Next Article in Journal
Perception of Velocity during Free-Weight Exercises: Difference between Back Squat and Bench Press
Previous Article in Journal
Positive Effect of Kinesiotape on 1 km Run Performance in University-Level Males: A Cross-Sectional Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Moral Disengagement in Youth Athletes: A Narrative Review

1
Department of Psychology, Educational Sciences and Human Movement, University of Palermo, 90133 Palermo, Italy
2
Faculty of Sport and Physical Education, University of Novi Sad, 21000 Novi Sad, Serbia
3
Institute for Kinesiology Research, Science and Research Centre Koper, 6000 Koper, Slovenia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2022, 7(2), 33; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jfmk7020033
Submission received: 4 March 2022 / Revised: 5 April 2022 / Accepted: 14 April 2022 / Published: 15 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Psychology of Development and Education Applied to Movement 2022)

Abstract

:
The sports environment can be considered as a context characterized by interactions typical of social groups, where children have the chance to learn good values. Positive and negative behaviours in sports, also called prosocial and antisocial behaviours, have been studied according to a moral perspective, as has doping behaviour, taking into consideration the concept of moral disengagement. Moral disengagement in children has been associated with maladaptive behaviours later in life, even though it should disappear with growth. Concerning the sports environment, previous reviews on the topic have extensively illustrated the role of moral variables in sport and their relation to antisocial behaviour and doping, positing some research questions that should be investigated in the future. Starting from these questions, the current narrative review aims to update literature about the effects of moral disengagement on youth athletes. Therefore, new studies about the predictors of moral disengagement are introduced, followed by contributions concerning the relationship between moral disengagement and doping and between moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour. Finally, the review summarizes which research questions have been solved in the last decade and which should be researched further on.

1. Introduction

Sports can be considered as a social context characterized by interactions and reciprocal influence among the actors. These interactions can produce positive consequences for others, such as prosocial behaviour, discipline, and honesty, but also negative actions, like cheating, intimidating, and injuring other participants [1]. Studies concerning children and youth in sports have pointed out the role of moral reasoning as a determinant of prosocial and antisocial behaviour [2]. The degree to which children act prosocially depends on their achievement goal orientation [2,3,4], while aggressive tendencies and antisocial behaviour are predicted by a less mature moral reasoning [5,6] and a high level of moral disengagement.
Moral disengagement is a set of psychological mechanisms used to disengage transgressive behaviour from the self-sanctions that keep behaviour in line with moral standards [2,7]. These mechanisms consist of eight cognitive strategies converting a transgressive behaviour into an acceptable one to keep the behaviour in line with moral standards [8]. The strategies are: moral justification, euphemistic labelling, advantageous comparison, displacement of responsibility, diffusion of responsibility, distortion of consequences, dehumanization, and attribution of blame [9].
The concept of moral disengagement has been identified as an explanation for several disruptive behaviours, such as drug abuse and delinquency [10]. For what concerns children and adolescents, it was associated with aggression and rule-breaking behaviour [11]. Longitudinal studies showed that this tendency usually disappears with growth, but 10% of the population maintains it also in adult life [12].
Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli [9] suggested that moral disengagement is a context-specific phenomenon and should be separately analysed. This is particularly true if we consider that when it comes to youth, moral reasoning—i.e., the criteria that people use to resolve moral conflicts—shows differences between sports and daily life. This phenomenon is well-known as “bracketed morality” [13]. Moreover, certain conditions existing in sports may facilitate moral disengagement [14].
Concerning the sports field, in the last decades moral disengagement has been extensively studied in relation to antisocial and aggressive behaviour [15,16,17] and doping [18,19]. Several reviews have pointed out the mechanisms of moral disengagement [11,20,21,22,23], and the latest contribution in the sports context was in 2011 [24], where the authors reported the need for further research in the role of regulatory emotions such as anticipatory guilt [9]. Therefore, the present descriptive review aims to update the scientific literature analysing the role of moral disengagement in youth athletes’ behaviour, focusing on predictors of moral disengagement in sports and referring to doping and antisocial behaviours as outcomes. The present review considers studies published between 2012 and 2022 with children, adolescents, and young adults as the target group, considered under the label “youth.”

2. Predictors of Moral Disengagement in Sports

Morality is a strong component of sport participation. A number of studies have considered morality as a good predictor of antisocial behaviour [4,25,26,27,28]. Specifically, moral disengagement has been identified as the key predictor of antisocial behaviour. The questionnaire Moral Disengagement in Sport Scale (MDSS) by Boardley and Kavussanu [29] reported a strong positive correlation with self-reported antisocial behaviour, especially in boys. Given the cruciality of the topic for youth development, it also becomes important to point out what the predictors are.
Shields et al. [30] hypothesized some factors that could predict moral disengagement in sports. Several studies reported gender as a significant predictor of moral disengagement, where boys tended to disengage to a bigger extent than girls, also in the sports context [15,17], and the authors confirmed this result. Age is also revealed to be a significant predictor of moral disengagement, with a stronger relationship between moral disengagement and bullying in adolescents than in children, regardless of their involvement in sports activities [31].
Another predictor of moral disengagement, identified by Shields, Funk, and Bredemeier, is moral identity, which refers to the cognition people hold when thinking about their moral character and their desire to be moral persons [32,33]. The authors found that, after gender, moral identity was the strongest predictor of moral disengagement. In other words, the desire to be a moral person negatively predicts the likelihood of morally disengaging from an antisocial action. Furthermore, moral attentiveness, which relates to the importance that individuals give to morality in their experiences [34], was a significant predictor. Finally, contesting orientation, which is the metaphorical framework used to understand the values and meaning of contesting [35], was considered a predictor. The authors found that a contesting orientation focused on “partnership” orientation, where contesting is seen as a mutually beneficial process of enjoyment and growth, negatively predicted moral disengagement. In contrast, a contesting orientation of “war,” which is metaphorically meant as a battle allowing only one winner, positively predicted moral disengagement. Empathy did not predict moral disengagement in sports.
Budziszewski et al. [36] found similar results in a sample of athlete trainers, whose warfare–contesting orientation, partnership–contesting orientation, sports ethic, commitment, and social identity were significantly related to moral disengagement. Specifically, athlete trainers reporting a stronger sports ethic and contesting orientations with lower commitment and social connection to student–athletes were more prone to moral disengagement, allowing athletes to play through injury.
Another study by Jones et al. [37] found that narcissism predicts moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour within the sports context. The authors hypothesized that, since narcissism is negatively related to empathy and positively related to the feeling of entitlement, narcissists should be more likely to disengage morally and to behave antisocially, and their result seems to support this hypothesis. Indeed, typical traits of narcissists are manipulation and antisocial personality [38,39], together with the desire for success [40]. In line with this hypothesis, narcissists may think that moral standards are not valid in the sporting context, transforming antisocial behaviour into a morally acceptable action [41].
Finally, Caz et al. [42] hypothesized that athletic identity could be a predictor of moral disengagement associated with doping. Brewer et al. [43] identified three features of athletic identity, namely social identity, exclusivity, and negative affectivity. Social identity focuses on the extent to which the athletes view themselves as athletes in other people’s eyes [44]. Exclusivity is the degree to which the athlete assumes a self-concept related only to the athletic image. Negative affectivity is the extent to which the athlete worries about poor performance in the game or not being able to fulfill the athletic role. The study of Caz found that athletes’ perception of social identity has a positive effect on moral disengagement related to doping, while exclusivity was negatively related to it. Negative affectivity did not have any relation with moral disengagement. According to the authors, the social identity perceptions of the athletes trigger the tendency towards moral disengagement associated with doping. Moreover, concerning exclusivity, when sports become the unique goal in athletes’ lives and success becomes of secondary importance, the likelihood of using moral disengagement mechanisms concerning doping decreases.

3. Moral Disengagement as a Predictor of Doping Behaviour

The use of performance enhancement drugs to improve athletic performance is a serious concern in sports (see Table 1). Understanding the mechanisms behind doping becomes critical because previous studies have highlighted a worrisome tendency towards doping in adolescent athletes [45,46,47,48,49].
First of all, enhancing performance through illicit substances may have adverse health effects at the cardiovascular, psychiatric, endocrine, neurologic, hepatic, renal, or musculoskeletal levels, but these negative consequences are often underestimated [58]. Moreover, apart from the health risks, doping is a moral issue [59,60]. Framing doping as a moral problem allows one to examine the influence of morality on the likelihood of engaging in doping behaviour [19]. Barkoukis and Elbe [61] analysed both the moral and ethical implications of doping, concluding that morality has been mainly framed in a socio-cognitive approach. In contrast, the ethical aspect has been considered to a less extent. Two qualitative studies by Boardley et al. [56,57] reported that athletes who previously had used performance enhancement drugs tend to use from six to seven moral disengagement mechanisms. Another study of Drewery and Wilson [55] specifically found that displacement of responsibility and advantageous comparison significantly predict attitudes towards doping, especially in athletes with obsessive passion.
A review of Kavussanu [62] deeply examined the process of moral disengagement occurring in doping in the sports field, taking into account the theory of planned behaviour [63,64], the self-determination theory (SDT) [65], and the social cognitive theory [7].
According to the theory of planned behaviour, the best predictor of action is not the attitude towards the targeted behaviour, but the intention of engaging in it [64]. Lucidi, Grano, Leone, Lombardo and Pesce [63] employed this framework to detect doping by asking the intention to use illegal substances to improve sports performance and physical appearance, together with a questionnaire of moral disengagement related to doping [66]. After three months, the researchers asked the sample to indicate which doping substances they used during the past three months to improve their athletic performance and/or physical appearance. The results showed a positive prediction of moral disengagement towards the intention to dope and the later doping engagement, and higher scores in moral disengagement were related to more positive attitudes towards doping. Moreover, participants with a high level of moral disengagement tended to consider important the approval of others and were less likely to withdraw from doping if there was the chance. The real limitation of the study concerned the participating sample, which consisted of athletes who did not regularly compete in sports.
In recent times, the intention to engage in doping was related to moral disengagement, also considering the motivational climate. One of the possible understandings of motivational climate in sports is the distinction between a task-involving climate, where the athletes are provided with a clear rationale for tasks and non-controlling competence feedback, and ego-involving climate, where competence feedback is provided and the athletes define success as outperforming others and winning [67]. A study by Guo, Liang, Baker and Mao [19] examined the association among perceived motivational climate (i.e., task-involving and ego-involving), moral variables (i.e., moral disengagement and sportspersonship), and attitudes towards doping with doping intention. The results showed that the task-involving motivational climate was negatively associated with doping intention through sportspersonship, while the ego-involving climate was positively related to doping intention through moral disengagement. Therefore, athletes not receiving feedback on their competencies reported higher levels of sportspersonship and a lower likelihood to engage in doping, while athletes receiving feedback on their competencies were more likely to morally disengage from their actions and reported higher intention to engage in doping behaviour.
Moral disengagement has also been related to the self-determination theory (SDT) [65], particularly controlled motivation, which occurs when individuals join sports for extrinsic rewards associated with sports participation. Following this perspective, doping is seen as the mean of allowing the athletes to achieve their goals [62]. A study by Hodge, Hargreaves, Gerrard and Lonsdale [18] reported a correlation between controlled motivation and moral disengagement, meaning that athletes competing in sports for extrinsic reasons are at risk of moral disengagement. However, the correlation between moral disengagement and controlled motivation was low, suggesting that SDT might not be the best framework for explaining the engagement in doping.
Another study focused on the influence of coaching style on athletes’ moral disengagement [54]. Coaches usually play a fundamental role in shaping athletic performance and relative psychological experience [68]. Coaching style, according to the SDT framework, can be divided into autonomy-supportive and controlling styles [69]. A coach who follows the autonomy-supportive style gives athletes the opportunity to contribute to decision-making, is attentive to their views and feelings, and allows them to choose appropriate tactics or techniques. Conversely, a controlling coaching style is characterized by an authoritarian and coercive manner, guilt induction strategies, manipulation, threats, and a lack of recognition of the athletes’ perspectives and feelings. The study of Chen, Wang, Wang and Huang [54] found a relationship between the controlling style of coaching and attitudes towards doping and the mediation of moral disengagement. This means that a controlling coaching style predicts the probability of moral disengagement, which, in turn, produces a positive attitude towards doping.
The social-cognitive theory has been used as a framework for explaining moral disengagement in sports through the concept of anticipated guilt [7]. After acting out a transgressive behaviour, guilt is an adaptive emotion that prevents people from repeating the same action that moral disengagement can reduce, thus facilitating doping [62].
A study of Ring and Hurst [52] examined the effects of moral disengagement on the likelihood of doping, with a mediation of guilt and a moderation of moral traits. In this study, the anticipated guilt, that is, the emotion preventing feelings of guilt, was related to six moral scenarios corresponding to the six moral disengagement mechanisms in sports. The findings indicated that the likelihood of doping was higher in all the six mechanisms compared to the neutral scenario. Moreover, anticipated guilt mediated the relationships between five mechanisms (excepted from euphemistic labeling) and doping likelihood. Finally, the effect of moral disengagement on the likelihood of doping was moderated by moral agency, moral perfectionism, and moral values. These results indicated that moral disengagement increases the likelihood of engaging in doping by decreasing affective self-sanction for doping [52]. Similar results were also reported by Kavussanu and Ring [53], Harris et al. [50], Ring and Kavussanu [48], and Stanger and Backhouse [51], where the anticipated guilt about potential doping mediated the relationship between moral disengagement and the likelihood of doping.

4. Moral Disengagement as a Predictor of Antisocial Behaviour

In past decades, a high number of studies have connected moral disengagement to antisocial behaviour in the practice of sports (see Table 2). Antisocial behaviour in sports is defined as a negative, intentional, motivated social action directed to opponents and/or teammates, such as provoking, pushing, and being aggressive or violent [70]. Traditionally, moral disengagement has been considered a predictor of antisocial behaviour, but Kavussanu and Stanger [71] hypothesized that it could be equally plausible to consider it as an outcome. As maintained by the authors, this relation can be thought of as bidirectional.
Stanger et al. [76] investigated the effect of moral disengagement mechanisms on emotions and antisocial behaviour towards opponents by first analysing the mediating role of anticipated guilt and then investigating the role of attribution of blame, which is a mechanism of moral disengagement. Specifically, in the second study, the authors administered a picture-viewing task, where athletes are required to rate images depicting antisocial behaviour situations (e.g., a rugby player who had “fouled” and had hurt another player). Afterward, participants in the experimental group read that the victim’s injurious behaviour was deliberate and that he had mocked the perpetrator. In this way, the participants were forced to believe that the perpetrators were retaliating after being “fouled” by the victim. In the control group, participants did not receive any instruction. The findings from the first study reported a positive correlation between moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour, with a partial mediation of anticipated guilt. In other words, athletes were more likely to act antisocially when they did not experience anticipated guilt. From the second study, the authors reported that the attribution-of-blame mechanism decreased the negative emotional reaction to antisocial behaviour, increasing the likelihood of antisocial behaviour.
In a longitudinal perspective, a recent study by Boardley et al. [73] examined the relationship between moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour in sports. The authors found that earlier antisocial behaviour was a strong positive predictor of later antisocial behaviour and that earlier moral development was related to later moral development. Moreover, moral development predicted longitudinal changes in antisocial behaviour towards opponents.
Recent studies also considered the role of the group in the investigation of moral disengagement. Danioni et al. [72], referring to a recent study of Gini, Pozzoli and Bussey [31], posited that interpersonal and social factors should be considered when investigating moral disengagement. According to Bandura [79], collective moral disengagement arises from interactive, coordinative, and synergistic group dynamics when justifying negative actions within a significant social group and can contribute to the development of group norms and of behaviours, including the same mechanisms as those of individual moral disengagement. Therefore, collective moral disengagement might be a significant construct for sports teams, particularly for adolescents. In Danioni’s study, collective moral disengagement significantly predicted antisocial behaviour towards teammates and opponents. The most interesting finding was that a performance-oriented climate moderated this relationship. In other words, when the climate was performance-oriented, collective moral disengagement was a predictor of antisocial behaviour.
Other studies have related moral disengagement to the team motivational climate in antisocial behaviours [16,75,80]. As mentioned before, motivational climate can be referred to goal orientations (i.e., ego-orientation vs. task-orientation) that interact with social climate factors [81]. Concerning social climate factors, Ames [82] distinguished between mastery (task) and performance (ego) perceptions of motivational climates. When students are involved in a decision-making task whose success depends on the efforts of all the members, it is more likely that the students will perceive their classroom as mastery-oriented [83]. Conversely, students will perceive the climate as performance-oriented when they are evaluated by normative standards and when the interpersonal comparison is cued. Ames and Archer [83] argued that this latter motivational climate increases the probability of observing maladaptive motivational responses in students. Concerning moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour, Stanger, Backhouse, Jennings and McKenna [16] found that mastery climate was negatively associated with prosocial behaviour towards teammates both directly and indirectly via social support and that the prosocial opponent behaviour was indirectly and positively associated via social support and perspective-taking. Moreover, mastery climate was negatively associated with antisocial behaviours towards teammates and opponents indirectly via social support, perspective-taking, and moral disengagement. Finally, concerning antisocial behaviours, moral disengagement directly and indirectly moderated the relationship between antisocial behaviours towards both teammates and opponents. These findings suggest that in adopting a mastery climate and avoiding overemphasis on a performance climate, the likelihood of adopting antisocial behaviour notably decreases.
Bruner et al. [78] examined moral disengagement in a social identity theory (SIT) perspective [84], focusing on two out of the three key dimensions of social identity: ingroup ties and ingroup affect. “Ingroup ties” refers to the perception of similarity, bonding, and belongingness with other group members, while “ingroup affect” relates to the positive feeling associated with group membership [85]. In the study of Bruner, ingroup ties and ingroup affect were related to moral disengagement and to prosocial and antisocial behaviour. The results showed that overall ingroup affect has a positive effect on prosocial behaviour towards teammates and negative effects on antisocial behaviour toward teammates and opponents. Furthermore, all effects are mediated by moral disengagement. Overall ingroup ties have a positive effect on prosocial behaviour towards teammates and antisocial behaviour towards teammates and opponents; no effects of ingroup ties on the three types of behaviour are mediated by moral disengagement. Finally, no dimension of social identity influenced prosocial behaviour towards opponents.
Boardley and Kavussanu [24] maintained that moral disengagement mechanisms should be related to specific antisocial behaviours, such as cheating and aggression. Concerning cheating, a study of Šukys [77] specifically investigated the relationship between moral disengagement and cheating behaviour by analysing the justification of deception. The justification was related to the manipulation of the results of the sports contest, to the manipulation of the rules of the sport contest, and to provocative behaviour towards opponents. Moreover, the study also considered the respondents’ years of experience. The results showed that moral disengagement is a predictor of the general justification of cheating in sports. Deceptive actions associated with athletes’ manipulation of the rules of the sports contest were justified to a larger extent by athletes with more experience in sports and by younger athletes. Regarding aggression, youth playing contact sports tended to report higher levels of moral disengagement than athletes in non-contact sports and a higher perception of legitimacy of aggressive behaviour in competition [74]. The study also confirmed the existence of age differences in moral disengagement, where younger athletes (17–18 years) tended to report higher levels than older athletes.

5. Conclusions

Moral disengagement theory appeared almost thirty years ago, but its applications to daily life and sports remain still valid. As regards sports, moral disengagement has been identified as one of the main determinants of negative behaviours such as doping and of antisocial behaviours. Starting from the review of Boardley and Kavussanu [24], the main goal of the present descriptive review was to provide an update from the last ten years about the investigation of moral disengagement in youth athletes.
Some research questions that have been raised in previous studies, such as cheating and aggression behaviour and the role of regulatory emotions (anticipatory guilt), were answered through various studies. Specifically, in the first case, several studies highlighted the mediational role of anticipatory guilt in the relation between moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour/doping intention. Concerning the predictors of moral disengagement in sports, few studies were found in the last decade. Considering doping behaviour, the research mainly focused on self-regulatory mechanisms such as anticipated guilt and on motivational climate, athletic identity, moral identity, moral traits, and coaching style. Antisocial behaviour research focused on the role of motivational climate, locus of control, negative emotions, and social identity.
Despite the progress reached in these years, some research questions still remained unsolved, such as the influence of fear of failure on the relationship between moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour [30] or the use of a research design other than the cross-sectional or longitudinal that could offer a different perspective on these relationships [24]. Furthermore, it would be useful to analyse potential age differences in anticipatory guilt.
Future research should focus on these questions and provide new insights for the prevention of negative behaviours in the field of sports. Since sports activity has a strong impact on children and adolescents’ growth, being aware of this process may help reduce antisocial and doping behaviours during growth and adulthood in favour of a “clean sports” message.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.G., G.L.; methodology, A.G., A.B., writing—original draft preparation, A.G., I.M., S.P.; writing—review and editing, A.B., G.L., supervision, G.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kavussanu, M. Moral behaviour in sport: A critical review of the literature. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2008, 1, 124–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Kavussanu, M.; Al-Yaaribi, A. Prosocial and antisocial behaviour in sport. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2021, 19, 179–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Kavussanu, M.; Stanger, N.; Boardley, I.D. The Prosocial and Antisocial Behaviour in Sport Scale: Further evidence for construct validity and reliability. J. Sports Sci. 2013, 31, 1208–1221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Shields, D.L.; Funk, C.D.; Bredemeier, B.L. Relationships among moral and contesting variables and prosocial and antisocial behavior in sport. J. Moral Educ. 2018, 47, 17–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bredemeier, B.J.; Shields, D.L.; Weiss, M.R.; Cooper, B.A. The relationship of sport involvement with children’s moral reasoning and aggression tendencies. J. Sport Psychol. 1986, 8, 304–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bredemeier, B.J.; Weiss, M.R.; Shields, D.L.; Cooper, B.A. The relationship between children’s legitimacy judgments and their moral reasoning, aggression tendencies, and sport involvement. Sociol. Sport J. 1987, 4, 48–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bandura, A. Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In Handbook of Moral Behavior and Development; Kurtines, W.M., Gewirtz, J., Lamb, J.L., Eds.; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 1991; pp. 1–45. [Google Scholar]
  8. Gómez Tabares, A.S.; Landinez-Martínez, D.A. Moral disengagement mechanisms and its relationship with aggression and bullying behaviour among school children and youth at psychosocial risk. Emot. Behav. Diffic. 2021, 26, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bandura, A.; Barbaranelli, C.; Caprara, G.V.; Pastorelli, C. Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 71, 364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Passini, S. The delinquency–drug relationship: The influence of social reputation and moral disengagement. Addict. Behav. 2012, 37, 577–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gini, G.; Pozzoli, T.; Hymel, S. Moral disengagement among children and youth: A meta-analytic review of links to aggressive behavior. Aggress. Behav. 2014, 40, 56–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Pepler, D.; Jiang, D.; Craig, W.; Connolly, J. Developmental trajectories of bullying and associated factors. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 325–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Shields, D.L.L.; Bredemeier, B.J.L. Character Development and Physical Activity; Human Kinetics Publishers: Champaign, IL, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kavussanu, M.; Boardley, I.D.; Sagar, S.S.; Ring, C. Bracketed morality revisited: How do athletes behave in two contexts? J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2013, 35, 449–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Tsai, J.-J.; Wang, C.-H.; Lo, H.-J. Locus of control, moral disengagement in sport, and rule transgression of athletes. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2014, 42, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Stanger, N.; Backhouse, S.H.; Jennings, A.; McKenna, J. Linking motivational climate with moral behavior in youth sport: The role of social support, perspective taking, and moral disengagement. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2018, 7, 392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Šukys, S.; Jansonienė, A.J. Relationship between athletes’ values and moral disengagement in sport, and differences across gender, level and years of involvement. Balt. J. Sport Health Sci. 2012, 1, 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hodge, K.; Hargreaves, E.A.; Gerrard, D.; Lonsdale, C. Psychological mechanisms underlying doping attitudes in sport: Motivation and moral disengagement. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2013, 35, 419–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Guo, L.; Liang, W.; Baker, J.S.; Mao, Z.-X. Perceived motivational climates and doping intention in adolescent athletes: The mediating role of moral disengagement and sportspersonship. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Newman, A.; Le, H.; North-Samardzic, A.; Cohen, M. Moral disengagement at work: A review and research agenda. J. Bus. Ethics 2020, 167, 535–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hartmann, T.; Krakowiak, K.M.; Tsay-Vogel, M. How violent video games communicate violence: A literature review and content analysis of moral disengagement factors. Commun. Monogr. 2014, 81, 310–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Moore, C. Moral disengagement. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015, 6, 199–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Killer, B.; Bussey, K.; Hawes, D.J.; Hunt, C. A meta-analysis of the relationship between moral disengagement and bullying roles in youth. Aggress. Behav. 2019, 45, 450–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Boardley, I.D.; Kavussanu, M. Moral disengagement in sport. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2011, 4, 93–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Wyatt, J.M.; Carlo, G. What Will My Parents Think? Relations Amongadolescents’ Expected Parental Reactions, Prosocial Moral Reasoning, and Prosocial and Antisocial Behaviors. J. Adolesc. Res. 2002, 17, 646–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Raine, A.; Yang, Y. Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2006, 1, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Li, C.X.; Koh, K.T.; Wang, C.K.J.; Chian, L.K. Sports Participation and Moral Development Outcomes: Examination of Validity and Reliability of the Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior in Sport Scale. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 2015, 10, 505–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Mallia, L.; Lucidi, F.; Zelli, A.; Chirico, A.; Hagger, M.S. Predicting moral attitudes and antisocial behavior in young team sport athletes: A self-determination theory perspective. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2019, 49, 249–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Boardley, I.D.; Kavussanu, M. Development and validation of the moral disengagement in sport scale. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2007, 29, 608–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Shields, D.L.; Funk, C.D.; Bredemeier, B.L. Predictors of moral disengagement in sport. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2015, 37, 646–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gini, G.; Pozzoli, T.; Bussey, K. The role of individual and collective moral disengagement in peer aggression and bystanding: A multilevel analysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2015, 43, 441–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Aquino, K.; Freeman, D.; Reed II, A.; Lim, V.K.; Felps, W. Testing a social-cognitive model of moral behavior: The interactive influence of situations and moral identity centrality. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 97, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Aquino, K.; Reed, A., II. The self-importance of moral identity. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 83, 1423–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Reynolds, S.J. Moral attentiveness: Who pays attention to the moral aspects of life? J. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 93, 1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Shields, D.; Bredemeier, B. Contest, competition, and metaphor. J. Philos. Sport 2011, 38, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Budziszewski, R.; Graupensperger, S.A.; Vierimaa, M. Exploring Predictors of Moral Disengagement in Collegiate Athletic Trainers. J. Athl. Train. 2020, 55, 96–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Jones, B.D.; Woodman, T.; Barlow, M.; Roberts, R. The darker side of personality: Narcissism predicts moral disengagement and antisocial behavior in sport. Sport Psychol. 2017, 31, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Morf, C.C.; Rhodewalt, F. Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychol. Inq. 2001, 12, 177–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Bursten, B. What if antisocial personality is an illness? J. Am. Acad. Psychiatry Law Online 1982, 10, 97–102. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ong, C.W.; Roberts, R.; Arthur, C.A.; Woodman, T.; Akehurst, S. The leader ship is sinking: A temporal investigation of narcissistic leadership. J. Personal. 2016, 84, 237–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Brunell, A.B.; Gentry, W.A.; Campbell, W.K.; Hoffman, B.J.; Kuhnert, K.W.; DeMarree, K.G. Leader emergence: The case of the narcissistic leader. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 34, 1663–1676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Caz, C.; Kayhan, R.F.; Bardakci, S.; Hasaan, A. Athletic Identity and Moral Disengagement in Doping. S. Afr. J. Res. Sport Phys. Educ. Recreat. 2021, 43, 13–28. [Google Scholar]
  43. Brewer, B.W.; Van Raalte, J.L.; Linder, D.E. Athletic identity: Hercules’ muscles or Achilles heel? Int. J. Sport Psychol. 1993, 24, 237–254. [Google Scholar]
  44. Burns, G.N.; Jasinski, D.; Dunn, S.C.; Fletcher, D. Athlete identity and athlete satisfaction: The nonconformity of exclusivity. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2012, 52, 280–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ntoumanis, N.; Ng, J.Y.; Barkoukis, V.; Backhouse, S. Personal and psychosocial predictors of doping use in physical activity settings: A meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2014, 44, 1603–1624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Kavussanu, M.; Hatzigeorgiadis, A.; Elbe, A.-M.; Ring, C. The moral disengagement in doping scale. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2016, 24, 188–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Corrion, K.; Scoffier-Mériaux, S.; Longueville, F. Self-regulatory mechanisms of doping intentions in elite athletes: The role of self-determined motivation in sport. J. Sports Med. Doping Stud. 2017, 7, 1000197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Ring, C.; Kavussanu, M. The role of self-regulatory efficacy, moral disengagement and guilt on doping likelihood: A social cognitive theory perspective. J. Sports Sci. 2018, 36, 578–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Lucidi, F.; Zelli, A.; Mallia, L. The contribution of moral disengagement to adolescents’ use of doping substances. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2013, 44, 493–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Harris, T.; Smith, A.; Myers, N. Doping Moral Disengagement Predicts Anticipated Guilt and Doping Consideration Within the Context of Social Norms. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2021, 43, S68–S69. [Google Scholar]
  51. Stanger, N.; Backhouse, S.H. A Multistudy Cross-Sectional and Experimental Examination Into the Interactive Effects of Moral Identity and Moral Disengagement on Doping. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2020, 42, 185–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ring, C.; Hurst, P. The effects of moral disengagement mechanisms on doping likelihood are mediated by guilt and moderated by moral traits. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2019, 40, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kavussanu, M.; Ring, C. Moral identity predicts doping likelihood via moral disengagement and anticipated guilt. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2017, 39, 293–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Chen, Z.S.; Wang, D.; Wang, K.; Huang, T. Coaching style and attitudes toward doping in Chinese athletes: The mediating role of moral disengagement. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 2017, 12, 312–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Drewery, D.; Wilson, W. The potential dark side of passion in collegiate sport: Passion as a link between moral disengagement and attitudes towards PEDs. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2016, 38, S182–S183. [Google Scholar]
  56. Boardley, I.D.; Grix, J.; Harkin, J. Doping in team and individual sports: A qualitative investigation of moral disengagement and associated processes. Qual. Res. Sport Exerc. 2015, 7, 698–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Boardley, I.D.; Grix, J.; Dewar, A.J. Moral disengagement and associated processes in performance-enhancing drug use: A national qualitative investigation. J. Sports Sci. 2014, 32, 836–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Pope, H.G., Jr.; Wood, R.I.; Rogol, A.; Nyberg, F.; Bowers, L.; Bhasin, S. Adverse health consequences of performance-enhancing drugs: An Endocrine Society scientific statement. Endocr. Rev. 2014, 35, 341–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Negro, M.; Marzullo, N.; Caso, F.; Calanni, L.; D’Antona, G. Opinion paper: Scientific, philosophical and legal consideration of doping in sports. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2018, 118, 729–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Uyar, Y.; Gentile, A.; Uyar, H.; Erdeveciler, Ö.; Sunay, H.; Mîndrescu, V.; Mujkic, D.; Bianco, A. Competition, Gender Equality, and Doping in Sports in the Red Queen Effect Perspective. Sustainability 2022, 14, 2490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Barkoukis, V.; Elbe, A.-M. Moral behaviour and doping. In The Unknown in Sport, Exercise and Performance Psychology: Research Questions to Move the Field Forward; Edson, F., Basevitch, I., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  62. Kavussanu, M. Moral disengagement and doping. In The Psychology of Doping in Sport; Routledge: London, UK, 2015; pp. 177–190. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lucidi, F.; Grano, C.; Leone, L.; Lombardo, C.; Pesce, C. Determinants of the intention to use doping substances: An empirical contribution in a sample of Italian adolescents. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2004, 35, 133–148. [Google Scholar]
  64. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Overview of self-determination theory: An organismic dialectical perspective. Handb. Self-Determ. Res. 2002, 2, 3–33. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lucidi, F.; Zelli, A.; Mallia, L.; Grano, C.; Russo, P.M.; Violani, C. The social-cognitive mechanisms regulating adolescents’ use of doping substances. J. Sports Sci. 2008, 26, 447–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Allen, J.; Taylor, J.; Dimeo, P.; Dixon, S.; Robinson, L. Predicting elite Scottish athletes’ attitudes towards doping: Examining the contribution of achievement goals and motivational climate. J. Sports Sci. 2015, 33, 899–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Price, M.S.; Weiss, M.R. Relationships among coach burnout, coach behaviors, and athletes’ psychological responses. Sport Psychol. 2000, 14, 391–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Bartholomew, K.J.; Ntoumanis, N.; Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. The controlling interpersonal style in a coaching context: Development and initial validation of a psychometric scale. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2010, 32, 193–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Kavussanu, M.; Boardley, I.D. The prosocial and antisocial behavior in sport scale. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2009, 31, 97–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Kavussanu, M.; Stanger, N. Moral behavior in sport. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 16, 185–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Danioni, F.; Kavussanu, M.; Regalia, C.; Barni, D. “My teammates think it is alright to fight to protect friends”: Collective moral disengagement in team sports. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2021, 19, 598–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Boardley, I.D.; Matosic, D.; Bruner, M.W. A longitudinal examination of the relations between moral disengagement and antisocial behavior in sport. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2020, 42, 123–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Güvendi, B.; Işım Türksoy, A. Moral disengagement and aggression in fight sport athletes. J. Educ. Train. Stud. 2019, 7, 118–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Hodge, K.; Gucciardi, D.F. Antisocial and prosocial behavior in sport: The role of motivational climate, basic psychological needs, and moral disengagement. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2015, 37, 257–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Stanger, N.; Kavussanu, M.; Boardley, I.D.; Ring, C. The influence of moral disengagement and negative emotion on antisocial sport behavior. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2013, 2, 117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Šukys, S. Athletes’ justification of cheating in sport: Relationship with moral disengagement in sport and personal factors. Balt. J. Sport Health Sci. 2013, 3, 70–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Bruner, M.W.; Boardley, I.D.; Cote, J. Moral disengagement in sport and beyond: The gym, the pitch, and the classroom-Examining the role of social identity and moral disengagement on prosocial and antisocial behavior in youth sport. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2012, 34, S27. [Google Scholar]
  79. White, J.; Bandura, A.; Bero, L.A. Moral Disengagement in the Corporate World. Account. Res. 2009, 16, 41–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Hodge, K.; Lonsdale, C. Prosocial and antisocial behavior in sport: The role of coaching style, autonomous vs. controlled motivation, and moral disengagement. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2011, 33, 527–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Ntoumanis, N.; Biddle, S.J.H. A review of motivational climate in physical activity. J. Sports Sci. 1999, 17, 643–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Ames, C. Achievement goals, motivational climate, and motivational processes. In Motivation in Sport and Exercise; Roberts, G.C., Ed.; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 1992; pp. 161–176. [Google Scholar]
  83. Ames, C.; Archer, J. Achievement goals in the classroom: Students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. J. Educ. Psychol. 1988, 80, 260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Tajfel, H.; Turner, J.C.; Austin, W.G.; Worchel, S. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Organizational Identity: A Reader; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1979; pp. 56–63. [Google Scholar]
  85. Cameron, J.E. A Three-Factor Model of Social Identity. Self Identity 2004, 3, 239–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Studies on moral disengagement as predictor of doping behaviour published between 2012 and 2022.
Table 1. Studies on moral disengagement as predictor of doping behaviour published between 2012 and 2022.
AuthorsYearVariablesResults
Harris, Smith and Myers [50]2021Moral disengagement
Anticipated Guilt
Consideration of doping
Anticipated guilt mediates the relation between moral disengagement and consideration of doping.
Doping moral disengagement is a dominant predictor of doping-related cognitions, even when controlling for norms characterising the sport context.
Guo, Liang, Baker and Mao [19]2021Moral disengagement
Perceived Motivational Climate
Doping intention
Moral disengagement is positively associated with doping intention with a large effect size. Sportspersonship prevents doping intention. A task-involving motivational climate had a significant effect on doping intention.
An ego-involving motivational climate was also found to be significantly associated with doping intention and wasfully mediated by moral disengagement.
Caz, Kayhan, Bardakci and Hasaan [42]2021Moral disengagement
Athletic identity
Doping
Athletes’ perception of social identity has a positive effect on doping moral disengagement, while the exclusivity was negatively related to it. Negative affectivity had no relation with moral disengagement.
Stanger and Backhouse [51]2020Moral disengagement
Moral identity Doping
Anticipated guilt
Dispositional moral identity is negatively associated with doping intention. Moral disengagement was positively associated with doping intention. Moreover, moral disengagement increases doping likelihood via anticipated guilt. Dispositional moral identity and inducing moral identity are linked with lower doping likelihood and attenuate the relationship between moral disengagement and doping likelihood.
When the opportunities to morally disengage are amplified, the preventive effect of moral identity disappears.
Ring and Hurst [52]2019Moral disengagement mechanisms
Doping likelihood
Anticipated Guilt
Moral traits
Doping likelihood is higher in all the six mechanisms compared to the neutral scenario. Moreover, anticipated guilt mediates the relationships between five mechanisms (excepted from euphemistic labeling) and doping likelihood. Finally, the effect of moral disengagement on doping likelihood is moderated by moral agency, moral perfectionism, and moral values.
Ring and Kavussanu [48]2018Self-regulatory efficacy
Moral disengagement
Guilt
Doping likelihood
Doping self-regulatory efficacy is associated with doping likelihood both directly and indirectly through doping moral disengagement. Moral disengagement also contributes directly to higher doping likelihood and lower anticipated guilt about doping, which is associated with higher doping likelihood.
Kavussanu and Ring [53]2017Moral identity
Doping likelihood
Moral disengagement
Anticipated guilt
Moral identity predicts doping likelihood indirectly via moral disengagement and anticipated guilt.
Anticipated guilt about potential doping mediates the relationship between moral disengagement and doping likelihood.
Chen, Wang, Wang and Huang [54]2017Coaching style
Attitude towards doping
Moral disengagement
Controlling coaching style is positively associated with attitudes towards doping among Chinese athletes. The relationship is mediated by moral disengagement. An autonomy-supportive coaching style is not associated with attitudes towards doping.
Drewery and Wilson [55]2016Moral disengagement
Passion
Attitudes towards doping
Non-responsibility and advantageous comparison are significant predictors of attitudes towards doping. Obsessive passion mediates the influence of these factors on attitudes towards doping, while harmonious passion does not relate with them.
Boardley, Grix and Harkin [56]2014Moral disengagement mechanisms
Doping behaviour
In 12 interviews, seven out of eight moral disengagement mechanisms were employed from athletes who had used performance enhancement drugs within the previous two years.
Boardley, Grix and Dewar [57]2014Moral disengagement mechanisms
Doping behaviour
In 45 interviews, six out of eight moral disengagement mechanisms were employed by bodybuilders who had used performance enhancement drugs in the previous three months.
Lucidi, Zelli and Mallia [49]2013Moral disengagement
Positive attitudes
Self-regulatory efficacy to resist social pressure for doping
Doping
Moral disengagement influences adolescents’ doping intention and doping use. From a longitudinal point of view, the more adolescents considered doping behaviour as acceptable, the more they showed positive attitudes towards it. Finally, adolescents with higher levels of doping moral disengagement expressed less personal self-regulatory efficacy.
Hodge, Hargreaves, Gerrard and Lonsdale [18]2013Doping Attitudes
Motivation
Moral disengagement
Moral disengagement strongly predicts positive attitudes towards doping, which, in turn, is a strong predictor of doping susceptibility.
Table 2. Studies on moral disengagement as predictor of antisocial behaviour published between 2012 and 2022.
Table 2. Studies on moral disengagement as predictor of antisocial behaviour published between 2012 and 2022.
AuthorsYearVariablesResults
Danioni, Kavussanu, Regalia and Barni [72]2021Collective moral disengagement
Motivational climate
Antisocial behaviour
Collective moral disengagement significantly predicts antisocial behaviour towards teammates and opponents, with the moderation of a performance-oriented climate.
Boardley, Matosic and Bruner [73]2020Moral disengagement
Antisocial behaviour
Earlier antisocial behaviour was a strong positive predictor of later antisocial behaviour, and earlier moral development was related to later moral development. Moreover, moral development predicted longitudinal changes in antisocial behaviour towards opponents.
Güvendi and Işım Türksoy [74]2019Moral disengagement
Aggression
Fight sports
Youth playing contact sports tended to report higher levels of moral disengagement than athletes in non-contact sports and a higher perception of legitimacy of aggressive behaviour in competition. Younger athletes (17–18 years) tended to report higher levels of moral disengagement than older athletes.
Jones, Woodman, Barlow and Roberts [37]2017Narcissism
Moral disengagement
Motivational climate
Social desirability
Sex
Sport type
Narcissism predicts antisocial behaviour via moral disengagement that remained significant when controlling for motivational climate, social desirability, sex, and type of sport.
Hodge and Gucciardi [75]2015Antisocial behaviour
Prosocial behaviour
Motivational climate
Basic psychological needs
Moral disengagement
Coach and teammate autonomy-supportive climates have significant direct relations with need satisfaction and prosocial behaviours. Coach and teammate controlling climates significantly relate with antisocial behaviours. Need satisfaction is both directly and indirectly linked with both prosocial and antisocial behaviours. Moral disengagement is directly and indirectly related with antisocial behaviours.
Tsai, Wang and Lo [15]2014Locus of control
Moral disengagement
Rule transgression
An external locus of control is associated with higher levels of moral disengagement compared to the internal locus of control.
Moral disengagement is highly correlated with rule transgression among athletes.
Stanger, Kavussanu, Boardley and Ring [76]2013Moral disengagement
Negative emotion
Antisocial Behaviour
The relationship between moral disengagement and antisocial behaviour is partially mediated by anticipated guilt.
Attribution of blame reduces negative emotional reactions to antisocial behaviour and increases reported likelihood to act antisocially; this relation is mediated by anticipated guilt.
Šukys [77]2013Cheating
Moral disengagement
Personal Factors
Moral disengagement is a predictor of the general justification of cheating in sports. Deceptive actions associated with athletes’ manipulation of the rules of the sport contest were more justified by athletes with more experience in sports. Deceptive actions associated with athletes’ manipulation of the results of the sport contest were justified to a greater extent by younger athletes.
Bruner, Boardley and Cote [78]2012Social Identity (ingroup ties, ingroup affect)
Moral disengagement
Antisocial behaviour
Prosocial behaviour
Overall ingroup affect has a positive effect on prosocial behaviour towards teammates and negative effects on antisocial behaviour towards teammates and opponents; all effects are mediated by moral disengagement.
Overall ingroup ties have a positive effect on prosocial behaviour towards teammates and antisocial behaviour towards teammates and opponents; no effects of ingroup ties on the three types of behaviour are mediated by moral disengagement. Finally, neither dimension of social identity influenced prosocial behaviour towards opponents.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gentile, A.; Milovanovic, I.; Pišot, S.; Bianco, A.; Lavanco, G. Moral Disengagement in Youth Athletes: A Narrative Review. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2022, 7, 33. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jfmk7020033

AMA Style

Gentile A, Milovanovic I, Pišot S, Bianco A, Lavanco G. Moral Disengagement in Youth Athletes: A Narrative Review. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology. 2022; 7(2):33. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jfmk7020033

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gentile, Ambra, Ivana Milovanovic, Saša Pišot, Antonino Bianco, and Gioacchino Lavanco. 2022. "Moral Disengagement in Youth Athletes: A Narrative Review" Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology 7, no. 2: 33. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jfmk7020033

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop