Next Article in Journal
Acknowledgement to Reviewers of Foods in 2017
Next Article in Special Issue
What Is “Natural”? Consumer Responses to Selected Ingredients
Previous Article in Journal
Absorption and Metabolism of Phenolics from Digests of Polyphenol-Rich Potato Extracts Using the Caco-2/HepG2 Co-Culture System
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effect of Emulsion Intensity on Selected Sensory and Instrumental Texture Properties of Full-Fat Mayonnaise

1
Research Environment MEAL, Faculty of Natural Science, Kristianstad University, SE-291 88 Kristianstad, Sweden
2
Perten Instruments AB, SE-254 66 Helsingborg, Sweden
3
Department of Food Technology, Engineering and Nutrition, Lund University, SE-221 00 Lund, Sweden
4
Department of Food Science, University of Copenhagen, DK-1958 Frederiksberg, Denmark
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 13 November 2017 / Revised: 11 January 2018 / Accepted: 12 January 2018 / Published: 17 January 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Analysis of Sensory Properties in Foods)

Abstract

:
Varying processing conditions can strongly affect the microstructure of mayonnaise, opening up new applications for the creation of products tailored to meet different consumer preferences. The aim of the study was to evaluate the effect of emulsification intensity on sensory and instrumental characteristics of full-fat mayonnaise. Mayonnaise, based on a standard recipe, was processed at low and high emulsification intensities, with selected sensory and instrumental properties then evaluated using an analytical panel and a back extrusion method. The evaluation also included a commercial reference mayonnaise. The overall effects of a higher emulsification intensity on the sensory and instrumental characteristics of full-fat mayonnaise were limited. However, texture was affected, with a more intense emulsification resulting in a firmer mayonnaise according to both back extrusion data and the analytical sensory panel. Appearance, taste and flavor attributes were not affected by processing.

1. Introduction

Mayonnaise is an oil-in-water emulsion stabilized by egg yolk and has been produced commercially for more than one hundred years [1]. Traditional mayonnaise is produced in a batch process by slowly adding the oil to the water phase under vigorous mixing, thereby creating an emulsion [2]. Industrially, mixing is achieved using high-intensity rotor-stator mixers, also referred to as high-shear mixers [3]. Although the taste and texture of mayonnaise is appreciated by many consumers, local markets often value different sensory properties. Therefore, as it is known that production techniques such as mixing/homogenization may have a considerable effect on the final product structure [1,4], better knowledge of how processing conditions affect the sensory and instrumental properties of the emulsion could help cater for such varying consumer preferences.
Due to a high oil content, mayonnaise exhibits a semisolid and viscoelastic behavior that influences its particular rheological properties, which in turn contribute to the perceived texture and flavor of the product [5]. In this context, texture is defined as the sensory perception of the structure of a food [6]. According to van Aken et al. [7], the rheological properties of a food product are very important for the perception of a creamy mouthfeel, although other authors have stressed that a variety of aspects may also play a role. For example, the oil droplet size is another parameter of interest due to its ability to influence product appearance, texture, and flavor profile [8].
One way in which the texture of mayonnaise is perceived by the consumer is through its processing and breakdown in the mouth (intra orally) before it is swallowed. In fact, most sensations associated with food texture occur only when the food is manipulated, deformed, or moved across the receptors in the mouth [4]. Through texture analysis, it is possible to choose a compression technique similar to that performed by the mouth, and then measure the behavior of the food using this technique. Such tests are valuable since they can confirm various textural properties, including the creaminess of mayonnaise.
Texture is also perceived outside the mouth (extra orally). Before the food item enters the mouth, visual cues related to the item’s appearance provide information regarding its texture, while additional information can also be obtained by handling the food, e.g., by stirring, spooning, and cutting [4].
The emulsification taking place when mayonnaise is formed in rotor-stator mixers is relatively well understood, and proceeds via hydrodynamic interactions between the dispersed phase and the fluid in the rotor-stator region. Experiments suggest that the dispersed phase is predominantly broken up by turbulent viscous stresses [9]. The diameter of the emulsion drops, U, is produced in the rotor-stator mixer scales with the rotor tip-speed, determined by
U = π N D
and decreases according to the power-law function [9,10]. In Equation (1), N is the rotor speed and D is the rotor diameter. Drop size also decreases with processing time, and scales with the average number of passages, p, through the rotor-stator region [9], which is written as
p = t Q V
where t is processing time, V is the fluid volume, and Q is the flow through the stator screen of the mixer [11,12], expressed as follows:
Q = N Q N D 3
where NQ is a mixer-specific design constant. However, the dynamics of the process is very slow and the droplet size continues to decrease after the emulsion has been processed for more than the equivalent of an average of 100 passages through the rotor-stator region [9].
The effect of processing conditions on the sensory response of mayonnaise is not as well understood as the effect on emulsion drop diameters. Furthermore, studies in which the rheological properties of mayonnaise have been related to perceived texture have predominantly focused on low-fat mayonnaises with oil concentrations ranging from 15–30% [13], thus creating a knowledge gap with regard to how full-fat (~75–80%) mayonnaises are affected.
It has been shown that fat content has a significant effect on perceived thickness and fattiness, with a higher fat content yielding a higher perception of both qualities. However, increased emulsification intensity, which produces smaller droplets, has the opposite effect and has also been shown to affect the perceived sweetness and whiteness of mayonnaise with added aromas [14]. Taste, flavor and textural attributes are also of interest in mayonnaise without added aromas.
The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of emulsification intensity on the sensory and instrumental characteristics of full-fat mayonnaise.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Mayonnaise Ingredients and Processing

The ingredients in the experimental mayonnaise are presented in Table 1 and were assembled according to the following protocol: All ingredients, with the exception of the rapeseed oil and vinegar, were weighed into a 500 mL dispersing vessel (Kinematica, Luzern, Switzerland) and allowed to rest for adjustment to room temperature. The dispersing aggregate PT-DA 20/2 EC-E192 (Kinematica, Luzern, Switzerland) of the rotor-stator mixer (Kinematica Polytron, PT 2500 E, Luzern, Switzerland) was adjusted in the vessel and the mixture processed for 30 s at 6000 rpm. The rapeseed oil was then added, initially dropwise, and the mixture processed at 6000 rpm until all oil was emulsified. Following this stage, the vinegar was added and the mayonnaise mixed for an additional 30 s. Thereafter, one batch (High emulsification intensity) was processed at 9000 rpm (corresponding to a rotor tip-speed of 7.1 m/s) for 6 min, and a second batch (Low emulsification intensity) at 6000 rpm (rotor tip-speed 4.7 m/s) for 9.2 min. These processing times were chosen to achieve the same number of average rotor-stator passages for each rotor speed, i.e., equal p for different N (see Equations (1)–(3)). This procedure was repeated 3 times per processing condition for both batches (High and Low) and pooled in order to obtain enough mayonnaise for sensory and instrumental texture analysis. A commercial mayonnaise (Äkta Majonnäs, Findus), produced in Sweden, was also included as reference; this mayonnaise contained 81% (w/v) rapeseed oil and 4.6% (w/v) egg yolk (Table 2). The rationale for including a commercial reference was dual-fold; not only did it provide an indication of the sensory characteristics of the experimental mayonnaise in comparison to those of a commercial mayonnaise sold on the Swedish retail market, but it also served as a control when comparing the results of the present panel with those of other analytical panels.

2.2. Sensory Evaluation

Sensory evaluation and training were carried out over a period of three days by an external panel (Kristianstad University, Kristianstad, Sweden) of ten assessors, who were selected and trained according to the following guidelines: ISO 3972, ISO8586-1, and ISO8586-2. The sensory laboratory was designed according to ISO 8589 and sensory analysis performed using sensory descriptive analysis [15]. Across two training sessions lasting approximately 2 h each, the panel developed descriptions of the perceived sensory attributes of the products, generating a set of attributes and developing a consensus regarding the evaluation of each attribute (Table 3). Reference materials were used in training for selected attributes such as yellow color, acid taste, and egg flavor.
Product evaluations were performed individually, in isolated booths. Samples (20 g) of mayonnaise were served on coded, disposable plastic dishes and handled using a plastic spoon. The serving temperature was controlled by leaving the samples at room temperature for 10 min prior to serving. Panelists were instructed to rinse their mouths with still or carbonated water after each sample, and were also provided with fresh cucumber, apple, and soft white bread for further palate cleansing. Samples were coded with three-digit codes and served in a randomized order. The panelists then evaluated the perceived intensities by hand on a continuous 100 mm line-scale labeled “low intensity” at 10 mm and “high intensity” at 90 mm. During one evaluation session, lasting 60 min, the panelists evaluated duplicates of each product, with the intensity ratings then translated into numbers.

2.3. Instrumental Texture Analysis

Texture measurements were performed in duplicate, via a back extrusion method using a TVT-300XP analyzer (Perten Instruments AB, Stockholm, Sweden) equipped with a 7 kg load cell and a back extrusion set consisting of a sample container (50 mm diameter) and a compression plate (40 mm diameter). The sampling distance was 20 mm, the test speed 1 mm/s, and the retraction speed 5 mm/s. Texture properties (Table 4) were measured using the TexCalc software (version 4.0.4.67).

2.4. Statistical Methods

From the obtained data, mean values and standard deviations were calculated both for sensory and instrumental analysis. The sensory data were further subjected to a three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), with samples panelists and replicates as fixed effects. The instrumental data were subjected to a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Significant differences (p < 0.05) between samples were calculated via the Bonferroni’s pairwise comparison test (Panel Check, v. 1.4.2, https://sourceforge.net/projects/sensorytool, Nofima, Norway).
Pearson correlations between instrumental and sensory data were calculated using Excel 2013 (Office for Windows), and Principal Component Analysis (PCA) performed using Panel Check (v. 1.4.2).
Panel performance was checked by calculating p- and MSE-values and then plotting these values in p-MSE-diagrams (Panel Check, v. 1.4.2).

3. Results

3.1. Sensory Evaluation

Panelist performance was found to be reliable based on the calculation of p-MSE-values showing low p- and MSE-values for all panelists. No significant effects were obtained due to replicates, with the exception of the attribute saltiness for which there was no significant difference between products.
Mixing intensity affected extra-oral texture attributes, i.e., those obtained by handling the mayonnaise through stirring and spooning (Table 5). The higher mixing intensity (rotor tip-speed 7.1 m/s) led to a significantly firmer, more viscous mayonnaise compared to the lower mixing intensity (rotor tip-speed 4.7 m/s) when handled by spoon. The results also indicated that the higher emulsification intensity produced a mayonnaise perceived as more creamy, although not to a statistically significant level.
Neither appearance nor taste or flavor attributes were affected by emulsion intensity (Table 5). The commercial, reference mayonnaise stood out in the sensory evaluation as having a more pronounced yellow color and a firmer, more creamy texture when assessed both extra- and intra-orally. The panel also perceived a more intense acidic flavor and a more intense total flavor in the commercial reference mayonnaise as compared to those prepared for the study (Figure 2).

3.2. Instrumental Texture Analysis

All samples exhibited the same behavior during back extrusion measurements, but to different extents, as shown in Figure 3.
The commercial reference sample was found to be the most firm, sticky and adhesive mayonnaise, followed by the mayonnaise produced with a high emulsification intensity (Table 6).
Consequently, both the sensory and instrumental data imply that a higher emulsification intensity results in a firmer full-fat mayonnaise. When correlating the instrumental data to the sensory data the correlations were generally high (r ≥ 0.9), primarily reflecting the pronounced difference between (i) the experimental mayonnaises (high and low emulsification intensity) and (ii) the commercial reference sample.

4. Discussion

A higher emulsification intensity affects the microstructure of mayonnaise by decreasing the droplet size [9,10], thus potentially affecting sensory traits such as texture [13], color [14], and flavor [8]. However, our results revealed no significant difference between the experimental samples with regard to color, taste, or flavor. As smaller particles increase light scattering, a reduced droplet size leads to a whiter mayonnaise, a phenomenon that has been illustrated in cream cheese, in which homogenization was found to lower the intensity of the yellow color [16]. In theory, flavor release decreases with increasing droplet size, as it takes longer for flavor molecules to diffuse out of a larger droplet. However, polar and non-polar flavor molecules behave differently in this respect, and the influence of droplet size on the rate of flavor release depends on the nature of the system [8]. In the study conducted by Wendin, Langton, Caous and Hall [16], smaller droplet sizes in cream cheese resulted in a shorter duration of the dynamic sensation of “fat-creamy”.
The samples showed significant textural differences linked to the intensity of emulsification, with a more intense emulsification producing higher firmness and creaminess, as well as a decrease in adhesiveness to the spoon when handled. The textural attributes of mayonnaise can be explained by the elastic parameters of dynamic viscoelasticity (G’). This property is strongly related to particle size at 10% cumulative volume, which is in turn negatively correlated with sensory attributes including hardness, fracturability, and adhesiveness [13]. The perception of texture is a complex process involving the senses of vision, hearing, somesthesis, and kinesthesis [17]. Neurologically, texture perception results from the interaction of sensory and motor components of the peripheral nervous system with the central nervous system. Initially, the sight and extra-oral manipulation of food, e.g., through using a spoon, sets up sensory expectations regarding texture [18]. Then, once the food is put into the mouth, texture perception is a dynamic process, as the physical properties of foods change continuously when manipulated intra-orally. In this respect it would be interesting to further examine the question of how well texture attributes that are perceived extra-orally correlate with perceived oral-somatosensory attributes.
In the present study, the emulsion drop size distribution was not measured. However, previous investigations have shown that the scaling of drop-diameter averages with rotor tip-speed is highly predictable [9,10]. Using this previously established scaling, the higher emulsification intensity (rotor tip-speed 7.1 m/s compared to 4.7 m/s) corresponds to an expected reduction in the average oil drop diameter by a factor of two [9]. This is a rather substantial reduction that was expected to lead to quality differences with regard to the appearance, texture and flavor of the product. However, only extra-oral textural attributes were affected to a degree that could be perceived by the sensory panel.
Our findings, that a more intense emulsification and hence a decreased oil drop diameter produces a firmer mayonnaise, compare well with earlier results regarding the effect of microstructure on food emulsions [13,16]. The instrumental texture analysis data support the theory that a decreased droplet size leads to textural alterations, resulting in a more firm and adhesive mayonnaise. These findings may be helpful for the control and prediction of mayonnaise texture using processing conditions rather than more common approaches such as adding texture modifiers, which in the age of growing consumer preference for “clean labels” are unwanted in many products. Understanding the microstructural changes that occur during processing and the role of different mayonnaise ingredients will allow for better control of product structure and, ultimately, the manipulation and regulation of product texture [17].
The study conducted by Maruyama, Sakashita, Hagura and Suzuki [13] is just one among many to report that temperature during preparation may affect the physical properties of mayonnaise. Thus, if the aim is to obtain reproducible results, a consistent temperature is essential. In the present study, the ingredients were left to adjust to room temperature at approximately 20 °C, with the mayonnaise thereafter prepared at the same temperature. Compared to industrial preconditions, in which emulsification is commonly performed under cooling, the temperature in this study was high and control was inadequate, which might have influenced the results. Since emulsion formation is controlled by viscous drop breakup [9], a high temperature at the onset of emulsification will decrease the viscosity of the emulsion, reducing the viscous shear forces and thus resulting in larger drop sizes.

5. Conclusions

The effects of a higher emulsification intensity, corresponding to an expected reduction in the average oil droplet diameter by a factor of two, on the sensory and instrumental characteristics of full-fat mayonnaise were limited. Perceived and instrumentally measured texture was affected, with a more intense emulsification resulting in a firmer mayonnaise, as measured via back extrusion and by an analytical sensory panel. However, appearance, taste and flavor attributes were not affected by processing.

Acknowledgments

We hereby acknowledge the valuable technical assistance provided by Sarah Forsberg and Therése Svensson. Funding: This work was supported by Kristianstad University.

Author Contributions

Viktoria Olsson, Andreas Håkansson and Karin Wendin conceived and designed the experiments; Viktoria Olsson and Jeanette Purhagen performed the experiments; Karin Wendin and Andreas Håkansson analyzed the data; Viktoria Olsson, Andreas Håkansson, Jeanette Purhagen and Karin Wendin wrote the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Harrison, L.; Cunningham, F. Factors influencing the quality of mayonnaise: A review. J. Food Qual. 1985, 8, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Depree, J.A.; Savage, G.P. Physical and flavour stability of mayonnaise. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2001, 12, 157–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bengoechea, C.; Lopez, M.L.; Cordobes, F.; Guerrero, A. Influence of semicontinuous processing on the rheology and droplet size distribution of mayonnaise-like emulsions. Food Sci. Technol. Int. 2009, 15, 367–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Engelen, L.; Van Der Bilt, A. Oral physiology and texture perception of semisolids. J. Texture Stud. 2008, 39, 83–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Stern, P.; Valentova, H.; Pokorny, J. Rheological properties and sensory texture of mayonnaise. Eur. J. Lipid Sci. Technol. 2001, 103, 23–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Weenen, H.; Van Gemert, L.J.; Van Doorn, J.M.; Dijksterhuis, G.B.; De Wijk, R.A. Texture and mouthfeel of semisolid foods: Commercial mayonnaises, dressings, custard desserts and warm sauces. J. Texture Stud. 2003, 34, 159–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Van Aken, G.A.; Vingerhoeds, M.H.; de Wijk, R.A. Textural perception of liquid emulsions: Role of oil content, oil viscosity and emulsion viscosity. Food Hydrocoll. 2011, 25, 789–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. McClements, D.J. Food Emulsions: Principles, Practices, and Techniques, 3rd ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  9. Håkansson, A.; Chaudhry, Z.; Innings, F. Model emulsions to study the mechanism of industrial mayonnaise emulsification. Food Bioprod. Process. 2016, 98, 189–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Adler-Nissen, J.; Mason, S.L.; Jacobsen, C. Apparatus for emulsion production in small scale and under controlled shear conditions. Food Bioprod. Process. 2004, 82, 311–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Cooke, M.; Rodgers, T.L.; Kowalski, A.J. Power consumption characteristics of an in-line silverson high shear mixer. AIChE J. 2012, 58, 1683–1692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Mortensen, H.H.; Innings, F.; Håkansson, A. The effect of stator design on flowrate and velocity fields in a rotor-stator mixer-an experimental investigation. Chem. Eng. Res. Des. 2017, 121, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Maruyama, K.; Sakashita, T.; Hagura, Y.; Suzuki, K. Relationship between rheology, particle size and texture of mayonnaise. Food Sci. Technol. Res. 2007, 13, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Wendin, K.; Ellekjaer, M.R.; Solheim, R. Fat content and homogenization effects on flavour and texture of mayonnaise with added aroma. LWT-Food Sci. Technol. 1999, 32, 377–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Stone, H.; Sidel, J.L. Sensory Evaluation Practices, 3rd ed.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  16. Wendin, K.; Langton, M.; Caous, L.; Hall, C. Dynamic analyses of sensory and microstructural properties of cream cheese. Food Chem. 2000, 71, 363–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Wilkinson, C.; Dijksterhuis, G.B.; Minekus, M. From food structure to texture. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2000, 11, 442–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Spence, C.; Hobkinson, C.; Gallace, A.; Fiszman, B.P. A touch of gastronomy. Flavour 2013, 2, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Movement of sample: (a) work of compression; (b) adhesiveness.
Figure 1. Movement of sample: (a) work of compression; (b) adhesiveness.
Foods 07 00009 g001
Figure 2. Principal component analysis (PCA) illustrating the sensory and instrumental characteristics of the experimental and commercial reference (Ref.) mayonnaises. The PCA plot shows 100% of the explained variance, meaning that the total variance is explained by two dimensions. A: Shiny; B: Yellow; C: Adhesiveness Spoon; D: Firmness; E: Fatty Mouthfeel; F: Creaminess; G: Acidity; H: Sweetness; I: Egg flavor; J: Saltiness; K: Total Flavour; L: Inst Firmness; M: Inst Compression; N: Inst Stickiness; O: Inst Adhesiveness: P: Inst Gradient 1; Q: Inst Gradient 2.
Figure 2. Principal component analysis (PCA) illustrating the sensory and instrumental characteristics of the experimental and commercial reference (Ref.) mayonnaises. The PCA plot shows 100% of the explained variance, meaning that the total variance is explained by two dimensions. A: Shiny; B: Yellow; C: Adhesiveness Spoon; D: Firmness; E: Fatty Mouthfeel; F: Creaminess; G: Acidity; H: Sweetness; I: Egg flavor; J: Saltiness; K: Total Flavour; L: Inst Firmness; M: Inst Compression; N: Inst Stickiness; O: Inst Adhesiveness: P: Inst Gradient 1; Q: Inst Gradient 2.
Foods 07 00009 g002
Figure 3. Graphs of back extrusion measurements. The black line represents the commercial reference mayonnaise, the gray line with circles the high emulsification intensity mayonnaise, and the gray line with squares the low emulsification intensity mayonnaise.
Figure 3. Graphs of back extrusion measurements. The black line represents the commercial reference mayonnaise, the gray line with circles the high emulsification intensity mayonnaise, and the gray line with squares the low emulsification intensity mayonnaise.
Foods 07 00009 g003
Table 1. List of ingredients in the experimental mayonnaise.
Table 1. List of ingredients in the experimental mayonnaise.
IngredientWeight (g)w/v (%)
Rapeseed oil321.681.2
Egg yolk34.18.6
Water23.35.9
Mustard102.5
Vinegar (acetic acid 12%)4.81.2
Salt1.20.3
Sugar1.20.3
Table 2. List of ingredients in the commercial reference sample.
Table 2. List of ingredients in the commercial reference sample.
Ingredientw/v (%)
Rapeseed oil81
Vinegar
Egg yolk4.6
Mustard seeds
Sugar
Salt0.7
White pepper
Thickener (E401, E412, E417)
Cayenne pepper
Preservatives(E211)
Colorant, beta-carotene
Table 3. Sensory attributes and definitions established by the panel.
Table 3. Sensory attributes and definitions established by the panel.
CategoryAttributeDefinition
AppearanceShinyDegree of shininess
YellowGradation from a weak to a strong tone of (vanilla) yellow
Texture (extra-oral)Adhesiveness to spoonAmount of mayonnaise remaining on the spoon when held vertically
FirmnessDegree of resistance when stirring with a spoon
Texture (intra-oral)Fatty mouthfeelGraded from a little to a high grade of perceived fattiness
CreaminessDegree of creaminess; yoghurt used as reference
TasteAcidityTaste of sourness; vinegar and lemon used as reference
SweetnessThe pure taste of sucrose; no reference used, evaluation relied on individual recollection of sweet taste
SaltinessThe pure taste of sodium chloride; no reference used, evaluation relied on individual recollection of salty taste
FlavorEgg flavorSulfur, boiled egg; boiled eggs used as reference
Total flavorThe total intensity of taste and flavor
Table 4. Texture properties measured.
Table 4. Texture properties measured.
Texture PropertyDefinition
Firmness (g)The maximum compression force
Work of compression (g·mm)The ability of the sample to flow around the probe (see Figure 1)
Stickiness (g)The maximum (negative) force recorded during the withdrawal phase.
Adhesiveness (g∙mm)The work required to withdraw the probe through the sample (see Figure 1)
Gradient 1 (g/mm)The gradient of the first third of the compression distance
Gradient 2 (g/mm)The gradient of the second third of the compression distance
Table 5. Sensory evaluation of the intensity of selected attributes, comparing the experimental mayonnaises and the commercial reference. Different letters in the same row indicate significant differences at p ≤ 0.05.
Table 5. Sensory evaluation of the intensity of selected attributes, comparing the experimental mayonnaises and the commercial reference. Different letters in the same row indicate significant differences at p ≤ 0.05.
Sensory AttributeHigh Emulsification IntensityLow Emulsification IntensityCommercial Reference Mayonnaise
Shiny62 ± 12 a65 ± 9 a70 ± 11 a
Yellow43 ± 11 a41 ± 12 a84 ± 8 b
Adhesiveness to spoon40 ± 17 a58 ± 19 b28 ± 14 c
Firmness54 ± 9 a47 ± 11 b70 ± 12 c
Fatty mouthfeel58 ± 17 a57 ± 14 a58 ± 14 a
Creaminess60 ± 9 a56 ± 13 a70 ± 11 b
Acidity34 ± 15 a33 ± 11 a55 ± 14 b
Sweetness29 ± 10 a28 ± 9 a24 ± 9 a
Saltiness23 ± 7 a23 ± 7 a28 ± 11 a
Egg flavor32 ± 10 a32 ± 9 a35 ± 7 a
Total flavor45 ± 10 a43 ± 9 a61 ± 13 b
Different letters in the same row indicate significant differences at p ≤ 0.05.
Table 6. Instrumental texture analysis as performed via a back extrusion method. Different letters in the same row indicate significant differences at p ≤ 0.05.
Table 6. Instrumental texture analysis as performed via a back extrusion method. Different letters in the same row indicate significant differences at p ≤ 0.05.
High Emulsification IntensityLow Emulsification IntensityCommercial Reference Mayonnaise
Firmness (g)252 ± 14 a230 ± 15 a292 ± 11 b
Stickiness (g)−301 ± 22 a−269 ± 19 a−349 ± 23 b
Adhesiveness (J)40 ± 3 a35 ± 5 a48 ± 3 b
Gradient 1 (g/mm)29 ± 1 b27 ± 1 a37 ± 11 a,b
Gradient 2 (g/mm)5 ± 2 a5 ± 1 a10 ± 3 a
Different letters in the same row indicate significant differences at p ≤ 0.05.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Olsson, V.; Håkansson, A.; Purhagen, J.; Wendin, K. The Effect of Emulsion Intensity on Selected Sensory and Instrumental Texture Properties of Full-Fat Mayonnaise. Foods 2018, 7, 9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/foods7010009

AMA Style

Olsson V, Håkansson A, Purhagen J, Wendin K. The Effect of Emulsion Intensity on Selected Sensory and Instrumental Texture Properties of Full-Fat Mayonnaise. Foods. 2018; 7(1):9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/foods7010009

Chicago/Turabian Style

Olsson, Viktoria, Andreas Håkansson, Jeanette Purhagen, and Karin Wendin. 2018. "The Effect of Emulsion Intensity on Selected Sensory and Instrumental Texture Properties of Full-Fat Mayonnaise" Foods 7, no. 1: 9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/foods7010009

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop