Next Article in Journal
Brain White Matter: A Substrate for Resilience and a Substance for Subcortical Small Vessel Disease
Next Article in Special Issue
Is It Speech or Song? Effect of Melody Priming on Pitch Perception of Modified Mandarin Speech
Previous Article in Journal
Gambling Problems and Alexithymia: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Domain-Specific Expectations in Music Segmentation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Event-Related Potential Evidence of Implicit Metric Structure during Silent Reading

by
Mara Breen
1,*,
Ahren B. Fitzroy
1,2 and
Michelle Oraa Ali
1,3
1
Department of Psychology and Education, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA 01075, USA
2
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
3
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 17 July 2019 / Revised: 31 July 2019 / Accepted: 5 August 2019 / Published: 8 August 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Advances in the Neurocognition of Music and Language)

Abstract

:
Under the Implicit Prosody Hypothesis, readers generate prosodic structures during silent reading that can direct their real-time interpretations of the text. In the current study, we investigated the processing of implicit meter by recording event-related potentials (ERPs) while participants read a series of 160 rhyming couplets, where the rhyme target was always a stress-alternating noun–verb homograph (e.g., permit, which is pronounced PERmit as a noun and perMIT as a verb). The target had a strong–weak or weak–strong stress pattern, which was either consistent or inconsistent with the stress expectation generated by the couplet. Inconsistent strong–weak targets elicited negativities between 80–155 ms and 325–375 ms relative to consistent strong–weak targets; inconsistent weak–strong targets elicited a positivity between 365–435 ms relative to consistent weak–strong targets. These results are largely consistent with effects of metric violations during listening, demonstrating that implicit prosodic representations are similar to explicit prosodic representations.

1. Introduction

According to the Implicit Prosody Hypothesis [1,2,3], readers generate imagined representations of prosodic structure during silent reading that are similar to the explicit prosodic representations that readers produce when reading aloud. This hypothesis has been supported by behavioral evidence demonstrating similarity between real and imagined representations of a variety of prosodic phenomena, including intonation, phrasing, stress, and meter [4,5]. For example, evidence for implicit intonational structure is provided by the fact that readers are faster to recognize target words that are produced aloud with a previously imagined intonation contour [6,7]. Readers impose implicit phrase boundaries in sentences that are long enough to have a phrase break [8] and tend to balance the size of adjacent phrases even during silent reading [9,10], providing evidence for implicit prosodic phrasing. Readers take longer to silently read words with two stressed syllables than words with one stressed syllable [11], and take longer to read sentences in which a local lexical stress pattern mismatches the predicted metric structure as determined by prior sentence material [12,13,14,15,16], providing evidence for an implicit metric structure. Although these behavioral similarities between patterns associated with explicit and implicit prosody provide indirect support for implicit prosodic representations, they cannot tell us to what extent implicit prosodic representations are processed similarly to explicit prosodic representations. In the current study, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate the processing of implicit prosodic representations, and how it compares to that of explicit prosody.

1.1. Behavioral Studies of Explicit and Implicit Linguistic Metric Representation

The specific focus of the current study is the similarity between implicit and explicit metric processing. For this investigation, we exploit metrical regularity in English; English is a stress-timed language, meaning that speakers produce temporally regularized sequences of strong (stressed) and weak (unstressed) syllables. The metric structure in stress-timed languages is conveyed by the timing of strong syllables [17,18]. There are constraints on the ordering of strong and weak beats in stress-timed languages, as strong beats tend to occur at regular intervals [19], speakers avoid clashes of strong beats and lapses of weak beats [20], and under some circumstances, speakers shift the location of stress on words to maintain metric regularity (e.g., thirTEEN MEN → THIRteen MEN) [18]. Speakers signal strong syllables in speech with a variety of acoustic cues, including longer duration and higher intensity [21,22,23,24]. Strong syllables also hold a privileged position in auditory language comprehension; listeners are faster to detect phonemes in stressed syllables [25], lexical access is more disrupted by the mispronunciation of stressed syllables than unstressed syllables [26], and listeners tend to interpret stressed syllables as word onsets [27,28]. Moreover, listeners use the pattern of strong and weak syllables to predict what words will come next [29], and to resolve lexical ambiguity [30,31,32,33].
Like speakers and listeners, there is evidence that readers are also sensitive to a metric structure. For example, readers spend more time fixating four-syllable words with two stressed syllables (e.g., RAdiAtion) than four-syllable words with one stressed syllable (e.g., geOmetry) [11]. In silent reading, syntactically ambiguous sentences are more likely to be resolved in ways that maintain alternating strong and weak syllables [14,15]. In the study that serves as the inspiration for the current study, Breen and Clifton tracked participants’ eye movements as they read limericks designed to induce readers to generate strong expectations about the stress pattern of upcoming words [13]. The target word in the critical items, which was always the final word of the second line of the limerick, was a stress-alternating noun–verb homograph; these words are realized with strong–weak (SW) stress as a noun (e.g., PERmit), but weak–strong (WS) stress as a verb (e.g., perMIT) [29]. In this way, the target was either SW or WS, and this lexical stress pattern was either consistent or inconsistent with the metric structure of the limerick (see Table 1). Throughout this paper, we will refer to the occurrence of an inconsistent SW word when a WS word is predicted as a strong–weak (SW) violation, and to the occurrence of an inconsistent WS word when a SW word is predicted as a weak–strong (WS) violation.
Breen and Clifton predicted that readers would encounter difficulty whenever the stress pattern of the target word mismatched the pattern of the limerick. However, they only observed an effect of metric mismatch for WS violations (e.g., Table 1D); reading times for SW violations (e.g., Table 1B) did not differ from those of consistent SW words. Breen and Clifton argued that these results reflect the uneven distribution of SW and WS words in the English lexicon; 85–90% of content words in English have an initial stressed syllable [34]. Specifically, there is a minimal cost to encountering a SW word in a context where a WS word is predicted because SW is the default stress pattern. Identifying a WS word in a context that predicts SW, on the other hand, is costly because of both the conflict with context and the lower base frequency of the WS pattern. This interpretation is supported by previous work showing that auditory word identification is more disrupted when a canonically SW word is pronounced as WS, than when a canonically WS word is pronounced as SW [35]. Moreover, the observed effect was not on initial reading times, but only on the combined duration of fixations on the target word and time spent rereading earlier sentence material. The latency of this effect, therefore, suggests that the WS violation did not disrupt initial reading times but required later reanalysis.

1.2. Event-Related Potential Studies of Explicit Linguistic Metric Processing

In ERP investigations of explicit metric processing during speech perception, multiple methods have been used to investigate metric violations. One major source of variation among these studies is whether the metric violation is determined by the lexical stress pattern of the word in isolation or only by the context in which the word occurs. In studies of the first variety, researchers presented multisyllabic words auditorily with the correct or incorrect stress pattern either in isolation [36,37,38] or in a sentence context [39,40]. In studies of the second variety, researchers established a context that created an expectation of a specific metric pattern, then presented a target that had the correct metric pattern in isolation but was consistent or inconsistent with the expected pattern created by the context. One such paradigm used word strings to create metric context: listeners heard a string of three or four prime words with the same lexical stress pattern (all SW or all WS, e.g., BANKer, HELPful, PARty or moRALE, emBRACE, deLIGHT) followed by a target word with the same stress pattern as the primes or the opposite pattern [41,42]. In another such paradigm, participants heard sentences with a consistent metric structure including a target which was either consistent or inconsistent with the established pattern [43,44,45,46,47,48] (e.g., stress clash in “The chamPAGNE COCKtails are very delicious”). A final method used cross-modal information to inform prosodic interpretation, as in [49] where participants viewed pictures which disambiguated the meaning of semantically ambiguous two-syllable strings like greenhouse, which are disambiguated by stress patterns (GREENhouse vs. green HOUSE).
Regardless of the type of manipulation, these ERP studies demonstrate that encountering metric violations while listening generally gives rise to an early negativity between 250 and 500 ms [36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48]. However, this early effect is not consistent across studies, in terms of timing and polarity. Some of the variance can be explained by the different responses to SW violations and WS violations in two-syllable words; SW violations, where a SW word appears when a WS word is predicted, typically elicit an early negativity [41,42,43,44,45,46,47,49]. The results are more mixed for WS violations, where a WS word appears when a SW word is predicted, which elicit an early negativity in some cases [41,42] but has also been shown to elicit an early positivity relative to predicted metric patterns [36,37,40,42,48]. In two studies, both SW and WS violations elicited an early negativity, but the negativity to SW violations peaked earlier [41,49].
Additionally, explicit metric violations have often been shown to elicit a late positivity between 500 and 1000 ms [36,37,38,39,40,42,43,44,46,48]. In contrast to the early time window, this later effect does not seem to differ in polarity or timing as a function of target lexical stress pattern. However, its presence is dependent on the experimental task; in cases where the participants’ task is to make an explicit assessment of the accuracy of the metric structure of the target, that target usually elicits a late positivity [36,37,38,46,48,49], though this is not always the case [45,47]. In contrast, if the participants’ task does not include a specific assessment of the metric structure, a late positivity is absent [41]. Indeed, in cases where the explicitness of a metric judgment is varied within the experiment, a late positivity is generally evident only when the task requires this judgment [39,40,42,43,44].
Despite some variation across studies, these neural effects of metric inconsistency appear to be distinct from the neural effects of either syntactic or semantic violations. Syntactic violations typically elicit a biphasic response consisting of a left-lateralized anterior negativity peaking around 300 ms (LAN) and a posterior positivity peaking around 600 ms after stimulus onset (P600/LPC) [50]. A simultaneous test of metric and syntactic violations reported distinct negativities for each violation type however, with the negativity to metric violations occurring earlier than the negativity evoked by syntactic violations (which was interpreted as a LAN) [43]. Semantic violations typically elicit a parietally-maximal negativity around 400 ms (N400) [51]. Although some authors have interpreted the early negativity elicited by metric violations as an N400 [39,40], this metric negativity has been observed in response to illegal stress shifts in pseudowords which have no lexico-semantic content and should not result in an N400 [45]. Further, semantic incongruity and metric incongruity have been shown to modulate the amplitude of an early negativity differently when considered in the same design, even by authors who categorize deviations from a predicted metric structure as N400 effects [39,40]. Finally, [44] observed that simultaneous metric and semantic violations lead to a larger negativity than that observed for semantic violation alone, and [52] used neuroimaging to demonstrate that the responses to semantic and metric violations have different neural generators, providing evidence that metric violations are not simply processed as semantic violations.

1.3. Event-Related Potential Studies of Implicit Linguistic Metric Processing

ERPs have also been used to explore implicit metric representations during silent reading. In one study, readers were presented with strings of four two-syllable English prime words with consistent lexical stress patterns, followed by a target word that was consistent or inconsistent with the stress pattern of the previous words [53]. Both SW and WS violations resulted in a larger fronto-central negativity from 250–400 ms after word onset, relative to words with a predicted stress pattern. In addition, all SW targets, whether consistent or inconsistent with the context, elicited a larger negativity (350–450 ms after word onset) than WS targets. In another study exploring silent metric processing in word lists, readers were presented with strings of three two-syllable German prime words followed by a SW or WS target. In this case, there were no observable ERP differences for SW violations, but WS violations were more positive than correct WS targets in three time windows: between 250–400, 400–600, and 600–800 ms after target onset [54]. A final study presented participants with an auditory tone sequence with a SW or WS pattern followed by a visually presented two-syllable English word which was consistent or inconsistent with the tone sequence stress pattern [55]. The results demonstrated a larger negativity from 300–700 ms after target presentation for SW violations compared to correct SW targets, but no significant ERP effect for WS violations. In general, these studies demonstrate that, similar to explicit metric violations, implicit metric violations often evoke an early negativity that is more reliably observed for SW than WS violations. Moreover, two of these studies are consistent with results from explicit meter studies in that when the task does not require an explicit metric judgment (and none of these did; rather, participants’ task was to make an old/new judgment of the target [53], a lexical decision judgment [55], or answer a semantic question about the word strings [54]), there is no late positivity.
Multiple factors could be contributing to the variability in results observed across previous investigations of ERP responses to implicit metric violations. First, these studies have used different target words in the SW and WS conditions, meaning that the observed results may reflect differences beyond prosody, including phonetic, orthographic, or lexical differences between conditions. Second, these studies used single words or word lists to create metric expectations but, in these contexts, readers are not required to fully process the syntactic and semantic structure of the targets; this variability could lead to heterogenous depth of processing across conditions. Therefore, in the current study, we implemented metric expectations using metrically regular rhyming couplets, which encourage readers to make strong predictions about when strong and weak syllables will occur but also require deep linguistic processing. Moreover, our target words are stress-alternating noun–verb homographs, which can have SW or WS stress depending on the syntactic category. In this way, readers are exposed to the same visual, orthographic, and segmental input across all conditions.
If readers are generating implicit metric predictions during silent reading, we predict that targets which are inconsistent with the metric context will result in early differences in the ERP waveform compared to metrically consistent targets. However, based on prior work, we predict that this early effect may differ depending on the type of violation. Specifically, we predict SW violations will elicit an early negativity relative to consistent SW words. Conversely, WS violations may result in either a reduced negativity, or a positivity, relative to WS consistent targets. Moreover, we predict the absence of a late positivity in response to metric violations, as participants are not making explicit judgments about the metric structure.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Eighteen participants from Mount Holyoke College with an average age of 20 years (SD = 1.57 years) contributed data to the analyses. Seventeen participants identified as female and one identified as nonbinary/genderqueer. All participants were right-handed native speakers of American English, meaning they had been speaking English in the US since at least the age of three. One participant was born outside the US to English-speaking parents and moved to the US at age three. Five participants identified as bilingual as they had acquired high proficiency in another language starting before the age of three. All participants reported having normal or corrected-to-normal vision and had not taken psychoactive medications in the 24 h prior to the experiment. For the two-hour experiment, participants received compensation in the form of Psychology course research credit or $20. Data were collected but discarded from an additional four female participants due to voluntary withdrawal from the experiment (n = 1), recording equipment malfunction (n = 1), or excessive noise in the EEG (exclusion of more than 50% of trials from one or more conditions due to artifact; n = 2).

2.2. Materials

Experimental materials consisted of 160 limerick couplets (i.e., the first two lines of the limerick) adapted from the stimuli in [13], in which the final word of the second line was one of 40 stress-alternating noun–verb homograph targets (see Table 1). The stress pattern of these targets varies depending on the syntactic category: the noun form has a strong–weak pattern (e.g., PERmit), the verb (or adjective) form has a weak–strong pattern (e.g., perMIT). The homographs were selected from [29] and the Kucera–Francis corpus [56]. The frequency of occurrence of each target as a noun or verb/adjective in the Kucera–Francis corpus did not differ, as measured by a paired t-test, t(39) = 0.28, p = 0.78.
For each target homograph, four couplets were constructed, crossing the factors stress pattern (SW vs. WS) and metric consistency (consistent vs. inconsistent). All experimental couplets can be found in the Appendix A. The first line of each couplet established the metric and rhyming context for the target word. The stress pattern manipulation was implemented such that for half of the couplets, the target (e.g., permit in Table 1) was the noun form with a SW pattern (Table 1A,B). For the other half, the target was the verb/adjective form (Table 1C,D). The metric consistency manipulation meant that for half of the couplets, the stress pattern of the target homograph was consistent with the stress pattern predicted by the couplet (Table 1A,C). For the other half, the stress pattern of the target was inconsistent with the established pattern (Table 1B,D). The occurrence of an inconsistent SW word when a WS word is predicted is a strong–weak (SW) violation (Table 1B), and the occurrence of an inconsistent WS word when a SW word is predicted is a weak–strong (WS) violation (Table 1D).
In addition to the 160 experimental couplets, participants read 160 filler couplets which were always metrically consistent but varied in the stress pattern of the target regions (see Table S1 and examples (1), (2)). In this way, participants read a total of 80 rhythmically inconsistent items in a pool of 320 (25% of the total).
Examples:
1. There once was a man from Peru, who dreamt about eating his shoe
2. There once was a young man named Randy, who loved to eat all kinds of candy

2.3. Procedure

After providing informed consent, participants were seated comfortably in a sound-isolated room where they viewed the couplets on a computer screen located approximately 90 cm away. The 320 experimental and filler couplets were presented in a different randomized order for each participant. Each trial began with the presentation of the word “Ready?” which stayed on the screen until the participant responded with a keypress. The word was then replaced by a fixation cross, which remained on the screen for 1000 ms (Figure 1). Following the fixation cross, couplets were presented in six one-to-four-word (one-to-five-syllable) segments in the center of the screen (see Table S1). The 1st, 2nd, 4th and 5th segments were presented for 1000 ms each; the 3rd and 6th segments, corresponding to the end of the first and second lines, respectively, were presented for 2000 ms each. In the experimental couplets, the 3rd and 6th segments always contained two-to-three syllables, one of which was strong. The 1st, 2nd, 4th and 5th segments were more variable, but constrained so that each contained one strong syllable, and one-to-four weak syllables. The number of words and syllables varied across these segments because the couplets varied widely in terms of the number of words and stress patterns of the words that made them up. However, segments were consistently defined based on the first author’s intuition of natural syntactic and prosodic breaks in limerick structure.
To ensure that participants were reading for meaning, 25% of the filler trials (12.5% of all trials) were followed by a yes/no comprehension question about the semantic content. Participants held a response box in their lap for the duration of the experiment which they used to answer comprehension questions, and to advance the presentation of trials. Participants were given breaks between trials to allow time for blinking, as well as a longer break after every 40 trials; the length of these breaks were determined by the participant. The entire experimental session lasted approximately 2 h. All experimental procedures were approved in advance by the Institutional Review Board of Mount Holyoke College.
Reference-free electroencephalogram (EEG) data were collected using 64 active Ag/AgCl electrodes placed in an elastic cap and connected to a BioSemi Active-Two system, which digitized the EEG at a sampling rate of 2048 Hz and employed a hardware lowpass filter reaching −3 dB at 409.6 Hz. Reference-free EEG was also collected from two active electrodes attached bilaterally to the participant’s mastoids, and from four active electrodes placed above and below the left eye and bilaterally outside the outer canthi. All electrode offsets were brought below 20 mV at the start of the recording and kept below 50 mV throughout the recording. Continuous EEG data were referenced offline to the averaged mastoid recording, downsampled to 512 Hz, and filtered at 60 Hz using a Parks–McClellan notch filter. Bipolar vertical and horizontal electrooculogram (VEOG, HEOG) signals were derived by subtracting the above eye signal from the below eye signal, and the left from the right eye signal, respectively. Continuous EEG was segmented into epochs from 100 ms prior to target word onset to 800 ms following target word onset, and baseline-corrected to the 100 ms prestimulus period. Electrodes Oz and Iz were each identified as unusable for at least one participant and were excluded from further processing and analysis. Epochs containing eyeblinks or eye movements were identified algorithmically using moving window peak-to-peak voltage deflection detection on the VEOG channel (threshold = 150 µV, window size = 200 ms, window step = 25 ms) and step-like artifact detection on the HEOG channel (threshold = 100 µV, window size = 400 ms, window step = 25 ms), respectively. Additionally, epochs exceeding ±170 µV in any EEG channel were marked as artifact. The results of automatic artifact detection were then manually inspected and if needed, adjusted, and trials found to contain artifacts were excised. Artifact-free trials were then averaged by participant and condition; participants included in the analysis contributed data from at least 20 out of 40 trials (M = 31; SD = 6) in every condition. EEG data processing was performed in MATLAB using the EEGLAB [57] and ERPLAB [58] analysis packages.

2.4. Analysis

Previous ERP investigations of implicit linguistic metric processing have revealed effects across multiple time windows, with inconsistent time windows observed across studies [36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49]. We therefore opted to define our temporal regions of interest using a data-driven approach. To minimize implicit multiple comparisons when selecting time windows [59], we performed a series of cluster-based permutation tests over a moving 50 ms window (5 ms step) using the Mass Univariate ERP Toolbox [60]. These tests were performed separately for SW and WS violations. Within each moving window, electrodes at which the inconsistent vs. consistent t-test of mean window amplitude resulted in p ≤ 0.01 were identified, then clustered if they were within 5.44 cm of one another. Cluster magnitudes were then calculated as the sum of all t-scores for electrodes contained within a cluster. Lower-tailed t-tests were used for the SW comparisons based on prior findings that SW violations consistently elicit relative negativities [36,41,44,45,49,53,55], whereas two-tailed t-tests were used for the WS comparisons based on prior findings that WS violations elicit both relative negativities and positivities [40,41,42,54]. This process was replicated over 5000 shuffled iterations, and a cluster magnitude threshold was defined as the magnitude that clusters met or exceeded on only 5% of the shuffled (i.e., chance) iterations. Moving windows within which any clusters identified in the experimental data met or exceeded the cluster magnitude threshold were defined as temporal regions of interest (see Figure S1). This approach revealed three regions of interest, which were further investigated using conventional, ANOVA-based ERP analyses: 80–155 ms (SW), 325–375 ms (SW), and 365–435 ms (WS).
We selected 49 electrodes for conventional, ANOVA-based ERP analysis (Figure 2). Scalp position was treated as two factors in the statistical model: electrode anteriority had seven levels ranging from most anterior to most posterior electrodes, and electrode laterality had seven levels, ranging from left to right. Based on our cluster-based permutation tests, we assessed SW ERP amplitudes in two time windows (80–155 ms and 325–375 ms), and WS ERP amplitudes in one time window (365–435 ms). Mean amplitudes from each participant in each time window were entered into a 2 (metric consistency) × 7 (anteriority) × 7 (laterality) repeated-measures ANOVA. Significant and marginal interactions of metric consistency with electrode position in the absence of a main effect of metric consistency were further investigated with follow-up ANOVAs over constrained scalp regions. Only main effects and interactions which involve metric consistency will be discussed. Whenever Mauchly’s Test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated for comparisons with more than two levels, Huynh–Feldt-corrected degrees of freedom were used to compute statistical significance. All statistical analyses were implemented in the R statistical framework [61] with the ez package [62].

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral

Participants answered comprehension questions with an average accuracy rate of 96.25% (SD = 4.2%), demonstrating that they were attending to the couplets and engaged with the task.

3.2. Event-Related Potentials

3.2.1. SW Violations

SW violations elicited a negativity from 80–155 ms relative to predicted SW targets over left and medial scalp positions (Figure 2). An overall 2 × 7 × 7 ANOVA looking only at SW targets revealed a marginal interaction of metric consistency and electrode laterality, F(2.74,46.55) = 2.38, p = 0.087, η2 = 0.007. A follow-up 2 × 7 ANOVA looking only at SW targets over left and medial scalp positions revealed a negativity elicited by SW violations relative to predicted SW targets, F(1,17) = 4.75, p = 0.044, η2 = 0.14.
SW violations also elicited a negativity from 325–375 ms relative to predicted SW targets over the entire scalp, that was largest over left and medial scalp positions (Figure 2). An overall 2 × 7 × 7 ANOVA looking only at SW targets revealed a main effect of metric consistency, F(1,17) = 5.32, p = 0.034, η2 = 0.09, and a marginal interaction of metric consistency and electrode laterality indicated that this effect was largest over left and medial scalp positions, F(3.38,57.45) = 2.51, p = 0.06, η2 = 0.004.

3.2.2. WS violations

WS violations elicited a positivity from 365–435 ms relative to predicted WS targets over the entire scalp, that was largest over central and posterior scalp positions (Figure 2). An overall 2 × 7 × 7 ANOVA looking only at WS targets revealed a main effect of metric consistency, F(1,17) = 4.99, p = 0.039, η2 = 0.06, and a marginal interaction of metric consistency and electrode anteriority indicated that this effect was largest over central and posterior scalp positions, F(2.68,45.54) = 2.78, p = 0.06, η2 = 0.01.

4. Discussion

The goal of the current study was to investigate the realization of metric representations during silent reading using ERPs. Participants silently read metrically regular rhyming couplets in which the final target word had a strong–weak (SW) or weak–strong (WS) lexical stress pattern that was either consistent or inconsistent with the metric stress pattern predicted by the couplet. The results demonstrated that SW targets which were inconsistent with the stress pattern of the couplet (i.e., SW violations) elicited two separate negativities (80–155 ms and 325–375 ms after word onset) relative to SW targets which were consistent with the predicted stress pattern. Conversely, WS targets inconsistent with the stress pattern of the couplet (i.e., WS violations) elicited an early positivity (365–435 ms after word onset) relative to WS targets which were consistent with the predicted stress pattern. Neither SW nor WS violations elicited a late positivity. Together with prior results, the current results support the Implicit Prosody Hypothesis, which maintains that readers are generating implicit versions of prosodic structure even when reading silently, and that these representations are similar to explicit ones.
The observation of a significant negative left-lateralized deflection from 80–155 ms in response to SW violations is an unexpected result based on prior work on explicit and implicit linguistic metric processing. Few studies of linguistic meter have reported consistent differences in components this early, though one study demonstrated a significant negativity between 100–320 ms in response to an inappropriate stressed syllable [46]. However, negativities in the 100–200 ms time window have been widely observed in response to metric violations in musical studies. This effect, termed the metric mismatch negativity (MMN), has been observed when a strong tone occurs at an unpredicted temporal location (i.e., when a weak tone is predicted) [63,64,65]. This situation is analogous to the circumstance under which we observed the early negativity in the current study, such that a strong beat at a predicted weak time elicits the early negativity (SW violation), whereas a weak beat at a predicted strong time does not (WS violation). Importantly, as this effect was detected based on a marginal interaction of metric consistency with electrode position and this is the first study we are aware of to report this early negativity in response to an implicit strong beat occurring at a predicted weak time, additional experiments will be required to determine the reliability and meaning of this component.
The negativity between 325–375 ms observed for SW violations is consistent with results from previous investigations of both explicit and implicit violations of metric structure. Specifically, previous studies have demonstrated that SW metric violations result in a negative deflection in the 250–500 ms range relative to metrically consistent targets [36,41,44,45,49]. Moreover, a similar effect has also been shown in a small set of studies investigating metric structure in silent reading of single words [53,55]. The current study extends this finding to silent reading of metric violations in sentence contexts using orthographically identical items across all conditions. The observation of a positivity for WS violations from 365–435 ms after word onset is also consistent with both prior listening and reading studies. Two prior studies of explicit metric violations [40,42] and one prior study of implicit metric processing [54] have observed positive deflections for consistent WS targets relative to inconsistent WS targets. Our results therefore suggest that prior findings of different responses to SW and WS violations are not simply due to idiosyncratic differences between the SW and WS target items chosen for these prior experiments, but do indeed reflect the activation of abstract metric representations during silent reading.
The different results observed across multiple studies for SW vs. WS violations may be due to differences in the underlying phonological structure of the target words. According to [17], the trochaic foot (SW) is the default phonological structure in Germanic languages, including English. This phonological constraint is realized in the lexical stress patterns of words, such that most two-syllable words begin with a stressed syllable (85–90% of the time in English [34]; 73% of the time in German [66]). This asymmetry means that accessing a SW (trochaic) representation of a target is globally easier than accessing a WS (iambic) representation, irrespective of the context in which the target occurs. Therefore, the lexical representation of a SW target is harder to access when its stress pattern conflicts with the local metric context, than when its stress pattern is consistent with the local context. Conversely, resolving WS violations is more challenging for readers, due to conflicting cues in both the local environment and the global environment.
Under this view of phonological asymmetry, the negativity observed between 325–375 ms for SW violations in the current study, and in a similar time window in other studies, could be related to the N400, which reflects the ease with which lexical access is achieved. The negativity for SW violations could reflect either additional lexical processing due to the added difficulty of accessing the appropriate lexical content in the presence of lexical stress mismatch, or lexical repair processes due to automatic activation of the metrically consistent, but semantically inconsistent, alternate form of the noun/verb homograph. However, it is important to note that this interpretation of the negativity as indexing lexical processing is challenged by previous work exploring simultaneous violations of metric and semantic structure, in which the latency and distribution of the negativity differs across violation types [39,40], as well as evidence that metrically inconsistent pseudowords also elicit such negativities, even though they lack semantic context [45]. Alternatively, it could be that the negativity we observed in the current experiment indicates the violation of a consistent, rule-based sequence, in this case realized as the metric structure [45].
In contrast, the positivity observed between 365–435 ms for WS violations in the current study, and in a similar time window in other studies, could be related to conflict processing. When a WS violation occurs, the reader must resolve the conflict between a metric context which leads them to predict a SW target and a semantic context which leads them to predict a WS target. In addition, there is the added conflict that WS two-syllable words are phonologically marked in the language. These factors together may lead to the observed positivity, which is signaling an error in processing that is harder for readers to recover from. This interpretation is consistent with previous ERP research of the metric structure in German, where metric violations in three-syllable words that did not violate metric foot structure led to an early negativity, whereas violations that also conflicted with foot structure resulted in an early positivity [36], similar to the results in the current study.
Consistent with other explicit and implicit metric processing studies that do not involve an explicit metric task, we did not observe evidence of a late positivity for metric violations relative to consistent metric conditions. Previous studies of both explicit and implicit metric processing demonstrate that late positivities in response to metric violation are most likely observed when the participant’s task is to assess the metric structure. Indeed, only one previous study of implicit metric processing observed a late positivity in response to metric violations [54] while two others did not [53,55], and none of these studies required an explicit metric judgment. This interpretation is in line with previous work showing a dissociation between early and late ERP effects of syntactic violations, where early negativities are thought to reflect automatic processing and late positivities are thought to reflect controlled processes of repair [67,68] and the difficulty of the required repair process [69]. The current results suggest that although both implicit and explicit metric violations are automatically detected, as evidenced by early (<500 ms) waveform differences, only violations that rise to the level of awareness give rise to a late positivity.
It is also possible that the lack of a late positivity in the current study reflects a lack of power; our choice to present the same orthographic information across conditions meant that the number of items in the experiment was limited by the number of two-syllable stress-alternating noun–verb homographs in English that were known to our participants and could be embedded in rhyming couplets. Moreover, compared to previous studies using word lists, the stress pattern of the target in the current study was locally ambiguous, and only disambiguated by the implicit metric structure provided by the context. Although this manipulation is a better test of the abstract metric structure compared to other studies that used different items across SW and WS conditions, it produces a less clearly defined metric violation than paradigms employing single target words with unambiguous stress patterns.
Although the current results are generally consistent with prior ERP work on explicit and implicit linguistic metric structures, they are inconsistent with results observed in a previous eye-tracking experiment using the same materials. Recall that Breen and Clifton observed inflated reading times only for WS violations, and not for SW violations [13]; moreover, these effects were observed only in relatively later reading time measures. Conversely, our results demonstrate significant early ERP differences for both SW and WS violations, though they differ in polarity, timing, and topography. These differential effects are likely due to differences in the temporal control of stimulus presentation between the studies. In Breen and Clifton’s experiment, participants read normally at their own pace, meaning they could take as much time as needed to process material in advance of the critical word, and could look back to prior sentence material to resolve difficulty generated at the target word. In contrast, materials in the current study were presented in a region-by-region segmented fashion, giving participants less time to generate predictions about upcoming material, and disallowing regressions. Moreover, the fact that the current materials were presented in a time-controlled manner means that the metric structure of the sentence materials was more obvious for readers, making the metric inconsistency more explicit, resulting in significant ERP effects of both types of metric violations.
Future work could directly investigate the role of temporal stimulus control on implicit metric violation processing by replicating the current paradigm using simultaneous collection of eye-tracking data and ERPs, a method which has already been used to successfully adjudicate debates about linguistic processing in eye movements [70,71]. In this way, the role of metric inconsistency in silent reading could be assessed without explicitly controlling the timing of materials. Additionally, while current results demonstrate that readers engage in implicit prosody during silent reading of poetry, it is an open question to what extent these findings generalize to normal reading. The couplets used in the current study were designed to have strict metric and rhyming structure, which is rare in non-poetic language. However, our study does provide an insight to the role of meter in implicit prosody. To determine whether our result can be replicated in non-poetic contexts which do not have concomitantly high metrical expectancies, future work will explore differences in brain activity in response to metric violations in silently-read prose sentences.

5. Conclusions

The current results provide further evidence of an intimate link between metric processing during listening and metric processing during silent reading, which may help inform our understanding of previously described relationships between children’s sensitivity to an auditory metric structure and silent reading comprehension. For example, the ability of older children to track a perceived metric structure predicts phonological awareness and reading outcomes [72,73], and children’s ability to detect a mis-stressed word predicts phonological awareness and word knowledge [74]. It may be the case that these reading abilities are facilitated by implicit metric structure representations. This claim is further bolstered by a relationship between prosodic fluency and reading comprehension in high school students—those who demonstrate higher prosodic fluency also showed an increased comprehension ability [75,76]. Research about implicit prosody and the underlying neurocognitive processes occurring during silent reading may, therefore, inform future work designing prosodic interventions to improve children’s reading comprehension abilities.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/2076-3425/9/8/192/s1, Table S1: Distribution of stress patterns across segments for experimental and filler couplets, Figure S1: Moving window cluster-based permutation test results.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.B.; methodology, M.B., A.B.F.; software, M.B., A.B.F.; validation, M.B., A.B.F.; formal analysis, M.B., A.B.F., M.O.A.; investigation, M.O.A.; resources, M.B.; data curation, M.B., A.B.F., M.O.A.; writing—original draft preparation, M.B.; writing—review and editing, A.B.F., M.O.A.; visualization, M.B., A.B.F., M.O.A.; supervision, M.B.; project administration, M.B., A.B.F.; funding acquisition, M.B., M.O.A.

Funding

This research was funded in part by a James S. McDonnell Foundation Scholar Award in Understanding Human Cognition to author M.B. and a Harap Scholarship Fund Award to author M.O.A.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the assistance of Charles Clifton with materials development, Elizabeth Brija with experimental coding, and Xuefei Chen, Hannah Galloway, Margaret Golder, Johanna Kneifel, Priscilla Lopez, and Corrin Moss with data collection for the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

1a. SW/consistent
  • You must hear/my story,/your highness.
  • I have the/young princess’s/address
1b. SW/inconsistent
  • My workspace/is such a/big mess.
  • I lost an/important/address
1c. WS/consistent
  • My workspace/is such a/big mess.
  • My clutter/I have to/address
1d. WS/inconsistent
  • You must hear/my story,/your highness.
  • Your habits I/find I must/address
2a. SW/consistent
  • The guy who/got lost on/a flyby
  • Dropped all of/his bombs on an/ally
2b. SW/inconsistent
  • I know a/young woman/from Rye,
  • Who’d make such/a lovely/ally
2c. WS/consistent
  • I know a/young woman/from Rye,
  • With whom I/would like to/ally
2d. WS/inconsistent
  • The guy who/got lost on/a flyby
  • Killed folks with whom/we want to/ally
3a. SW/consistent
  • I just saw/a dog and/a tomcat,
  • Engaged in/some furious/combat
3b. SW/inconsistent
  • I witnessed/a dog and/a cat,
  • Engaged in/some angry/combat
3c. WS/consistent
  • I witnessed/a dog and/a cat,
  • Who seemingly/tried to/combat
3d. WS/inconsistent
  • I just saw/a dog and/a tomcat,
  • That we must/be ready to/combat
4a. SW/consistent
  • I heard someone/say through/the grapevine:
  • The farmer is/driving his/combine
4b. SW/inconsistent
  • The farmer/got caught/drinking wine,
  • Then harvesting/in his/combine
4c. WS/consistent
  • The farmer/got caught/drinking wine,
  • And shotguns/and booze don’t/combine
4d. WS/inconsistent
  • I heard someone/say through/the grapevine:
  • That farmer is/hoping to/combine
5a. SW/consistent
  • I processed/some prints in/the darkroom
  • Of people I’d/met on a/commune
5b. SW/inconsistent
  • I know some/who worship/the moon,
  • And live/in a hippie/commune
5c. WS/consistent
  • I know some/who worship/the moon,
  • With nature/they like to/commune
5d. WS/inconsistent
  • I processed/some prints in/the darkroom
  • Of folks who/just wanted to/commune
6a. SW/consistent
  • If out in/the mountains/you backpack,
  • Your team must/agree to this/compact
6b. SW/inconsistent
  • Before you/head out with/that pack,
  • Your team has/to sign this/compact
6c. WS/consistent
  • Before you/head out with/that pack,
  • Be sure that/your gear is/compact
6d. WS/inconsistent
  • If out in/the mountains/you backpack,
  • Your gear must/be basic and/compact
7a. SW/consistent
  • The crew worked/so hard for/their paychecks
  • They thought they’d/develop a/complex
7b. SW/inconsistent
  • There once was/a young man/named Rex
  • Who owned/an apartment/complex
7c. WS/consistent
  • There once was/a young man/named Rex
  • Whose theories/were big and/complex
7d. WS/inconsistent
  • The crew worked/so hard for/their paychecks
  • Their work was/so terribly/complex
8a. SW/consistent
  • We stayed in/the woods at/a campground,
  • Which wasn’t too/far from a/compound
8b. SW/inconsistent
  • We got that/old dog at/the pound
  • He came from/a private/compound
8c. WS/consistent
  • We stayed in/the woods at/a campground,
  • Our pleasure in/nature to/compound
8d. WS/inconsistent
  • We got that/old dog at/the pound
  • Our sadness/will surely/compound
9a. SW/consistent
  • There was a/young heroin/addict,
  • Who ended up/causing a/conflict
9b. SW/inconsistent
  • My parents/are being/quite strict.
  • Our views are/in open/conflict
9c. WS/consistent
  • My parents/are being/quite strict.
  • Their wishes/and mine do/conflict
9d. WS/inconsistent
  • There was a/young heroin/addict,
  • Whose habits and/others did/conflict
10a. SW/consistent
  • The athlete/who just failed/a drugtest,
  • Will soon face/a challenging/contest
10b. SW/inconsistent
  • The athlete/who thinks he’s/the best
  • Just lost an/important/contest
10c. WS/consistent
  • The athlete/who thinks he’s/the best
  • Holds titles/that others/contest
10d. WS/inconsistent
  • The athlete/who just failed/a drugtest,
  • Is planning the/charges to/contest
11a. SW/consistent
  • Although that/young man is/an addict,
  • He really should/not be a/convict
11b. SW/inconsistent
  • I think that/the judge was/too strict
  • In sentencing/that young/convict
11c. WS/consistent
  • I think that/the judge was/too strict,
  • The jury too/quick to/convict
11d. WS/inconsistent
  • Although that/young man is/an addict,
  • I think that the/judge shouldn’t/convict
12a. SW/consistent
  • That man applies/way too much/hair grease.
  • A friend should/suggest a big/decrease
12b. SW/inconsistent
  • Forgive me/for stating/my peace,
  • But you must/commence a/decrease
12c. WS/consistent
  • Forgive me/for stating/my peace,
  • Your appetite/you must/decrease
12d. WS/inconsistent
  • That man applies/way too much/hair grease.
  • I think the/amount he should/decrease
13a. SW/consistent
  • The Soviet/spy is a/suspect.
  • The case has/but one major/defect
13b. SW/inconsistent
  • The Soviet/spy they/suspect,
  • Has plans with/a major/defect
13c. WS/consistent
  • The Soviet/spy they/suspect,
  • Is planning/quite soon to/defect
13d. WS/inconsistent
  • The Soviet/spy is/a suspect.
  • I heard that/he’s planning to/defect
14a. SW/consistent
  • In nothing/but jeans and/a t-shirt,
  • That man took/a trip ‘cross the/desert
14b. SW/inconsistent
  • The fighting/he tried/to avert,
  • By running off/through the/desert
14c. WS/consistent
  • The fighting/he tried to/avert,
  • By choosing/his squad to/desert
14d. WS/inconsistent
  • In nothing but/jeans and a/t-shirt,
  • A soldier his/squad chose to/desert
15a. SW/consistent
  • I know of/an elegant/female
  • Her outfits lack/no fashion/detail
15b. SW/inconsistent
  • There once was/a woman/named Gail
  • Whose fashion/had every/detail
15c. WS/consistent
  • I know of/an elegant/female
  • Who wanted/her auto to/detail
15d. WS/inconsistent
  • There once was/a woman/named Gail
  • Who wanted/her car to/detail
16a. SW/consistent
  • We once had/a tiresome/house guest,
  • Who loved to/read Birdwatcher’s/Digest
16b. SW/inconsistent
  • We once had/a friend as/a guest,
  • Who loved to/skim Reader’s/Digest
16c. WS/consistent
  • We once had/a friend as/a guest,
  • Whose cooking/we could not/digest
16d. WS/inconsistent
  • We once had/a tiresome/house guest,
  • Whose humor/was painful to/digest
17a. SW/consistent
  • The gymnast/requested/a recount
  • Her score, she/thought, rated no/discount
17b. SW/inconsistent
  • He could not/afford the/amount,
  • And asked for/a modest/discount
17c. WS/consistent
  • He could not/afford the/amount.
  • The invoice they/would not/discount
17d. WS/inconsistent
  • The gymnast/requested/a recount
  • She thought it/was wrongful to/discount
18a. SW/consistent
  • In order to/prove your/attendance
  • You’ll have to/check in at the/entrance
18b. SW/inconsistent
  • This gorgeous/young woman/from France
  • Made everyone/jam the/entrance
18c. WS/consistent
  • This gorgeous/young woman/from France
  • Would often/the young men/entrance
18d. WS/inconsistent
  • There was a/young woman/whose nude dance
  • Would always/the gentlemen/entrance
19a. SW/consistent
  • He tried not/to get badly/sidetracked.
  • He needed/some raspberry/extract
19b. SW/inconsistent
  • The recipe/seemed quite/exact.
  • It called for/some almond/extract
19c. WS/consistent
  • The recipe/seemed quite/exact.
  • Some essence/you had to/extract
19d. WS/inconsistent
  • He tried not/to get badly/sidetracked
  • Some essence/he wanted to/extract
20a. SW/consistent
  • The city/must safeguard/the seaports,
  • To save us/from dangerous/imports
20b. SW/inconsistent
  • The panel/is set to/report
  • On how much/we pay for/imports
20c. WS/consistent
  • The panel/is set to/report
  • On how much/the city/imports
20d. WS/inconsistent
  • The city/must safeguard/the seaports,
  • Because of how/much it now/imports
21a. SW/consistent
  • The man who/asked you for/a consult
  • Was given/a horrible/insult
21b. WS/consistent
  • That woman/who likes the/occult,
  • Is very/unsafe to/insult
21c. WS/inconsistent
  • The man who/asked you for/a consult
  • Is no-one/you wanted to/insult
21d. SW/inconsistent
  • That woman/who likes/the occult,
  • Will tolerate/no more/insults
22a. SW/consistent
  • The teacher/assigned them/a project
  • To find an/unusual/object
22b. SW/inconsistent
  • The winners/will get to/select
  • A shiny/expensive/object
22c. WS/consistent
  • The mayor/that we might/elect
  • Has views to/which others/object
22d. WS/inconsistent
  • The teacher/assigned them/a project
  • That forced many/parents to/object
23a. SW/consistent
  • There once was/an old man/named Kermit,
  • Who hunted/without any/permit
23b. SW/inconsistent
  • There once was/an old man/named Britt
  • Who hunted/without a/permit
23c. WS/consistent
  • There once was/an old man/named Britt
  • Whose vices/no wife could/permit
23d. WS/inconsistent
  • There once was/an old man/named Kermit
  • Whose gambling his/wife would not/permit
24a. SW/consistent
  • I know of/an old man/named Herbert,
  • Who’s known around/town as a/pervert
24b. SW/inconsistent
  • The nun/did her best/to convert
  • A man whom/they call a/pervert
24c. WS/consistent
  • That nun/did her best/to convert
  • Young kids who/the truth do/pervert
24d. WS/inconsistent
  • I know of/an old man/named Herbert
  • Who always the/truth tries to/pervert
25a. SW/consistent
  • There once was/a penniless/peasant,
  • Who couldn’t/afford a nice/present
25b. SW/inconsistent
  • There once was/a clever/young gent,
  • Who bought for/his girl a/present
25c. WS/consistent
  • There once was/a clever/young gent,
  • Who had a/nice talk to/present
25d. WS/inconsistent
  • There once was/a penniless/peasant,
  • Who went to/his master to/present
26a. SW/consistent
  • He couldn’t/hide all of/his misdeeds,
  • But made off/with all of the/proceeds
26b. SW/inconsistent
  • In light/of the man’s/dirty deeds,
  • He won’t/receive any/proceeds
26c. WS/consistent
  • In light/of the man’s/dirty deeds,
  • On Monday/his trial/proceeds
26d. WS/inconsistent
  • He couldn’t/hide all of/his misdeeds
  • On Monday/his retrial/proceeds
27a. SW/consistent
  • There once was/a crusty old/recluse,
  • Who grew the/most wonderful/produce
27b. SW/inconsistent
  • There simply/is no good/excuse
  • For failing to/eat your/produce
27c. WS/consistent
  • There simply/is no good/excuse
  • For failing/to work and/produce
27d. WS/inconsistent
  • There once was/a crusty/old recluse,
  • Whose garden great/harvests would/produce
28a. SW/consistent
  • With all of/their time spent/at recess,
  • The children/make no forward/progress
28b. SW/inconsistent
  • The efforts/at peace,/I confess,
  • Are making/no forward/progress
28c. WS/consistent
  • The efforts/at peace,/I confess,
  • Will simply/no longer/progress
28d. WS/inconsistent
  • With all of/their time spent/at recess,
  • The children will/soon fail to/progress
29a. SW/consistent
  • I noticed/a ruinous/defect
  • In part of/the candidate’s/project
29b. SW/inconsistent
  • The man we/will likely/elect
  • Endorses/this wacky/project
29c. WS/consistent
  • The mayor that/folks will/elect
  • According/to what polls/project
29d. WS/inconsistent
  • I noticed/a ruinous/defect
  • In what that/new candidate/projects
30a. SW/consistent
  • There once was/a young man/named Ernest,
  • Who sponsored/a violent/protest
30b. SW/inconsistent
  • They put the/man under/arrest
  • For leading/an angry/protest
30c. WS/consistent
  • They put the/man under/arrest,
  • And gave him/no time to/protest
30d. WS/inconsistent
  • There once was/a young man/named Ernest,
  • Who rounded up/people to/protest
31a. SW/consistent
  • In a voice/that was piercing/and treble,
  • The serfs were/inspired by a/rebel
31b. SW/inconsistent
  • The infantry/failed to/repel
  • The followers/of the/rebel
31c. WS/consistent
  • The infantry/failed to/repel
  • The fighters/who want to/rebel
31d. WS/inconsistent
  • In a voice/that was piercing/and treble,
  • The leader urged/peasants to/rebel
32a. SW/consistent
  • That basketball/star’s like a/bloodhound.
  • He seeks out/and catches each/rebound
32b. SW/inconsistent
  • The basketball/star turned/around,
  • and caught an/amazing/rebound
32c. WS/consistent
  • The basketball/star turned/around,
  • And watched for/the shot to/rebound
32d. WS/inconsistent
  • That basketball/star’s like a/bloodhound.
  • He waits for/each jumpshot to/rebound
33a. SW/consistent
  • I met an/old friend who/played baseball,
  • Who warned of/a new safety/recall
33b. SW/inconsistent
  • I met an/old friend at/the mall,
  • Who warned of/a safety/recall
33c. WS/consistent
  • I met an/old friend at/the mall,
  • Whose name I/just could not/recall
33d. WS/inconsistent
  • I met an/old friend who/played baseball,
  • But what his/name was I can’t/recall
34a. SW/consistent
  • There once was/a young man/named Eckerd
  • Who broke an old/pole-vaulting/record
34b. SW/inconsistent
  • The athlete/won quite an/award
  • For breaking/the scoring/record
34c. WS/consistent
  • The athlete/won quite an/award
  • The cameras/were there to/record
34d. WS/inconsistent
  • There once was/a young man/named Eckerd
  • Whose pole-vaulting/feats they did/record
35a. SW/consistent
  • Last year I/created a/stock fund.
  • And managed to/get a big/refund
35b. SW/inconsistent
  • I have to/admit I/am stunned,
  • You didn’t/give me my/refund
35c. WS/consistent
  • I have to/admit I/am stunned,
  • My payments/you will not/refund
35d. WS/inconsistent
  • Last year I/created a/stock fund.
  • The fees they/would happily/refund
36a. SW/consistent
  • The judges must/all watch/the replay
  • To find out which/team won the/relay
36b. SW/inconsistent
  • A messenger/came by/today
  • To find out/who won the/relay
36c. WS/consistent
  • A messenger/came by/today;
  • A message/he had to/relay
36d. WS/inconsistent
  • The judges must/all watch/the replay.
  • Results to the/coach they will/relay
37a. SW/consistent
  • I read an/unusual/essay
  • ‘Bout how they/conducted a/survey
37b. SW/inconsistent
  • A lovely/young woman/named Fay
  • Was asked to/complete a/survey
37c. WS/consistent
  • A lovely/young woman/named Fay
  • The future/she liked to/survey
37d. WS/inconsistent
  • I read an/unusual/essay
  • Describing how/folks tried to/survey
38a. SW/consistent
  • The cops are/an interesting/subject
  • They bullied their/most recent/suspect
38b. SW/inconsistent
  • The cops/didn’t try/to protect
  • A recently/collared/suspect
38c. WS/consistent
  • The cops/didn’t try/to protect
  • The people/they chose to/suspect
38d. WS/inconsistent
  • The cops are/an interesting/subject
  • They bully/the people they/suspect
39a. SW/consistent
  • A striking young/woman named/Rembrandt,
  • From Portugal,/she was a/transplant
39b. SW/inconsistent
  • A striking young/dame named/van Zandt,
  • From Spain was/a recent/transplant
39c. WS/consistent
  • A striking young/dame named/van Zandt
  • Had roses/she hoped to/transplant
39d. WS/inconsistent
  • A striking young/woman named/Rembrandt,
  • Had roses she/wanted to/transplant
40a. SW/consistent
  • To get to/the local gym’s/squash court,
  • You must take/municipal/transport
40b. SW/inconsistent
  • The mafia/tried to/extort
  • The captain/of public/transport
40c. WS/consistent
  • The mafia/tried to/extort
  • A man who/had tried to/transport
40d. WS/inconsistent
  • To get to/the local/gym’s squash court,
  • Your gear should/be ready to/transport

References

  1. Fodor, J.D. Learning To Parse? J. Psycholinguist. Res. 1998, 27, 285–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Fodor, J.D. Psycholinguistics cannot escape prosody. In Proceedings of the Speech Prosody 2002, International Conference, Aix-en-Provence, France, 11–13 April 2002. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bader, M. Prosodic Influences on Reading Syntactically Ambiguous Sentences. In Reanalysis in Sentence Processing; Fodor, J.D., Ferreira, F., Eds.; Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1998; pp. 1–46. ISBN 978-90-481-5037-3. [Google Scholar]
  4. Breen, M. Empirical Investigations of the Role of Implicit Prosody in Sentence Processing. Lang. Linguist. Compass 2014, 8, 37–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Breen, M. Empirical Investigations of Implicit Prosody. In Explicit and Implicit Prosody in Sentence Processing: Studies in Honor of Janet Dean Fodor; Frazier, L., Gibson, E., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2015; pp. 177–192. ISBN 978-3-319-12960-0. [Google Scholar]
  6. Abramson, M. The Written Voice: Implicit Memory Effects of Voice Characteristics following Silent Reading and Auditory Presentation. Percept. Mot. Skills 2007, 105, 1171–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Speer, S.R.; Foltz, A. The implicit prosody of corrective contrast primes appropriately intonated probes (for some readers). In Explicit and Implicit Prosody in Sentence Processing: Studies in Honor of Janet Dean Fodor; Frazier, L., Gibson, E., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2015; pp. 263–285. ISBN 978-3-319-12960-0. [Google Scholar]
  8. Hwang, H.; Schafer, A.J. Constituent Length Affects Prosody and Processing for a Dative NP Ambiguity in Korean. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2009, 38, 151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Augurzky, P. Attaching Relative Clauses in German: The Role of Implicit and Explicit Prosody in Sentence Processing; MPI Series in Human Cognitive and Brain Science: Leipzig, Germany, 2006; Volume 77. [Google Scholar]
  10. Hirose, Y. Recycling Prosodic Boundaries. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2003, 32, 167–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Ashby, J.; Clifton, C., Jr. The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading. Cognition 2005, 96, B89–B100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Breen, M.; Clifton, C. Stress matters revisited: A boundary change experiment. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2013, 66, 1896–1909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Breen, M.; Clifton, C., Jr. Stress matters: Effects of anticipated lexical stress on silent reading. J. Mem. Lang. 2011, 64, 153–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Kentner, G. Linguistic rhythm guides parsing decisions in written sentence comprehension. Cognition 2012, 123, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kentner, G.; Vasishth, S. Prosodic Focus Marking in Silent Reading: Effects of Discourse Context and Rhythm. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. McCurdy, K.; Kentner, G.; Vasishth, S. Implicit prosody and contextual bias in silent reading. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2013, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hayes, B. Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1995; ISBN 978-0-226-32103-5. [Google Scholar]
  18. Liberman, M.; Prince, A. On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguist. Inq. 1977, 8, 249–336. [Google Scholar]
  19. Selkirk, E. References—Scientific Research Publish. In Phonology and Syntax: The Relation between Sound and Structure; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984; Available online: http://www.scirp.org/(S(czeh2tfqyw2orz553k1w0r45))/reference/ReferencesPapers.aspx?ReferenceID=918312 (accessed on 30 June 2017).
  20. Nespor, M.; Vogel, I. Prosodic Phonology: With a New Foreword; Walter de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2007; ISBN 978-3-11-019790-7. [Google Scholar]
  21. Beckman Mary, E. Stress and Non-Stress Accent; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany; Boston, MA, USA, 2012; ISBN 978-3-11-013729-3. [Google Scholar]
  22. Fry, D.B. Duration and Intensity as Physical Correlates of Linguistic Stress. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1955, 27, 765–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Breen, M. Effects of metric hierarchy and rhyme predictability on word duration in The Cat in the Hat. Cognition 2018, 174, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Fitzroy, A.B.; Breen, M. Metric Structure and Rhyme Predictability Modulate Speech Intensity During Child-Directed and Read-Alone Productions of Children’s Literature. Lang. Speech 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Shields, J.L.; McHugh, A.; Martin, J.G. Reaction time to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continuous speech. J. Exp. Psychol. 1974, 102, 250–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Mattys, S.L.; Samuel, A.G. How lexical stress affects speech segmentation and interactivity: Evidence from the migration paradigm. J. Mem. Lang. 1997, 36, 87–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Cutler, A.; Norris, D. The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1988, 14, 113–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cutler, A.; Dahan, D.; van Donselaar, W. Prosody in the Comprehension of Spoken Language: A Literature Review. Lang. Speech 1997, 40, 141–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Pitt, M.A.; Samuel, A.G. The use of rhythm in attending to speech. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1990, 16, 564–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Breen, M.; Dilley, L.C.; McAuley, J.D.; Sanders, L.D. Auditory evoked potentials reveal early perceptual effects of distal prosody on speech segmentation. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 29, 1132–1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Dilley, L.C.; Mattys, S.L.; Vinke, L. Potent prosody: Comparing the effects of distal prosody, proximal prosody, and semantic context on word segmentation. J. Mem. Lang. 2010, 63, 274–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Brown, M.; Salverda, A.P.; Dilley, L.C.; Tanenhaus, M.K. Expectations from preceding prosody influence segmentation in online sentence processing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2011, 18, 1189–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Dilley, L.C.; McAuley, J.D. Distal prosodic context affects word segmentation and lexical processing. J. Mem. Lang. 2008, 59, 294–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Cutler, A.; Carter, D.M. The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Comput. Speech Lang. 1987, 2, 133–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Cutler, A.; Clifton, C., Jr. The use of prosodic information in word recognition. In Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1984; pp. 183–196. [Google Scholar]
  36. Domahs, U.; Wiese, R.; Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I.; Schlesewsky, M. The Processing of German Word Stress: Evidence for the Prosodic Hierarchy. Phonology 2008, 25, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Domahs, U.; Genc, S.; Knaus, J.; Wiese, R.; Kabak, B. Processing (un-)predictable word stress: ERP evidence from Turkish. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2013, 28, 335–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Molczanow, J.; Domahs, U.; Knaus, J.; Wiese, R. The lexical representation of word stress in Russian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Ment. Lex. 2013, 8, 164–194. [Google Scholar]
  39. Magne, C.; Astésano, C.; Aramaki, M.; Ystad, S.; Kronland-Martinet, R.; Besson, M. Influence of syllabic lengthening on semantic processing in spoken French: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 2659–2668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Marie, C.; Magne, C.; Besson, M. Musicians and the metric structure of words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 294–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Magne, C.; Jordan, D.K.; Gordon, R.L. Speech rhythm sensitivity and musical aptitude: ERPs and individual differences. Brain Lang. 2016, 153–154, 13–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Böcker, K.B.E.; Bastiaansen, M.C.M.; Vroomen, J.; Brunia, C.H.M.; Gelder, B.D. An ERP correlate of metrical stress in spoken word recognition. Psychophysiology 1999, 36, 706–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Schmidt-Kassow, M.; Kotz, S.A. Event-related brain potentials suggest a late interaction of meter and syntax in the P600. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009, 21, 1693–1708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Rothermich, K.; Schmidt-Kassow, M.; Kotz, S.A. Rhythm’s gonna get you: Regular meter facilitates semantic sentence processing. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 232–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Rothermich, K.; Schmidt-Kassow, M.; Schwartze, M.; Kotz, S.A. Event-related potential responses to metric violations: Rules versus meaning. NeuroReport 2010, 21, 580–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Bohn, K.; Knaus, J.; Wiese, R.; Domahs, U. The influence of rhythmic (ir)regularities on speech processing: Evidence from an ERP study on German phrases. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 760–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Henrich, K.; Wiese, R.; Domahs, U. How information structure influences the processing of rhythmic irregularities: ERP evidence from German phrases. Neuropsychologia 2015, 75, 431–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Henrich, K.; Alter, K.; Wiese, R.; Domahs, U. The relevance of rhythmical alternation in language processing: An ERP study on English compounds. Brain Lang. 2014, 136, 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. McCauley, S.M.; Hestvik, A.; Vogel, I. Perception and bias in the processing of compound versus phrasal stress: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Lang. Speech 2013, 56, 23–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Friederici, A.D.; Kotz, S.A. The brain basis of syntactic processes: Functional imaging and lesion studies. NeuroImage 2003, 20, S8–S17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kutas, M.; Federmeier, K.D. Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 621–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Rothermich, K.; Kotz, S.A. Predictions in speech comprehension: fMRI evidence on the meter–semantic interface. NeuroImage 2013, 70, 89–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Magne, C.; Gordon, R.L.; Midha, S. Influence of metrical expectancy on reading words: An ERP study. In Proceedings of the Speech Prosody 2010-Fifth International Conference, Chicago, IL, USA, 10–14 May 2010. [Google Scholar]
  54. Kriukova, O.; Mani, N. Processing metrical information in silent reading: An ERP study. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Fotidzis, T.; Moon, H.; Steele, J.; Magne, C. Cross-Modal Priming Effect of Rhythm on Visual Word Recognition and Its Relationships to Music Aptitude and Reading Achievement. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Francis, W.; Kucera, H. Frequency Analysis of English Usage; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  57. Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Lopez-Calderon, J.; Luck, S.J. ERPLAB: An open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Luck, S.J. An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-0-262-32406-9. [Google Scholar]
  60. Groppe, D.M.; Urbach, T.P.; Kutas, M. Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: A critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology 2011, 48, 1711–1725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lawrence, M.A. ez: Easy Analysis and Visualization of Factorial Experiments. R Package Version 4.4-0. 2016. Available online: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ez (accessed on 30 June 2017).
  63. Zhao, T.C.; Lam, H.T.G.; Sohi, H.; Kuhl, P.K. Neural processing of musical meter in musicians and non-musicians. Neuropsychologia 2017, 106, 289–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Vuust, P.; Pallesen, K.J.; Bailey, C.; van Zuijen, T.L.; Gjedde, A.; Roepstorff, A.; Østergaard, L. To musicians, the message is in the meter: Pre-attentive neuronal responses to incongruent rhythm are left-lateralized in musicians. NeuroImage 2005, 24, 560–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Geiser, E.; Ziegler, E.; Jancke, L.; Meyer, M. Early electrophysiological correlates of meter and rhythm processing in music perception. Cortex 2009, 45, 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Féry, C. German Word Stress in Optimality Theory. J. Comp. Ger. Linguist. 1998, 2, 101–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Hahne, A.; Friederici, A.D. Electrophysiological Evidence for Two Steps in Syntactic Analysis: Early Automatic and Late Controlled Processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999, 11, 194–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Kuperberg, G.R. Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain Res. 2007, 1146, 23–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. O’Rourke, P.L.; Van Petten, C. Morphological agreement at a distance: Dissociation between early and late components of the event-related brain potential. Brain Res. 2011, 1392, 62–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Kretzschmar, F.; Schlesewsky, M.; Staub, A. Dissociating word frequency and predictability effects in reading: Evidence from coregistration of eye movements and EEG. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2015, 41, 1648–1662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Henderson, J.M.; Luke, S.G.; Schmidt, J.; Richards, J.E. Co-registration of eye movements and event-related potentials in connected-text paragraph reading. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Gordon, R.L.; Shivers, C.M.; Wieland, E.A.; Kotz, S.A.; Yoder, P.J.; Devin McAuley, J. Musical rhythm discrimination explains individual differences in grammar skills in children. Dev. Sci. 2015, 18, 635–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Huss, M.; Verney, J.P.; Fosker, T.; Mead, N.; Goswami, U. Music, rhythm, rise time perception and developmental dyslexia: Perception of musical meter predicts reading and phonology. Cortex 2011, 47, 674–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Wood, C. Metrical stress sensitivity in young children and its relationship to phonological awareness and reading. J. Res. Read. 2006, 29, 270–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Breen, M.; Kaswer, L.; Van Dyke, J.A.; Krivokapić, J.; Landi, N. Imitated prosodic fluency predicts reading comprehension ability in good and poor high school readers. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Benjamin, R.G.; Schwanenflugel, P.J. Text Complexity and Oral Reading Prosody in Young Readers. Read. Res. Q. 2010, 45, 388–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Presentation times in milliseconds of each region of the limerick couplets.
Figure 1. Presentation times in milliseconds of each region of the limerick couplets.
Brainsci 09 00192 g001
Figure 2. Event-related potential (ERP) results. (A) Effects of metric predictability on a grand average (n = 18) waveform amplitude for strong–weak (SW) targets (left) and weak–strong (WS) targets (right). Temporal regions of interest identified in the cluster-based permutation analyses are highlighted in grey. Temporal regions of interest that revealed a significant (p < 0.05) main effect of metric consistency in conventional ANOVA analyses are indicated with an asterisk. Waveforms are averaged over the 49 electrodes included in the ANOVA analyses; the 7 (anteriority) × 7 (laterality) grid arrangement used to model electrode position in all ANOVAs is shown in the inset. (B) Scalp maps showing the topography of mean amplitude differences between the inconsistent and consistent conditions within the two temporal regions of interest identified for SW targets (left), and the one temporal region of interest identified for WS targets (right). The scalp region over which a significant (p < 0.05) main effect of metric consistency was observed within the specified time window is outlined in black for each scalp map.
Figure 2. Event-related potential (ERP) results. (A) Effects of metric predictability on a grand average (n = 18) waveform amplitude for strong–weak (SW) targets (left) and weak–strong (WS) targets (right). Temporal regions of interest identified in the cluster-based permutation analyses are highlighted in grey. Temporal regions of interest that revealed a significant (p < 0.05) main effect of metric consistency in conventional ANOVA analyses are indicated with an asterisk. Waveforms are averaged over the 49 electrodes included in the ANOVA analyses; the 7 (anteriority) × 7 (laterality) grid arrangement used to model electrode position in all ANOVAs is shown in the inset. (B) Scalp maps showing the topography of mean amplitude differences between the inconsistent and consistent conditions within the two temporal regions of interest identified for SW targets (left), and the one temporal region of interest identified for WS targets (right). The scalp region over which a significant (p < 0.05) main effect of metric consistency was observed within the specified time window is outlined in black for each scalp map.
Brainsci 09 00192 g002
Table 1. Metric structure of experimental couplets in each of the four conditions for the target word ‘permit’.
Table 1. Metric structure of experimental couplets in each of the four conditions for the target word ‘permit’.
WSWWSWWSW
A.Strong–weak, consistentThereoncewasanoldmannamedKer-mit
whohunt-edwith-outan-yPER-mit
B.Strong–weak, inconsistentThereoncewasanoldmannamedBritt
whohunt-edwith-outa*PER-mit
C.Weak–strong, consistentThereoncewasanoldmannamedBritt
whosevic-esnowifecouldper-MIT
D.Weak–strong, inconsistentThereoncewasanoldmannamedKer-mit
Whosegamb-linghiswifewouldnot*per-MIT
Italics and underlines indicate metrically strong syllables, bold indicates target words, and capital letters indicate lexical stressed syllables within target words. Asterisks (*) indicate metrically inconsistent targets. Screen breaks are indicated with solid vertical lines. Text emphasis is for descriptive purposes only; in the experiment, all words were presented in plain text (see Figure 1).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Breen, M.; Fitzroy, A.B.; Oraa Ali, M. Event-Related Potential Evidence of Implicit Metric Structure during Silent Reading. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 192. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci9080192

AMA Style

Breen M, Fitzroy AB, Oraa Ali M. Event-Related Potential Evidence of Implicit Metric Structure during Silent Reading. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(8):192. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci9080192

Chicago/Turabian Style

Breen, Mara, Ahren B. Fitzroy, and Michelle Oraa Ali. 2019. "Event-Related Potential Evidence of Implicit Metric Structure during Silent Reading" Brain Sciences 9, no. 8: 192. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci9080192

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop