Brain 1999, 122, 1901–1917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Paulesu, E.; Démonet, J.F.; Fazio, F.; McCrory, E.; Chanoine, V.; Brunswick, N.; Cappa, S.F.; Cossu, G.; Habib, M.; Frith, C.D.; et al. Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science 2001, 291, 2165–2167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Simos, P.G.; Breier, J.I.; Fletcher, J.M.; Bergman, E.; Papanicolaou, A.C. Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: A magnetic source imaging approach. Cereb. Cortex 2000, 10, 809–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Fraga González, G.; Žarić, G.; Tijms, J.; Bonte, M.; Blomert, L.; van der Molen, M.W. Brain-potential analysis of visual word recognition in dyslexics and typically reading children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Fraga González, G.; Žarić, G.; Tijms, J.; Bonte, M.L.M.; Blomert, L.; Leppänen, P.H.T.; van der Molen, M.W. Responsivity to dyslexia training indexed by the N170 amplitude of the brain potential elicited by word reading. Brain Cogn. 2016, 106, 42–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Fraga González, G.; Žarić, G.; Tijms, J.; Bonte, M.; Blomert, L.; van der Molen, M.W. A Randomized Controlled Trial on The Beneficial Effects of Training Letter-Speech Sound Integration on Reading Fluency in Children with Dyslexia. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0143914. [Google Scholar]
  71. Tijms, J. Effectiveness of computer-based treatment for dyslexia in a clinical care setting: Outcomes and moderators. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 873–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Fraga González, G.; van der Molen, M.J.W.; Žarić, G.; Bonte, M.M.; Tijms, J.; Blomert, L.; Stam, C.J. Graph analysis of EEG resting state functional networks in dyslexic readers. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2016, 127, 3165–3175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Žarić, G.; Correia, J.M.; Fraga González, G.; Tijms, J.; van der Molen, M.W.; Blomert, L.; Bonte, M. Altered patterns of directed connectivity within the reading network of dyslexic children and their relation to reading dysfluency. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 23, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Näätänen, R. The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismtch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology 2001, 38, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Näätänen, R.; Paavilainen, P.; Rinne, T.; Alho, K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 2544–2590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Froyen, D.; Bonte, M.; van Atteveldt, N.; Blomert, L. The long road to automation: Neurocognitive development of letter-speech sound processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009, 21, 567–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Froyen, D.; Van Atteveldt, N.; Bonte, M.; Blomert, L. Cross-modal enhancement of the MMN to speech-sounds indicates early and automatic integration of letters and speech-sounds. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 430, 23–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Blomert, L.; Vaessen, A.A. 3DM Differential Diagnostics for Dyslexia: Cognitive Analysis of Reading and Spelling; Boom Test: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  79. Bentin, S.; Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y.; Giard, M.H.; Echallier, J.F.; Pernier, J. ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: Time course and scalp distribution. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999, 11, 235–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Maurer, U.; Brem, S.; Bucher, K.; Brandeis, D. Emerging neurophysiological specialization for letter strings. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 1532–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Maurer, U.; Mccandliss, B.D. The Development of the Visual Expertise for Words: The Contribution of Electrophysiology; Grigorenko, E.L., Naples, A.J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  82. Maurer, U.; Schulz, E.; Brem, S.; Van der Mark, S.; Bucher, K.; Martin, E.; Brandeis, D. The development of print tuning in children with dyslexia: Evidence from longitudinal ERP data supported by fMRI. Neuroimage 2011, 57, 714–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Araújo, S.; Bramão, I.; Faísca, L.; Petersson, K.M.; Reis, A. Electrophysiological correlates of impaired reading in dyslexic pre-adolescent children. Brain Cogn. 2012, 79, 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Helenius, P.; Tarkiainen, A.; Cornelissen, P.L.; Hansen, P.C.; Salmelin, R. Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. Cereb. Cortex 1999, 9, 476–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Mahé, G.; Bonnefond, A.; Gavens, N.; Dufour, A.; Doignon-Camus, N. Impaired visual expertise for print in French adults with dyslexia as shown by N170 tuning. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 3200–3206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Korinth, S.P.; Sommer, W.; Breznitz, Z. Does silent reading speed in normal adult readers depend on early visual processes? evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Lang. 2012, 120, 15–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Maurer, U.; Brem, S.; Kranz, F.; Bucher, K.; Benz, R.; Halder, P.; Steinhausen, H.-C.; Brandeis, D. Coarse neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read. Neuroimage 2006, 33, 749–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Maurer, U.; Brem, S.; Bucher, K.; Kranz, F.; Benz, R.; Steinhausen, H.-C.; Brandeis, D. Impaired tuning of a fast occipito-temporal response for print in dyslexic children learning to read. Brain 2007, 130, 3200–3210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Bonte, M.L.M.; Blomert, L. Developmental dyslexia: ERP correlates of anomalous phonological processing during spoken word recognition. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2004, 21, 360–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Savill, N.J.; Thierry, G. Reading for sound with dyslexia: Evidence for early orthographic and late phonological integration deficits. Brain Res. 2011, 1385, 192–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Ramus, F.; Ahissar, M. Developmental dyslexia: The difficulties of interpreting poor performance, and the importance of normal performance. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2012, 29, 104–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Neuhoff, N.; Bruder, J.; Bartling, J.; Warnke, A.; Remschmidt, H.; Müller-Myhsok, B.; Schulte-Körne, G. Evidence for the late MMN as a neurophysiological endophenotype for dyslexia. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e34909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  93. Shestakova, A.; Huotilainen, M.; Čeponienė, R.; Cheour, M. Event-related potentials associated with second language learning in children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2003, 114, 1507–1512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Nash, H.M.; Gooch, D.; Hulme, C.; Mahajan, Y.; McArthur, G.; Steinmetzger, K.; Snowling, M.J. Are the literacy difficulties that characterize developmental dyslexia associated with a failure to integrate letters and speech sounds? Dev. Sci. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Dujardin, T.; Etienne, Y.; Contentin, C.; Bernard, C.; Largy, P.; Mellier, D.; Lalonde, R.; Rebaï, M. Behavioral performances in participants with phonological dyslexia and different patterns on the N170 component. Brain Cogn. 2011, 75, 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Alexander, A.W.; Slinger-Constant, A.-M. Current status of treatments for dyslexia: Critical review. J. Child Neurol. 2004, 19, 744–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Gabrieli, J.D.E. Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science 2009, 325, 280–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Wolff, U. Effects of a randomised reading intervention study: An application of structural equation modelling. Dyslexia 2011, 17, 295–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Ehri, L.C. Phases of acquisition in learning to read words and implications for teaching. In Word Recognition in Beginning Literacy; Stainthorp, R., Tomlinson, P., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2002; pp. 7–28. [Google Scholar]
  100. Tijms, J. A Process-Oriented Evaluation of a Computerised Treatment for Dyslexia. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 767–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. McCandliss, B.D.; Noble, K.G. The development of reading impairment: A cognitive neuroscience model. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2003, 9, 196–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Baron, J.; Thurston, I. An analysis of the world-superiority effect. Cogn. Psychol. 1973, 4, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Davydov, V.V. The state of research on learning activity. J. Russ. East Eur. Psychol. 1995, 33, 55–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Schneider, W. Controlled & automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cogn. Sci. 2003, 27, 525–559. [Google Scholar]
  105. Folia, V.; Uddén, J.; Forkstam, C.; Ingvar, M.; Hagoort, P.; Petersson, K.M. Implicit learning and dyslexia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1145, 132–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Gombert, J. Implicit and explicit learning to read: Implication as for subtypes of dyslexia. Curr. Psychol. Lett. 2003, 10, 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  107. Aravena, S.; Tijms, J. Reading Fluency and dyslexia: Innovative developments in the role of associative learning and repetitive exposure in skill acquisition. In Educational Psychology: Cognition and Learning, Individual Differences, and Motivation; Larson, J.E., Ed.; Nova Science: Hauppage, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 113–141. [Google Scholar]
  108. Tijms, J. The Development of Reading Accuracy and Reading Rate during Treatment of Dyslexia. Educ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 273–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Aravena, S.; Tijms, J.; Snellings, P.; van der Molen, M.W. Predicting responsiveness to intervention in dyslexia using dynamic assessment. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2016, 49, 209–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Aravena, S.; Tijms, J.; Snellings, P.; van der Molen, M.W. Predicting individual differences in reading and spelling skill with an artificial script-based letter-speech sound training. J. Learn. Disabil. 2017, in press. [Google Scholar]
  111. Hedenius, M.; Persson, J.; Alm, P.A.; Ullman, M.T.; Howard, J.H.; Howard, D.V.; Jennische, M. Impaired implicit sequence learning in children with developmental dyslexia. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 3924–3935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Harm, M.W.; Seidenberg, M.S. Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychol. Rev. 1999, 106, 491–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Frost, R.; Siegelman, N.; Narkiss, A.; Afek, L. What predicts successful literacy acquisition in a second language? Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1243–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Nissen, M.J.; Bullemer, P. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cogn. Psychol. 1987, 19, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Howard, J.H.; Howard, D.V.; Japikse, K.C.; Eden, G.F. Dyslexics are impaired on implicit higher-order sequence learning, but not on implicit spatial context learning. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 1131–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Gabay, Y.; Thiessen, E.D.; Holt, L.L. Impaired Statistical Learning in Developmental Dyslexia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2015, 58, 934–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Gabay, Y.; Schiff, R.; Vakil, E. Dissociation between online and offline learning in developmental dyslexia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2012, 34, 279–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Hedenius, M.; Persson, J.; Tremblay, A.; Adi-Japha, E.; Veríssimo, J.; Dye, C.D.; Alm, P.; Jennische, M.; Bruce Tomblin, J.; Ullman, M.T. Grammar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits in children with Specific Language Impairment. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2362–2375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Vakil, E.; Lowe, M.; Goldfus, C. Performance of Children With Developmental Dyslexia on Two Skill Learning Tasks—Serial Reaction Time and Tower of Hanoi Puzzle A Test of the Specific Procedural Learning Difficulties Theory. J. Learn. Disabil. 2015, 48, 471–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Kelly, S.W.; Griffiths, S.; Frith, U. Evidence for implicit sequence learning in dyslexia. Dyslexia 2002, 8, 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Rüsseler, J.; Gerth, I.; Münte, T.F. Implicit learning is intact in adult developmental dyslexic readers: Evidence from the serial reaction time task and artificial grammar learning. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2006, 28, 808–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Lum, J.A.G.; Ullman, M.T.; Conti-Ramsden, G. Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia: Evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 3460–3476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Frost, R.; Armstrong, B.C.; Siegelman, N.; Christiansen, M.H. Domain generality versus modality specificity: The paradox of statistical learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Pothos, E.M.; Kirk, J. Investigating learning deficits associated with dyslexia. Dyslexia 2004, 10, 61–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Pavlidou, E.V.; Williams, J.M. Implicit learning and reading: Insights from typical children and children with developmental dyslexia using the artificial grammar learning (AGL) paradigm. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014, 35, 1457–1472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Pavlidou, E.V.; Kelly, M.L.; Williams, J.M. Do children with developmental dyslexia have impairments in implicit learning? Dyslexia 2010, 16, 143–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Pavlidou, E.V.; Williams, J.M.; Kelly, L.M. Artificial grammar learning in primary school children with and without developmental dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2009, 59, 55–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Kahta, S.; Schiff, R. Implicit learning deficits among adults with developmental dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2016, 66, 235–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Katan, P.; Kahta, S.; Sasson, A.; Schiff, R. Performance of children with developmental dyslexia on high and low topological entropy artificial grammar learning task. Ann. Dyslexia 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Bonte, M.L.M.; Poelmans, H.; Blomert, L. Deviant neurophysiological responses to phonological regularities in speech in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 1427–1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Noordenbos, M.W.; Segers, E.; Mitterer, H.; Serniclaes, W.; Verhoeven, L. Deviant neural processing of phonotactic probabilities in adults with dyslexia. Neuroreport 2013, 24, 746–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Bonte, M.L.; Mitterer, H.; Zellagui, N.; Poelmans, H.; Blomert, L. Auditory cortical tuning to statistical regularities in phonology. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2005, 116, 2765–2774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Norris, D.; McQueen, J.M.; Cutler, A. Perceptual learning in speech. Cogn. Psychol. 2003, 47, 204–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Keetels, M.; Schakel, L.; Bonte, M.; Vroomen, J. Phonetic recalibration of speech by text. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 2016, 78, 938–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Keetels, M.; Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands. Unpublished work. 2017.
  136. Ridderinkhof, K.R.; van den Wildenberg, W.P.M.; Segalowitz, S.J.; Carter, C.S. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 2004, 56, 129–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  137. Yeung, N.; Botvinick, M.M.; Cohen, J.D. The Neural Basis of Error Detection: Conflict Monitoring and the Error-Related Negativity. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 111, 931–959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Nieuwenhuis, S.; Ridderinkhof, K.R.; Talsma, D.; Coles, M.G.H.; Holroyd, C.B.; Kok, A.; van der Molen, M.W. A computational account of altered error processing in older age: Dopamine and the error-related negativity. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2002, 2, 19–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Falkenstein, M.; Hoormann, J.; Christ, S.; Hohnsbein, J. ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: A tutorial. Biol. Psychol. 2000, 51, 87–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Coles, M.G.; Scheffers, M.K.; Holroyd, C.B. Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing. Biol. Psychol. 2001, 56, 173–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Holroyd, C.B.; Coles, M.G. The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 2002, 109, 679–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  142. Horowitz-Kraus, T. Improvement of the Error-detection Mechanism in Adults with Dyslexia Following Reading Acceleration Training. Dyslexia 2016, 22, 173–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Horowitz-Kraus, T. Does Development Affect the Error-Related Negativity of Impaired and Skilled Readers?: An ERP Study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011, 36, 914–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Horowitz-Kraus, T.; Breznitz, Z. Can the Error Detection Mechanism Benefit from Training the Working Memory? A Comparison between Dyslexics and Controls—An ERP Study. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e7141. [Google Scholar]
  145. Horowitz-Kraus, T.; Breznitz, Z. An error-detection mechanism in reading among dyslexic and regular readers—An ERP study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008, 119, 2238–2246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Horowitz-Kraus, T.; Breznitz, Z. Compensated Dyslexics Have a More Efficient Error Detection System Than Noncompensated Dyslexics. J. Child Neurol. 2012, 28, 1266–1276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. Sailer, U.; Fischmeister, F.P.S.; Bauer, H. Effects of learning on feedback-related brain potentials in a decision-making task. Brain Res. 2010, 1342, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Polich, J. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 2128–2148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Bellebaum, C.; Daum, I. Learning-related changes in reward expectancy are reflected in the feedback-related negativity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2008, 27, 1823–1835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. Groen, Y.; Wijers, A.A.; Mulder, L.J.M.; Minderaa, R.B.; Althaus, M. Physiological correlates of learning by performance feedback in children: A study of EEG event-related potentials and evoked heart rate. Biol. Psychol. 2007, 76, 174–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Kraus, D.; Horowitz-Kraus, T. The effect of learning on feedback-related potentials in adolescents with dyslexia: An EEG-ERP study. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e100486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Kruschke, J.K. Toward a Unified Model of Attention in Associative Learning. J. Math. Psychol. 2001, 45, 812–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Le Pelley, M.E. The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. B 2010, 57, 193–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  154. Facoetti, A.; Paganoni, P.; Turatto, M.; Marzola, V.; Mascetti, G.G. Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex 2000, 36, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Valdois, S.; Bosse, M.-L.; Tainturier, M.-J. The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia 2004, 10, 339–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Germanò, E.; Gagliano, A.; Curatolo, P. Comorbidity of ADHD and dyslexia. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2010, 35, 475–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  157. Willcutt, E.G.; Pennington, B.F. Comorbidity of Reading Disability and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Differences by Gender and Subtype. J. Learn. Disabil. 2000, 33, 179–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  158. Monzalvo, K.; Fluss, J.; Billard, C.; Dehaene, S.; Dehaene-Lambertz, G. Cortical networks for vision and language in dyslexic and normal children of variable socio-economic status. Neuroimage 2012, 61, 258–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  159. Hashimoto, R.; Sakai, K.L. Learning letters in adulthood: Direct visualization of cortical plasticity for forming a new link between orthography and phonology. Neuron 2004, 42, 311–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Perrone-Bertolotti, M.; Vidal, J.R.; de Palma, L.; Hamamé, C.M.; Ossandon, T.; Kahane, P.; Minotti, L.; Bertrand, O.; Lachaux, J.P. Turning visual shapes into sounds: Early stages of reading acquisition revealed in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex. Neuroimage 2014, 90, 298–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Xue, G.; Chen, C.; Jin, Z.; Dong, Q. Language experience shapes fusiform activation when processing a logographic artificial language: An fMRI training study. Neuroimage 2006, 31, 1315–1326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  162. Mei, L.; Xue, G.; Lu, Z.L.; He, Q.; Zhang, M.; Xue, F.; Chen, C.; Dong, Q. Orthographic transparency modulates the functional asymmetry in the fusiform cortex: An artificial language training study. Brain Lang. 2013, 125, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  163. Price, C.J.; Devlin, J.T. The interactive account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 246–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Maurer, U.; Zevin, J.D.; McCandliss, B.D. Left-lateralized N170 effects of visual expertise in reading: Evidence from Japanese syllabic and logographic scripts. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 20, 1878–1891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  165. Richlan, F.; Kronbichler, M.; Wimmer, H. Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. Neuroimage 2011, 56, 1735–1742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  166. Kronschnabel, J.; Schmid, R.; Maurer, U.; Brandeis, D. Visual print tuning deficits in dyslexic adolescents under minimized phonological demands. Neuroimage 2013, 74, 58–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  167. Crone, E.A.; Jennings, J.R.; van der Molen, M.W. Developmental change in feedback processing as reflected by phasic heart rate changes. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 40, 1228–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  168. Klimesch, W.; Doppelmayr, M.; Wimmer, H.; Gruber, W.; Röhm, D.; Schwaiger, J.; Hutzler, F. Alpha and beta band power changes in normal and dyslexic children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001, 112, 1186–1195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  169. Vourkas, M.; Micheloyannis, S.; Simos, P.G.; Rezaie, R.; Fletcher, J.M.; Cirino, P.T.; Papanicolaou, A.C. Dynamic task-specific brain network connectivity in children with severe reading difficulties. Neurosci. Lett. 2011, 488, 123–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  170. Pennington, B.F.; Santerre–Lemmon, L.; Rosenberg, J.; MacDonald, B.; Boada, R.; Friend, A.; Leopold, D.R.; Samuelsson, S.; Byrne, B.; Willcutt, E.G.; et al. Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2012, 121, 212–214. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Crossmodal MMN paradigm (A); and visual word recognition paradigm consisting of a one-back task with words and symbol strings (B).
Figure 1. Crossmodal MMN paradigm (A); and visual word recognition paradigm consisting of a one-back task with words and symbol strings (B).
Brainsci 07 00010 g001
Figure 2. Reading fluency scores (3DM total T scores; words and pseudowords combined) before starting training (T1), after the letter-speech sound training (T2), and after completion of the full remedial program (T3).
Figure 2. Reading fluency scores (3DM total T scores; words and pseudowords combined) before starting training (T1), after the letter-speech sound training (T2), and after completion of the full remedial program (T3).
Brainsci 07 00010 g002
Figure 3. (A) Crossmodal MMN latency at pretest (T1) was significantly related to standardized pseudoword reading fluency scores at the end of the training (T3) when controlling for pseudoword reading fluency at T1 and number of sessions. (B) Decrease in N170 amplitude from pretest (T1) to the end of the first part of the letter-speech sound training (T2) was significantly related to the combined word and pseudoword, and (C) word and (D) pseudoword reading fluency scores at the end of the training (T3) when controlling for the appropriate fluency scores at T1 and number of sessions.
Figure 3. (A) Crossmodal MMN latency at pretest (T1) was significantly related to standardized pseudoword reading fluency scores at the end of the training (T3) when controlling for pseudoword reading fluency at T1 and number of sessions. (B) Decrease in N170 amplitude from pretest (T1) to the end of the first part of the letter-speech sound training (T2) was significantly related to the combined word and pseudoword, and (C) word and (D) pseudoword reading fluency scores at the end of the training (T3) when controlling for the appropriate fluency scores at T1 and number of sessions.
Brainsci 07 00010 g003
Figure 4. Crossmodal MMN latency at pretest (T1) was related to the N170 amplitude decrease from pretest (T1) to the end of the first part of the training, which was aimed at letter-speech sound integration (T2). A change towards positive values along the y-axis refers to a decrease in the N170 amplitude.
Figure 4. Crossmodal MMN latency at pretest (T1) was related to the N170 amplitude decrease from pretest (T1) to the end of the first part of the training, which was aimed at letter-speech sound integration (T2). A change towards positive values along the y-axis refers to a decrease in the N170 amplitude.
Brainsci 07 00010 g004

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fraga González, G.; Žarić, G.; Tijms, J.; Bonte, M.; Van der Molen, M.W. Contributions of Letter-Speech Sound Learning and Visual Print Tuning to Reading Improvement: Evidence from Brain Potential and Dyslexia Training Studies. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 10. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci7010010

AMA Style

Fraga González G, Žarić G, Tijms J, Bonte M, Van der Molen MW. Contributions of Letter-Speech Sound Learning and Visual Print Tuning to Reading Improvement: Evidence from Brain Potential and Dyslexia Training Studies. Brain Sciences. 2017; 7(1):10. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci7010010

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fraga González, Gorka, Gojko Žarić, Jurgen Tijms, Milene Bonte, and Maurits W. Van der Molen. 2017. "Contributions of Letter-Speech Sound Learning and Visual Print Tuning to Reading Improvement: Evidence from Brain Potential and Dyslexia Training Studies" Brain Sciences 7, no. 1: 10. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci7010010

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop