Next Article in Journal
Hazard Warning Systems to Improve Young Distracted Drivers’ Hazard Perception Skills
Previous Article in Journal
Hazards That Can Affect CNC Machine Tools during Operation—An AHP Approach
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Testing the Effectiveness of a Mixed-Method Pilot Intervention in Reducing Risky Driving Due to Aggression and Stress

Laboratory of Health and Road Safety, School of Health Sciences, Hellenic Mediterranean University, 71410 Heraklion, Greece
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 22 December 2019 / Revised: 5 February 2020 / Accepted: 8 February 2020 / Published: 14 February 2020

Abstract

:
The study aimed at testing the effectiveness of a mixed-method pilot intervention in reducing risky self-reported driving performance, upon addressing stress and aggression while driving. The study recruited individuals who had performed these behaviors during the year preceding the study and allocated them into an intervention (n = 10) and a control group (n = 30). A pre-and postintervention evaluation design was employed to explore changes in risky self-reported driving behaviors, 12 months after the intervention. The intervention involved 2 h of experiential instruction and 1 h of cognitive restructuring using a driving simulator and scenarios appropriate for the processing of driving stress, aggression, and risk. The intervention group displayed significant improvements in the scales of “Hazard Monitoring” (p = 0.037) and “Covered Violations” (p = 0.049) at the postintervention level. No statistically significant differences were identified in terms of self-reported driving performance between the intervention and the control group at postintervention level. Launching large-scale experimental surveys with broadened cognitive restructuring approaches seems important to deepen our understanding of the behavioral change processes and increase the effectiveness of future interventions.

1. Introduction

Cognitions have been suggested to elicit driving anger and increase crash risk. In fact, biased and erroneous information processing have been thought to be cognitive processes associated with an endogenous propensity to experience excessive anger under provocation. Certain patterns of thinking (e.g., misattributing causation, overgeneralization) have been suggested to be dormant, until activated in stress situations, resulting in excessive anger. Such thoughts have been correlated with driver aggression, in the event of driving-related stressors and with road traffic incidents [1]. For example, it is very common for drivers to perceive other drivers’ behaviors as insults, ending up with a need for retaliation and consequently with aggressive driving and crash involvement. Such correlations between angry thoughts (cognition), driving anger (emotional component), and aggressive behavior (behavioral component) have been consistently recorded in international literature [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Unfair aggression in the form of violating other drivers’ rights and hostile behavior towards other drivers have been suggested to be dangerous consequences of driving in stressful situations, having also the potential to disrupt smooth circulation due to activating negative emotion and anger [7,8,9,10]. Moreover, prolonged or repeated experience of stressful conditions has been shown to increase aggression [11,12,13]. Several recently published studies have demonstrated the linear association between driver aggression and the probability of crash involvement [14,15].
Despite the scientific evidence highlighting the impact of driver stress and aggression on risky behavior, a recent systematic review of the literature underlined the fact that empirical evidence has only recently started to develop in terms of cognitive and behavioral interventions used for the improvement of driver aggression [16]. It seems from the review that such interventions are still missing from road safety research despite the wealth of evidence in support of these interventions from other fields of research. Novel interventions using virtual reality and simulation-induced cognitive restructuring in anxiety and anger management have only recently emerged and been tested in pilot studies [17]. Emphasis is thus placed on the need to develop targeted interventions with cognitive restructuring approaches tailored to the special characteristics of various population groups [16,17].
In response to this call for interventions, attention has lately been paid to interventions that use cognitive restructuring to address a wide range of nonclinical issues directly related to driving, such as speeding, unrealistic optimism, risk perception, and limitations of cognition [18,19,20]. Most of these studies have yielded a significant difference in safety attitudes of drivers between experimental and control groups upon completion of the intervention. Nevertheless, these studies suffered several limitations, including the lack of a control group, the lack of female participants, and a general methodological inconsistency [20].
In Greece, interventions to manage stress and aggression while driving are totally missing. There are various one-day private awareness raising initiatives focusing primarily on the risk behaviors of adults and minors, but none of these have been evaluated in terms of their effectiveness in achieving behavioral change [21]. The current study is preliminary research, conducted in a road safety lab, which is operated by social and behavioral scientists. It aimed to design and test the effectiveness of a mixed-methods pilot intervention in reducing risky driving behavior, upon addressing driving stress and aggression. The study was expected to detect improvements in attitudes and self-reported driving behaviors among those receiving the intervention, 12 months after the intervention. This is a first attempt to apply cognitive behavioral interventions to address driver aggression and the first time assessing and recording indicators of effectiveness of such interventions in the Greek context.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

A two-stage process was employed for the participants’ recruitment. Firstly, individuals were selected from public areas of the region of Crete in Greece, via convenience sampling, based on the following set of inclusion criteria: (a) age (adult men and women), (b) licensure (possessing a driver’s license), and (c) driving exposure (driving at least 3 times a week throughout the previous year). Secondly, individuals who met the criteria (n = 150), completed the Driver Behavior Questionnaire (DBQ), which served as a filter to identify individuals to be included in the intervention. Individuals displaying the highest scores of risky self-reported driving behavior, based on the DBQ (n = 40), were recruited for the intervention study and were randomly allocated to an intervention (n = 10) and a control group (n = 30) with a ratio of 1:3. In fact, although the optimum case-to-control ratio is thought to be 1:1, it has been suggested that recruiting multiple controls for each case can increase the statistical power when the number of cases is limited [22], as in our study. In terms of evaluation, a pre- and postintervention evaluation design was employed to explore changes in risky self-reported driving behaviors 12 months after the intervention.

2.2. Contents of the Intervention

A mixed-method intervention study was designed on the basis of international literature [23,24,25], combining interview- and simulation-based cognitive restructuring components. Individuals in the intervention group received a 3-hour individual intervention, which included 2 h of experiential instruction and 1 h of cognitive restructuring using a driving simulator. Certain scenarios were employed to be appropriate for the processing of driving stress, aggression, and risk. The first part of the intervention was about decision-making while driving. The second part addressed common myths on aggressive and risky driving and offered participants the opportunity to reflect on false beliefs about factors that contribute to crash risk. The third part of the intervention concerned the effect of stress on self-reported driving behavior and examined common cognitive errors and violations related to driving. External factors that trigger stress reactions were examined, as well as their impact on self-reported driving behavior. The fourth part of the intervention included time for practicing on the driving simulator and was aimed at increasing participants’ understanding of the cognitive errors made while driving. Participants were requested to drive under predetermined conditions, such as driving at high speeds, under the influence of alcohol, and while using a mobile phone. The exercises aimed at improving self-detection and assessment of stress levels experienced at different situations and their association with the crash risk. Participants were interviewed upon completion of the exercises. The interview aimed at exploring the stress levels experienced by the participants as well as their perception of risk, the decisions made while driving, and the actual crash risk.

2.3. Research Instruments—Outcome Measures

All the study participants (intervention and control group) completed the following two questionnaires, at pre- and 12 months postintervention:
(a)
Driver Stress Inventory [DSI] [26]: included four scales (“Aggression”, “Thrill Seeking”, “Dislike of Driving”, and “Hazard Monitoring”) with the responses ranging from 0 to 10.
(b)
Driver Behavior Questionnaire DBQ [27]: included two scales (“Unintentional Violations” and “Lapses”), with the responses ranging from 0 to 5.
Individuals allocated to the intervention group completed two additional scales concerning their self-reported performance, at pre- and 12 months post-intervention:
(a)
Driver Attitude Questionnaire (DAQ) [28]: included four scales (“Drinking and Driving”, “Close Following”, “Risky Overtaking”, and “Speeding”) with the responses ranging from 1 to 5.
(b)
Deffenbacher Driving Anger Scale [DAS] [29]: responses ranged from 1 to 5.

2.4. Data Analysis

All analyses were performed to describe the participants’ key characteristics. Fisher’s exact test (for categorical variables) and independent samples t-test (for continuous variables) were used to compare the intervention with the control group in terms of their main sociodemographic and driving background characteristics. The nonparametric Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test was used to compare the pre- and the postintervention performance of those who received the intervention in terms of anxiety, driver aggression, and risky self-reported driving behavior. The nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test was used to explore the differences between the intervention and the control group in terms of anxiety, driver aggression, and risky self-reported driving scores, at postintervention level. All analyses were performed with SPSS v. 21.0 and STATA v. 12.0 and a significance level of 0.05 was used as a threshold to determine the statistically significant differences.

3. Results

3.1. Participants’ Profile

The sociodemographic and the driving profile of the two groups are presented in Table 1. The intervention group involved 10 participants (8 men), with a mean age of 26.2 (SD 7.5) years. Six of them (60.0%) reported a driving experience of ≤5 years, 7 (70.0%) used a car on daily basis, and 5 (50.0%) had been involved in a car accident during the past year. On the other hand, the control group involved 30 participants with 19 male (63.3%) and a mean age of 27.8 (SD 9.9) years. More than half of them (n = 77; 56.7%) reported a driving experience of ≤5 years and the majority (68.7%) used a car on daily basis. The car was the most common means of transport for the majority of the participants (72.0%). Nearly half of them (n = 13; 43.3%) had been involved in a traffic accident in the past year. Comparisons at the preintervention level between the two groups in terms of their sociodemographic and driving profiles showed no statistically significant differences (p > 0.05, Table 1). This indicates that the two groups were relatively equivalent in terms of key personal characteristics.

3.2. Self-Reported Driving Behavior of the Intervention Group at Pre-and Postintervention Level

Comparison between the pre- and the postintervention self-reported driving behavior of the intervention group is presented in Table 2. A statistically significant difference was evident in the “Hazard Monitoring Scale” (DSI), with the median value being 0.8 at 12 months postintervention (median 25th–75th; 6.6 (5.6, 7.0) as compared with 5.8 (5.4, 6.9) at preintervention level (p = 0.037)). This difference implies that 12 months after the intervention, the participants of the intervention group displayed significant improvements in their self-reported driving performance in terms of danger detection while driving. Furthermore, there has been a statistically significant difference in the “Covered Violations Scale” (DBQ), with the median value being lower by 0.7 at postintervention (median (25th–75th): 0.8 (0.7, 1.3) as compared with 1.5 (1.3, 2.2) at preintervention level (p = 0.049). This difference implies that 12 months after the intervention, participants of the intervention group displayed significant improvements in their self-reported violations. No statistically significant differences were found in any of the other scales (p > 0.05, Table 2) although the scores at the postintervention level were slightly improved as compared with the ones at preintervention.

3.3. Self-Reported Driving Behavior of the Control Group at Pre-and Postintervention Level

The self-reported driving behavior of the control group is presented in Table 3. Comparisons between the pre- and the postintervention performance of the control group in the DSI and the DBQ scales showed no statistically significant difference (p > 0.05, Table 3).

3.4. Comparison between the Intervention and the Control Group in Terms of Their Self-Reported Driving Behavior after the Intervention

Despite the fact that the self-reported driving behavior of the intervention group was slightly better than the one of the control group in certain domains (e.g., “Aggression”, “Thrill Seeking”, “Hazard Monitoring”, “Unintentional Violations”), those differences were not shown to be statistically significant in any of the scales examined at 12 months postintervention (p > 0.05, Table 4).

4. Discussion

The mixed-model pilot intervention designed for this study has not been able to confirm all the original assumptions. No solid evidence was derived regarding the effectiveness of short-term cognitive restructuring and simulation stimuli in reducing self-reported stress and aggression while driving. Nevertheless, improvements achieved in certain domains of self-reported driving performance (Hazard Monitoring p = 0.037; Covered Violations p = 0.049) need to be acknowledged as they are considered to be initial evidence of effectiveness with great potential in influencing other domains of safe driving. In fact, Rowden et al. [10] noted that hazard detection encourages the driver to adopt a more responsible behavior in driving, thereby reducing offenses, and given the short term intervention applied, it is a positive outcome for the intervention group, which could mean that long-term intervention could be possible to make a significant difference. Our findings, even though not as strong as reported in Zinzow et al. [17], who found a 29% decline in aggressive driving and a 21% decline in risky driving, suggest that such interventions, if appropriately redesigned, can prove to be useful in terms of reducing risky self-reported driving behavior and stress levels while driving.
What also stands out from the results is the fact that improvements achieved in this study in the intervention group were still evidenced a year after the intervention. This could imply that our intervention holds the potential of bringing and maintaining changes in certain driving domains, in the long run. However, we need to acknowledge that the design of this study does not allow drawing such conclusions securely, as the investigation of other confounding factors, which may have influenced our participants’ self-reported behaviors, was not possible.
Interestingly, among the DAQ attitudes that were targeted through the intervention, attitudes towards “Speeding” became even more lenient after the intervention, while attitudes towards other domains either slightly improved (e.g., “Drinking and Driving” and “Risky Overtaking”), or remained unaffected a year after the intervention (“Close Following”). This finding is consistent with previous research identifying speeding as a common aberrant driving behavior, which is less influenced by interventions [28,30,31]. Although these observations were not found to be statistically significant, they still have the potential to indicate directions for future research.
Another important observation that needs to be acknowledged is related to the performance of the two groups in most of the domains of safe driving, a year after the intervention. Although the differences between the intervention and the control group in self-reported driving behaviors were not shown to be statistically significant, the fact that self-reported driving performance was improved in the intervention group and worsened in the control group a year after the intervention is a piece of evidence that holds a lot of promise and needs further investigation.

Strengths and Weaknesses

It is important to acknowledge a number of weaknesses and strengths of the study for future reference. Firstly, convenience sampling leads to results that are not generalizable to the general population. Secondly, the small number of participants in the current intervention cannot lead to safe conclusions. Thirdly, the self-report method that was employed in the current study may include recall errors. Fourthly, a brief intervention may not be sufficient enough to address all the important parameters of safe driving.
Among the strengths of this study, the most important ones are the mixed-method design of the intervention, the delivery of the intervention on an individual basis, the use of standardized instruments, and the long-term follow-up, which seems to be missing from similar interventions. The results could have been different if a longer intervention had been designed and if complementary teaching methods were employed, such as virtual reality technology. A pilot study conducted by Zinzow et al. [32] on U.S. veterans who presented with signs of driving phobia, hyperarousal, anxiety/anger related thoughts and behaviors, risky driving, and PTSD symptoms has shown great effectiveness in managing a decline in hyperarousal in driving situations and aggressive and risky driving with the use of virtual reality and cognitive behavioral intervention in multiple sessions.

5. Conclusions

The current pilot intervention has been partly successful in reducing driving violations, a major cause of traffic accidents. If the sample in the intervention group was larger, there would be a greater chance of detecting statistically significant differences in stress and risky driving between the intervention and the control group. Nevertheless, the maintenance of behavioral change in the long term could be considered the major success of this study, which offers us important input for future initiatives. The improvements found in this study should be handled with caution due to the various study limitations.
What comes out of the findings of this study is the fact that combining psychological interventions with practice learning on simulated environments could be effective in achieving and maintaining changes in the long term in certain behavioral elements. Revisiting the study design in future initiatives would be important. Introducing a large-scale intervention design that contains an extended number and duration of sessions, focusing on the individual or the group, with a therapeutic approach and plan, would probably strengthen the outcomes and contribute to the longer maintenance of the behavioral gains. Introducing complementary evaluation methods and more sophisticated analysis would also be important in the future, in order to be able to capture more information regarding the profile of participants who display greater propensity to risk behavior. Finally, future interventions could consider employing anger management interventions, tranquility enhancement, and general self-control strategies to treat aggression as an element of participants’ personality in cases of high levels of aggressive driving.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.P. and J.C.; methodology, M.P., S.L., and J.C.; validation, M.P., S.L., and J.C.; formal analysis, M.P., N.S., S.B., S.L., and J.C.; investigation, M.P., N.S., S.B., S.L., and J.C.; writing—original draft preparation, M.P., N.S., and S.B.; writing—review and editing, M.P., N.S., S.B., S.L., and J.C.; supervision, M.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethics Approval

Ethical approval for this study was granted by the University Review Board (Ref. No 3153/25.01.2017).

References

  1. Deffenbacher, J.L.; Petrilli, R.T.; Lynch, R.S.; Oetting, E.R.; Swaim, R.C. The Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire: A measure of angry cognitions when driving. Cogn. Res. 2003, 27, 383–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Eckhardt, C.; Norlander, B.; Deffenbacher, J. The assessment of anger and hostility: A critical review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2004, 9, 17–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Hennessy, D.A.; Wiesenthal, D.L. Aggression, Violence, and Vengeance among Male and Female Drivers. Transp. Q. 2002, 56, 65–75. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/239522324_Aggression_Violence_and_Vengeance_Among_Male_and_Female_Drivers (accessed on 1 November 2019).
  4. Neighbors, C.; Vietor, N.A.; Knee, R.C. A Motivational Model of Driving Anger and Aggression. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 324–335. Available online: http://faculty.washington.edu/claytonn/publications/Neighbors_Vietor_Knee_2002.pdf (accessed on 13 November 2019).
  5. Parker, D.; Lajunen, T.; Summala, H. Anger and aggression among drivers in three European countries. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2002, 2, 229–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wickens, C.M.; Wiesenthal, D.L.; Rippey, K. Motorists’ Perceptions of Aggressive Driving: A Comparative Analysis of Ontario and California Drivers. In Contemporary Issues in Road User Behavior and Traffic Safety; Hennessy, D.A., Wiesenthal, D.L., Eds.; Nova Science Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 23–36. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2006-04455-003 (accessed on 13 June 2019).
  7. Shinar, D. Aggressive driving: The contribution of the drivers and the situation. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 1998, 2, 137–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lagarde, E.; Chastang, J.F.; Gueguen, A.; Coeuret-Pellicer, M.; Chiron, M.; Lafont, S. Emotional Stress and Traffic Accidents: The Impact of Separation and Divorce. Epidemiology 2004, 15, 762–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Legree, P.J.; Heffner, T.S.; Psotka, J.; Martin, D.E.; Medsker, G. Traffic crash involvement: Experiential driving knowledge and stressful contextual antecedents. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 15–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Rowden, P.; Matthews, G.; Watson, B.; Biggs, H. The relative impact of work-related stress, life stress and driving environment stress on driving outcomes. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2011, 43, 1332–1340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Cohen, S. Aftereffects of stress on human performance and social behavior: A review of research and theory. Psychol. Bull. 1980, 88, 82–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Gibson, P.M.; Wiesenthal, D.L. The Driving Vengeance Questionnaire: The development of a scale to measure deviant drivers’ attitudes. Violence Vict. 2000, 15, 115–136. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hennessy, D.A.; Wiesenthal, D.L. Traffic congestion, driver stress, and driver aggression. Aggress. Behav. 1999, 25, 409–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ge, Y.; Qu, W.; Zhang, Q.; Zhao, W.; Zhang, K. Psychometric adaptation of the driving anger expression inventory in a Chinese sample. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2015, 33, 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kaiser, S.; Furian, G.; Schlembach, C. Aggressive Behaviour in Road Traffic—Findings from Austria. Transp. Res. Procedia 2016, 12, 4384–4392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Deffenbacher, J. A review of interventions for the reduction of driving anger. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2016, 42, 411–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Zinzow, H.M.; Jeffirs, S.M. Driving Aggression and Anxiety: Intersections, Assessment, and Interventions. J. Clin. Phsychol. 2018, 74, 43–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Najeeb, P.M.M. Study of effect of cognitive behavioral therapy for drivers Improvement. Presented at the 2008 Australasian Road Safety Research, Policing and Education Conference, Adelaide, Australia, 9–12 November 2008; Available online: http://acrs.org.au/files/arsrpe/RS080048.pdf (accessed on 12 June 2016).
  19. Strom, T.; Leskela, J.; Possis, E.; Thuras, P.; Leuty, M.E.; Doane, B.M.; Wilder-Schaaf, K.; Rosenzweig, L. Cognitive–behavioral group treatment for driving-related anger, aggression, and risky driving in combat veterans: A pilot study. J. Trauma Stress 2013, 26, 405–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fernandez-Medina, K.; Reed, N. Right Mind, Right Ride: An Exploration of Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) for the Treatment of Riding-Related Stress; Transport Research Laboratory: Wokingham, UK, 2015; Available online: https://www.iamroadsmart.com/docs/default-source/research-reports/report_cognitive-behavioural-therapy.pdf?sfvrsn=786ee750_4 (accessed on 12 June 2016).
  21. Papadakaki, M.; Tzamalouka, G.; Gnardellis, C.; Lajunen, T.J.; Chliaoutakis, J. Driving performance while using a mobile phone: A simulationstudy of Greek professional drivers. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2016, 38, 164–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Jewell, N. Statistics for Epidemiology; Chapman and Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hartley, L.R.; El Hassani, J. Stress, violations and accidents. Appl. Erg. 1994, 25, 221–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Westerman, S.J.; Haigney, D. Individual differences in driver stress, error and violation. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2000, 29, 981–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Wickens, C.M.; Toplak, M.; Wiesenthal, D.L. Cognitive failures as predictors of driving errors, lapses, and violations. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2008, 40, 1223–1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Matthews, G.; Desmond, P.A.; Joyner, L.; Carcary, B.; Gilliland, K. A comprehensive questionnaire measure of driver stress and affect. In Traffic and Transport Psychology: Theory and Application; Rothengatter, T., Vaya’s, E.C., Eds.; Elsevier Science Ltd.: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 317–324. [Google Scholar]
  27. Reason, J.; Manstead, A.; Stradling, S.; Baxter, J.; Campbell, K. Errors and violations on the roads: A real distinction? Ergonomics 1990, 33, 1315–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Parker, D.; Stradling, S.G.; Manstead, A. Modifying beliefs and attitudes to exceeding the speed limit: An intervention study based on the theory of planned behaviour. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 26, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Deffenbacher, J.L.; Oetting, E.R.; Lynch, R.S. Development of a Driving Anger Scale. Psychol. Rep. 1994, 74, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Rowland, B.D.; Freeman, J.E.; Davey, J.D.; Wishart, D.E. A profile of taxi drivers’ road safety attitudes and behaviours: Is safety important? In People Our Future; CD ROM; Grzebieta, R., Ed.; Australian College of Road Safety: Canberra, Australia, 2007; pp. 1–10. Available online: https://eprints.qut.edu.au/11660/ (accessed on 2 May 2018).
  31. Wishart, D.; Freeman, J.; Davey, J. Utilising the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire in an organizational fleet setting: Are modifications required? J. Aust. Coll. Road Saf. 2006, 17, 31–38. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/deref/http%3A%2F%2Feprints.qut.edu.au%2F (accessed on 4 July 2017).
  32. Zinzow, H.Z.; Brooks, J.O.; Rosopa, P.J.; Jeffirs, S.; Jenkins, C.; Seeanner, J.; McKeeman, A.; Hodges, L.F. Virtual Reality and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Driving Anxiety and Aggression in Veterans: A Pilot Study. Cogn. Behav. Pract. 2018, 25, 296–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Participant sociodemographic and driving characteristics.
Table 1. Participant sociodemographic and driving characteristics.
Personal CharacteristicsControl GroupIntervention Groupp-Value *
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Age (years)27.8 (9.9)26.2 (7.5)0.651
n (%)n (%)
Gender 0.451
Male19 (63.3)8 (80.0)
Female11 (36.7)2 (20.0)
Driving Years 0.230
<1 year1 (3.3)1 (10.0)
1–5 years17 (56.7)5 (50.0)
6–10 years6 (20.0)0 (0.0)
≥11 years6 (20.0)4 (40.0)
Driving Frequency 0.298
Every day21 (70.0)7 (70.0)
Sometimes weekly3 (10.0)3 (30.0)
Sometimes monthly2 (6.7)0 (0.0)
Less often4 (13.3)0 (0.0)
Not at all0 (0.0)0 (0.0)
Most common means of transport 0.186
Bus6 (20.0)0 (0.0)
Truck0 (0.0)1 (10.0)
Car21 (70.0)8 (80.0)
Motorcycle3 (10.0)1 (10.0)
Bicycle0 (0.0)0 (0.0)
Car accident in the last year 0.731
Yes13 (43.3)5 (50.0)
No17 (56.7)5 (50.0)
* Significance level 0.05.
Table 2. Self-reported driving performance (intervention group).
Table 2. Self-reported driving performance (intervention group).
ScalePreinterventionPostinterventionp-Value
Median (25th–75th)Median (25th–75th)
Aggression5.0 (4.7, 5.3)4.8 (4.3, 5.5)0.594
Dislike of Driving4.9 (3.6, 5.3)4.8 (4.1, 5.3)0.362
Thrill Seeking4.7 (0.3, 5.4)2.9 (1.8, 4.9)0.767
Hazard Monitoring5.8 (5.4, 6.9)6.6 (5.6, 7.0)0.037
Covered Violations1.5 (1.3, 2.2)0.8 (0.7, 1.3)0.049
Unintentional Violations1.0 (0.5, 1.0)0.5 (0.0, 1.0)0.217
Lapses1.1 (0.7, 1.6)0.9 (0.6, 1.1)0.260
Drinking and driving3.2 (2.6, 3.4)3.0 (2.8, 3.6)0.812
Close Following3.2 (3.0, 3.2)3.2 (3.0, 3.4)0.631
Risky Overtaking3.8 (3.4, 3.8)3.6 (3.4, 4.0)0.377
Speeding3.8 (3.8, 4.0)4.2 (3.2, 4.6)0.678
Driving Anger Scale3.2 (3.0, 3.2)2.6 (2.2, 3.2)0.214
Table 3. Self-reported driving performance (control group).
Table 3. Self-reported driving performance (control group).
ScalePreinterventionPostinterventionp-Value
Median (25th–75th)Median (25th–75th)
Aggression5.3 (4.4, 5.8)5.2 (4.3, 5.7)0.599
Dislike of driving3.9 (3.2, 5.1)3.9 (3.3, 4.5)0.681
Thrill Seeking3.3 (2.0, 4.1)3.4 (2.5, 4.3)0.992
Hazard Monitoring6.0 (5.1, 6.4)5.6 (5.3, 6.3)0.319
Covered Violations1.0 (0.9, 1.2)1.0 (0.7, 1.2)0.127
Unintentional Violations0.5 (0.5, 1.0)0.5 (0.5, 1.0)0.719
Lapses0.7 (0.6, 1.1)0.9 (0.7, 1.1)0.181
Table 4. Self-reported driving behavior after the intervention (intervention vs. control group).
Table 4. Self-reported driving behavior after the intervention (intervention vs. control group).
Driving BehaviorGroupp-Value
ControlIntervention
Median (25th–75th)Median (25th–75th)
Aggression5.2 (4.3, 5.7)4.8 (4.3, 5.5)0.802
Dislike of Driving3.9 (3.3, 4.5)4.8 (4.1, 5.3)0.072
Thrill Seeking3.4 (2.5, 4.3)2.9 (1.8, 4.9)0.764
Hazard Monitoring5.6 (5.3, 6.3)6.6 (5.6, 7.0)0.095
Unintentional Violations1.0 (0.7, 1.2)0.8 (0.7, 1.3)0.615
Covered Violations0.5 (0.5, 1.0)0.5 (0.0, 1.0)0.407
Lapses0.9 (0.7, 1.1)0.9 (0.6, 1.1)0.960
Drinking and Driving3.0 (2.6, 3.4)3.0 (2.8, 3.6)0.637
Close Following3.2 (2.8, 3.6)3.2 (3.0, 3.4)1.000
Risky Overtaking3.4 (3.0, 3.6)3.6 (3.4, 4.0)0.089
Speeding3.6 (3.2, 3.8)4.2 (3.2, 4.6)0.161
Driving Anger Scale2.8 (2.4, 3.5)2.6 (2.2, 3.2)0.317

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Papadakaki, M.; Stamou, N.; Bessas, S.; Lioliou, S.; Chliaoutakis, J. Testing the Effectiveness of a Mixed-Method Pilot Intervention in Reducing Risky Driving Due to Aggression and Stress. Safety 2020, 6, 11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/safety6010011

AMA Style

Papadakaki M, Stamou N, Bessas S, Lioliou S, Chliaoutakis J. Testing the Effectiveness of a Mixed-Method Pilot Intervention in Reducing Risky Driving Due to Aggression and Stress. Safety. 2020; 6(1):11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/safety6010011

Chicago/Turabian Style

Papadakaki, Maria, Nikolaos Stamou, Stefanos Bessas, Stavroula Lioliou, and Jooannes Chliaoutakis. 2020. "Testing the Effectiveness of a Mixed-Method Pilot Intervention in Reducing Risky Driving Due to Aggression and Stress" Safety 6, no. 1: 11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/safety6010011

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop