Next Article in Journal
Clock-Modulating Activities of the Anti-Arrhythmic Drug Moricizine
Next Article in Special Issue
No Effect of Chronotype on Sleepiness, Alertness, and Sustained Attention during a Single Night Shift
Previous Article in Journal
Cellular Effects of Rhynchophylline and Relevance to Sleep Regulation
Previous Article in Special Issue
Trait Interindividual Differences in the Magnitude of Subjective Sleepiness from Sleep Inertia
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Concordance of Chronotype Categorisations Based on Dim Light Melatonin Onset, the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire, and the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire

Appleton Institute for Behavioural Science, Central Queensland University, Goodwood, SA 5034, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 29 April 2021 / Revised: 1 June 2021 / Accepted: 8 June 2021 / Published: 17 June 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Shift-Work and the Individual)

Abstract

:
Chronotype reflects circadian timing and can be determined from biological markers (e.g., dim light melatonin onset; DLMO), or questionnaires (e.g., Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire; MEQ, or Munich Chronotype Questionnaire; MCTQ). The study’s aim was to quantify concordance between chronotype categorisations based on these measures. A total of 72 (36f) young, healthy adults completed the MEQ and MCTQ and provided saliva samples hourly in dim light during the evening in a laboratory. The corrected midpoint of sleep on free days (MSFsc) was derived from MCTQ, and tertile splits were used to define early, intermediate and late DLMO-CT, MEQ-CT and MSFsc-CT chronotype categories. DLMO correlated with MEQ score (r = −0.25, p = 0.035) and MSFsc (r = 0.32, p = 0.015). For early, intermediate and late DLMO-CT categories, mean(SD) DLMO were 20:25(0:46), 21:33(0:10) and 23:03(0:53). For early, intermediate and late MEQ-CT categories, mean(SD) MEQ scores were 60.5(5.3), 51.4(2.9) and 40.8 (5.0). For early, intermediate and late MSFsc-CT categories, mean(SD) MSFsc were 03:23(0:34), 04:37(0:12) and 05:55(0:48). Low concordance of categorisations between DLMO-CT and MEQ-CT (37%), and between DLMO-CT and MSFsc-CT (37%), suggests chronotype categorisations depend on the measure used. To enable valid comparisons with previous results and reduce the likelihood of misleading conclusions, researchers should select measures and statistical techniques appropriate to the construct of interest and research question.

1. Introduction

The body clock of each human individual is uniquely timed. Individual differences in body clock timing impact biological rhythms, resulting in a wide range of preferred and actual daily sleep and activity patterns. Chronotype is a construct that reflects individual differences underlying circadian timing [1,2]. Earlier chronotypes get up, achieve peak performance, and go to bed earlier than later chronotypes [3]. Mounting evidence suggests chronotype plays an important role in physical health (e.g., diabetes, metabolic disorders), mental health (e.g., depression, anxiety), and shift-work tolerance [4,5].
Chronobiology researchers have applied varied approaches to the operationalisation and analysis of chronotype for more than one hundred years. Chronotype operationalised as circadian phase or timing can be based on the timing of objective markers in biological variables that exhibit circadian rhythms (e.g., melatonin concentration, core body temperature). Dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) is the most common and reliable circadian phase marker and can be detected in periodic blood, saliva and urine samples [6]. However, collection of biological samples is invasive, laborious, and costly, limiting chronotype determination from objective measures to laboratory experiments with small samples [3]. Self-report questionnaires are simple to administer, and several have been developed to determine circadian preference or typical timing. Chronotype operationalised as preferred timing of sleep and activity is usually determined from the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) [7]. Chronotype operationalised as the phase of entrainment between the sleep-wake cycle and the 24-h clock can be based on sleep markers sourced from the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ) [8], which incorporates questions about typical times for sleep and activity.
Inconsistency in the approach to the operationalisation of chronotype has stimulated discussion in the literature [1,9]. Whilst chronotype based on objectively-measured biological rhythms is considered appropriate for examination of the impact of circadian timing on outcome variables (e.g., cognitive performance) [10], chronotype based on daily preference is appropriate for research on psychological traits, and chronotype based on the phase of entrainment is appropriate for research on circadian traits that are influenced by the entraining environment [9]. Several studies have assessed relationships between biological rhythms and MEQ/MCTQ measures, e.g., [11,12,13]. DLMO consistently correlates with both MEQ score and MSFsc (corrected midpoint of sleep on free days derived from the MCTQ), however the strength of correlations varies between populations (e.g., DLMO-MEQ score: r = −0.40, DLMO-MSFsc: r = 0.54 [14]; DLMO-MEQ score: r = −0.70, DLMO-MSFsc: r = 0.68 [2]). Although measures of daily preferences and phase of entrainment may correlate, they are not comparable or interchangeable [9]. It follows that any research applying circadian typology should clearly justify the selection of the chronotype definition and measure. Furthermore, to simplify analyses and interpretations, distributions of continuous measures are often stratified into chronotype (e.g., early, intermediate, and late) categories. However, the approach to determining the cut-offs that separate categories is also inconsistent; most researchers apply percentile splits, e.g., [15,16], however, others use self-decided or recommended absolute cut-offs, e.g., [17,18].
Although DLMO, MEQ score and MSFsc correlate, chronotype categorisations based on them do not necessarily concord. An individual may be categorised as early-type based on one measure, but as intermediate or late-type based on another measure. Low concordance between categorisations based on different chronotype measures and arbitrary cut-off scores limit meaningful comparisons of results between studies [1]. Some researchers have sought to mitigate this issue by categorising using a combination of subjective and objective measures, e.g., [19]. A weighted combination of multiple continuous circadian measures may provide more reliable chronotype categorisations, and several approaches have been suggested [20,21].
Surprisingly, there do not appear to be any studies that have systematically quantified concordance between chronotype classifications based on independent, subjective and objective circadian measures [22]. This study will close this gap by quantifying the concordance of chronotype categorisations based on an objective measure (DLMO) with categorisations based on two subjective measures (MEQ and MCTQ), for a relatively large sample of participants. As MSFsc (from MCTQ) is a measure of behaviour influenced by circadian timing, we predict greater concordance between DLMO and MCTQ chronotype categorisations than between DLMO and MEQ chronotype categorisations.

2. Results

2.1. DLMO, MEQ Score and MSFsc

The MEQ was completed by each of the 72 participants, and DLMO was available for 71 participants. MSFsc was calculated for the 57 participants who reported not using an alarm to wake up on free days on the MCTQ. Of these 57 participants, 28 (i.e., 49%) reported they had regular work schedules. Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests confirmed the DLMO, MEQ score, and MSFsc distributions were normal; D (71) = 0.10, p = 0.076; D (72) = 0.066, p = 0.200; D (57) = 0.099, p = 0.200, respectively (Figure 1). Pearson bivariate correlation analyses confirmed DLMO and MEQ score were correlated, r (71) = −0.25, p = 0.035, DLMO and MSFsc were correlated, r (57) = 0.32, p = 0.015, and MEQ score and MSFsc were correlated, r (57) = −0.64, p < 0.001 (Figure 2).

2.2. DLMO-CT Categories

DLMO and MEQ data were available for the 57 participants for which MSFsc could be calculated. The DLMO distribution for this sub-sample was divided into DLMO-CT categories; participants with DLMO earlier than 21:15 were categorised as early DLMO-CT, participants with DLMO later than 21:57 were categorised as late DLMO-CT, and the remaining participants were categorised as intermediate DLMO-CT. The corresponding mean MEQ scores and mean MSFsc were calculated for each DLMO-CT category (Table 1). A one-way between-groups ANOVA with DLMO-CT as the independent variable revealed no significant effect for MEQ score, F(2,54) = 2.12, p = 0.13, and a significant effect for MSFsc, F(2,54) = 4.79, p = 0.012. Post-hoc comparisons using the Games–Howell procedure revealed a difference between the mean MSFsc of the early and late categories (p = 0.027), but no differences between mean MSFsc of the other categories: early-intermediate (p = 0.083) and intermediate-late (p = 0.585).

2.3. MEQ-CT and MSFsc-CT Categories

For the sub-sample of 57 participants, the MEQ and MSFsc distributions were each divided into chronotype categories. For the MEQ score distribution, participants with scores greater than or equal to 56 were categorised as early MEQ-CT, participants with scores less than or equal to 46 were categorised as late MEQ-CT, and the remaining participants were categorised as intermediate MEQ-CT (Table 2a). For the MSFsc distribution, participants with MSFsc earlier than 04:16 h were categorised as early MSFsc-CT, participants with MSFsc later than 04:53 h were categorised as late MSFsc-CT, and the remaining participants were categorised as intermediate MSFsc-CT (Table 2b).

2.4. Concordance of Categorisations

To assess the concordance of categorisations based on the three measures, DLMO-CT categorisations were compared with MEQ-CT and MSFsc-CT categorisations for each participant. The number of participants for each of the nine permutations was tallied (Table 3, Part A). The number of concordances was calculated as the number of participants whose DLMO-CT categorisation matched the subjective measure categorisation (i.e., early-early, intermediate-intermediate, late-late). The number of 1-step errors was calculated as the number of participants for which there was a one-category difference between the DLMO-CT category and the subjective measure category (i.e., early-intermediate, intermediate-early, intermediate-late, late-intermediate). The number of 2-step errors was calculated as the number of participants for which there was a two-category difference between the DLMO-CT category and the subjective measure category (i.e., early-late, late-early) (Table 3, Part B).

3. Discussion

For this sample of young, healthy adults, the continuous measures of DLMO, MEQ score and MSFsc were normally distributed and correlated. Circadian questionnaires such as the MEQ and MCTQ are simple and suited to the field and large-scale studies, but their measures are not interchangeable with each other, or with DLMO. DLMO was more strongly correlated with MSFsc than MEQ score, although neither correlation is strong. MSFsc, a subjective measure of sleep timing, appears more closely related to endogenous circadian timing than the subjective preference for sleep timing measured by MEQ [2]. The correlation between DLMO and MEQ score (r = −0.25) is weaker than the same correlation reported for other studies (r = −0.35 to −0.76) [23]. The correlation between DLMO and MSFsc (r = 0.32) may be weaker than the same correlation reported by others (r = 0.54 [14], r = 0.68 [2]) because our participants were mostly international travellers or students. Application of the MCTQ to individuals with irregular work arrangements may be problematic, as its calculations assume structured work schedules [24]. Furthermore, chronotype can only be determined from the MCTQ when an alarm clock is not used on free days [25], and therefore MEQ is sometimes used in preference to MCTQ simply to maximise available data, e.g., [26]. In the present study, MSFsc could not be calculated for 14 participants, reducing our sample to 57. A recently developed short-form of MCTQ (µMCTQ) allows MSFsc to be calculated for all respondents by capturing wake times only on free days on which an alarm clock is not used [27], and a similar modification to the standard MCTQ may enhance its utility.
For chronotype categorisations based on DLMO, there were no differences in mean MEQ scores between each category, and only a difference in mean MSFsc between the early and late categories. This suggests that for this sample, MEQ scores and MSFsc of participants in the early, intermediate, and late chronotype categories based on DLMO were indistinguishable. Chronotype categorisations based on splits of both the MEQ score distribution and the MSFsc distribution had low concordance with chronotype categorisations based on a split of the DLMO distribution. Compared with categorisations based on DLMO, concordance for categorisations based on both MEQ score and MSFsc was 37% (i.e., 63% error rate). However, categorisations based on the MEQ score had a greater proportion of 2-step errors (21%) than categorisations based on MSFsc (14%). The low concordance may be a consequence of applying percentile splits to normally distributed continuous variables to create a categorical variable. This approach allows for the use of ANOVA statistical methods that simplify analyses and interpretation of differences between groups. A shortcoming of this methodology is that participants with measures near, but below or above, the percentile cut-offs can be assigned to different categories, although their measures are actually closer to each other than other members of their assigned category. Secondly, percentile splits of each study sample will yield different thresholds, limiting the generalisability of the results and comparisons with other studies. Although samples are sometimes dichotomised by a median split, e.g., [26], trichotomisation by tertile split, as in the present study, should provide superior between-group comparisons. The risk of drawing incorrect conclusions arising from categorisations based on continuous measures can usually be mitigated or overcome by regression and correlational analysis [28].
An important limitation of this study is that extreme chronotypes were excluded due to the selection criteria for the main shift work-study. However, our findings are generalisable to populations of younger, healthy adults, as epidemiological studies report that extreme chronotypes are uncommon [29]. Mean DLMO of our early and late DLMO chronotype categories (20:25 h and 23:03 h respectively) are comparable with criteria applied in other studies (e.g., early: <21:30 h, late >22.30 h [20]), indicating our sample included a broad range of chronotypes. We demonstrated greater concordance between categorisations based on DLMO and categorisations based on each of the subjective measures for the early and late categories than for the intermediate category, suggesting concordance may be greater with more extreme chronotypes. A second limitation to our study is that to illustrate the underlying measurement issue, we divided our sample into chronotype categories using tertile splits. Although this method is commonly used by chronotype researchers, categorisations based on other arbitrary or recommended percentiles or absolute cut-offs would likely yield different levels of categorisation concordance.
Valid measurement is a key element of scientific research. Our results suggest that laboratory experimental research on the differential effects of body clock timing on outcome variables should use objective biological measures where possible. DLMO, MEQ and MCTQ measure related, but different constructs that are not interchangeable. Although these measures were correlated for this sample, the correlations were not strong. Consequently, chronotype categorisations based on percentile splits of these distributions showed low concordance. To enable valid comparisons with previous results and reduce the likelihood of misleading conclusions, chronotype researchers should select the most appropriate measures and statistical techniques for their construct of interest and research question.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Participants

Data were collected during a simulated shift work-study conducted at the Appleton Institute in Adelaide, South Australia. Participants were 72 young, healthy adults (36 females, 36 males) with a mean (± SD) age of 23.1 (± 3.6) years and body mass index (BMI) of 21.5 (±1.9) kg/m2, recruited by advertisements posted at hostels, student accommodation and university campuses and on casual employment websites. Screening involved the completion of a general health questionnaire and an in-person interview. Key inclusion criteria included age (18–30 years), BMI (18–25 kg/m2) and good physical and mental health. Key exclusion criteria included smoking, use of medications (excluding oral contraceptives), use of recreational drugs, excessive alcohol or caffeine consumption, excessive exercise and shift work or transmeridian travel during the month prior to the study. Participants were mostly international travellers or students, provided written informed consent and were financially compensated with an honorarium.

4.2. Procedure

Data for this study were collected during the first two days of a multi-day laboratory shift work-study. During the week before the study, participants were requested to maintain their normal sleep patterns, complete a sleep diary, and wear an activity monitor on their non-dominant wrist. In the laboratory, each participant was accommodated with their own bedroom and bathroom. Days 1 and 2 were work-free days. On Day 1, participants entered the laboratory at 16:00 and were provided with a 9-h sleep opportunity (23:00–08:00). On Day 2, participants completed the MEQ and MCTQ and were familiarised with the simulated shift-work tasks between periods of free time. Nine saliva samples were then collected from each participant hourly in dim light (<10 lux) (19:00–03:00). Twenty minutes before each sample was collected, participants were instructed to gently rinse their mouths with water, remain seated, and refrain from eating and drinking until after the sample was collected. To collect saliva, participants rolled a cotton swab in their mouths for approximately 2–3 min. Swabs were refrigerated prior to centrifuging and freezing at −20 °C. After saliva sampling, participants were provided a 9-h sleep opportunity (03:00–12:00).

4.3. Measures

4.3.1. Dim Light Melatonin Onset (DLMO)

DLMO was determined from saliva collected using cotton swabs (Salivette; Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany). Melatonin concentration was measured by 4.3 pM direct radioimmunoassay, using reagents from Buhlmann Laboratories AG (Allschwill, Switzerland). DLMO was defined as the time melatonin concentration reached a fixed threshold of 10 pM and stayed above this threshold for at least two subsequent samples [30]. For one participant, whose melatonin concentration was above 10 pM for all samples, a higher relative threshold equal to the mean of the first three melatonin concentration values plus two standard deviations of those values was used [30]. Linear interpolation was applied to estimate the time of DLMO between the sample times immediately before and after concentration exceeded the threshold.

4.3.2. Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire

The Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) [7] is the most commonly used circadian typology questionnaire and consists of 19 questions with Likert responses about an individual’s preferred bedtimes, get-up times, and times for activity [22]. Responses to questions are scored and summed to produce an overall morningness score ranging from 16 to 86, with higher scores indicating greater morningness [7].

4.3.3. Munich Chronotype Questionnaire

The Munich Chronotype Questionnaire(MCTQ) [8] incorporates 14 questions about the timing of sleep and activity on work and free days. Responses can be used to assess work schedule, the timing of sleep and alarm clock use on work and free days and the number of days worked per week [31]. The midpoint of sleep on free days (MSF) can be extracted from the MCTQ and used to determine chronotype [8]. The premise of using MSF to determine chronotype is that an individual’s sleep and wake timing on free days corresponds to their circadian timing, with earlier MSF corresponding with earlier chronotype [15]. MSF is calculated as the sleep onset on free days (SOf), plus half the sleep duration on free days (SDf). As most individuals accumulate a sleep debt during the working week and extend their sleep on free days, MSF can be “sleep-corrected” to MSFsc, the corrected sleep midpoint on free days. MSFsc is calculated as MSF minus a correction for sleep debt equal to half the difference between sleep duration on free days (SDf) and average sleep duration over the week (SDweek), which is only applied if sleep duration on free days is greater than sleep duration on workdays [24]. An important limitation of the MCTQ is that individuals who use an alarm to wake up on free days cannot be chronotyped, as their MSF may be impacted and therefore not reflect internal timing [25].

4.4. Statistical Analyses

Data were analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0 (Armonk, NY, USA), with statistical significance, determined using an alpha level of 0.05. Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were used to assess the normality of the DLMO, MEQ and MSFsc distributions. Pearson pairwise bivariate correlation analyses were used to assess relationships between DLMO, MEQ and MSFsc. Consistent with the approach recommended for comparing chronotypes based on mid-sleep times derived from MCTQ within a sample [32], the DLMO, MEQ and MSFsc distributions were each divided into early, intermediate and late DLMO-CT, MEQ-CT, and MSFsc-CT categories by applying tertile splits.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: A.M.R.; methodology: A.M.R., G.D.R. and C.S.; formal analysis: A.M.R.; data curation: C.S.; writing—original draft: A.M.R.; writing—review and editing: G.D.R. and C.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Australian Research Council (ARC) under Grant DP160104909.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Central Queensland University Human Research Ethics Committee (Approval Number: H16/06-168, October 2016).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data for this study are not currently publicly available as they are part of a larger dataset that will be used for another purpose.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the study participants and the technical staff at Appleton Institute.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Levandovski, R.; Sasso, E.; Hidalgo, M.P. Chronotype: A review of the advances, limits and applicability of the main instruments used in the literature to assess human phenotype. Trends Psychiatry Psychother 2013, 35, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Kantermann, T.; Sung, H.; Burgess, H.J. Comparing the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire and Munich ChronoType Questionnaire to the Dim Light Melatonin Onset. J. Biol. Rhythms 2015, 30, 449–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Adan, A.; Archer, S.N.; Hidalgo, M.P.; Di Milia, L.; Natale, V.; Randler, C. Circadian typology: A comprehensive review. Chronobiol. Int. 2012, 29, 1153–1175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Fabbian, F.; Zucchi, B.; De Giorgi, A.; Tiseo, R.; Boari, B.; Salmi, R.; Cappadona, R.; Gianesini, G.; Bassi, E.; Signani, F.; et al. Chronotype, gender and general health. Chronobiol. Int. 2016, 33, 863–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Juda, M.; Vetter, C.; Roenneberg, T. Chronotype modulates sleep duration, sleep quality, and social jet lag in shift-workers. J. Biol. Rhythms 2013, 28, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Burgess, H.J.; Eastman, C.I. The dim light melatonin onset following fixed and free sleep schedules. J. Sleep Res. 2005, 14, 229–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Horne, J.A.; Ostberg, O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. 1976, 4, 97–110. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  8. Roenneberg, T.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Merrow, M. Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythms 2003, 18, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Roenneberg, T. Having trouble typing? What on earth is chronotype? J. Biol. Rhythms 2015, 30, 487–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Ritchie, H.K.; Burke, T.M.; Dear, T.B.; McHill, A.W.; Axelsson, J.; Wright, K.P., Jr. Impact of sleep inertia on visual selective attention for rare targets and the influence of chronotype. J. Sleep Res. 2017, 26, 551–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Hida, A.; Kitamura, S.; Ohsawa, Y.; Enomoto, M.; Katayose, Y.; Motomura, Y.; Moriguchi, Y.; Nozaki, K.; Watanabe, M.; Aritake, S.; et al. In vitro circadian period is associated with circadian/sleep preference. Sci. Rep. 2013, 3, 2074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Brown, S.A.; Kunz, D.; Dumas, A.; Westermark, P.O.; Vanselow, K.; Tilmann-Wahnschaffe, A.; Herzel, H.; Kramer, A. Molecular insights into human daily behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2008, 105, 1602–1607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Ferrante, A.; Gellerman, D.; Ay, A.; Woods, K.P.; Filipowicz, A.M.; Jain, K.; Bearden, N.; Ingram, K.K. Diurnal preference predicts phase differences in expression of human peripheral circadian clock genes. J. Circadian Rhythms 2015, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Kitamura, S.; Hida, A.; Aritake, S.; Higuchi, S.; Enomoto, M.; Kato, M.; Vetter, C.; Roenneberg, T.; Mishima, K. Validity of the Japanese version of the Munich ChronoType Questionnaire. Chronobiol. Int. 2014, 31, 845–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Ritonja, J.; Tranmer, J.; Aronson, K.J. The relationship between night work, chronotype, and cardiometabolic risk factors in female hospital employees. Chronobiol. Int. 2019, 36, 616–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Vetter, C.; Fischer, D.; Matera, J.L.; Roenneberg, T. Aligning work and circadian time in shift workers improves sleep and reduces circadian disruption. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 907–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Caci, H.; Deschaux, O.; Adan, A.; Natale, V. Comparing three morningness scales: Age and gender effects, structure and cut-off criteria. Sleep Med. 2009, 10, 240–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kervezee, L.; Gonzales-Aste, F.; Boudreau, P.; Boivin, D.B. The relationship between chronotype and sleep behavior during rotating shift work: A field study. Sleep 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Breithaupt, H.; Hildebrandt, G.; D HRE, D.; Josch, R.; Sieber, U.; Werner, M. Tolerance to shift of sleep, as related to the individual’s circadian phase position. Ergonomics 1978, 21, 767–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Facer-Childs, E.R.; Middleton, B.; Bagshaw, A.P.; Skene, D.J. Human Circadian Phenotyping and Diurnal Performance Testing in the Real World. JoVE 2020, e60448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Smith, C.S.; Reilly, C.; Midkiff, K. Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J. Appl. Psychol. 1989, 74, 728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Di Milia, L.; Adan, A.; Natale, V.; Randler, C. Reviewing the psychometric properties of contemporary circadian typology measures. Chronobiol. Int. 2013, 30, 1261–1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Sack, R.L.; Auckley, D.; Auger, R.R.; Carskadon, M.A.; Wright, K.P., Jr.; Vitiello, M.V.; Zhdanova, I.V.; American Academy of Sleep, M. Circadian rhythm sleep disorders: Part I, basic principles, shift work and jet lag disorders. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep 2007, 30, 1460–1483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Roenneberg, T.; Pilz, L.K.; Zerbini, G.; Winnebeck, E.C. Chronotype and Social Jetlag: A (Self-) Critical Review. Biology 2019, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Roenneberg, T.; Allebrandt, K.V.; Merrow, M.; Vetter, C. Social jetlag and obesity. Curr Biol 2012, 22, 939–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Thomas, J.M.; Kern, P.A.; Bush, H.M.; McQuerry, K.J.; Black, W.S.; Clasey, J.L.; Pendergast, J.S. Circadian rhythm phase shifts caused by timed exercise vary with chronotype. JCI Insight 2020, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Ghotbi, N.; Pilz, L.K.; Winnebeck, E.C.; Vetter, C.; Zerbini, G.; Lenssen, D.; Frighetto, G.; Salamanca, M.; Costa, R.; Montagnese, S.; et al. The µMCTQ: An Ultra-Short Version of the Munich ChronoType Questionnaire. J. Biol. Rhythms 2020, 35, 98–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. MacCallum, R.C.; Zhang, S.; Preacher, K.J.; Rucker, D.D. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychol. Methods 2002, 7, 19–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Roenneberg, T.; Kuehnle, T.; Juda, M.; Kantermann, T.; Allebrandt, K.; Gordijn, M.; Merrow, M. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med. Rev. 2007, 11, 429–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Voultsios, A.; Kennaway, D.J.; Dawson, D. Salivary melatonin as a circadian phase marker: Validation and comparison to plasma melatonin. J. Biol. Rhythms 1997, 12, 457–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Burgess, H.J.; Kikyo, F.; Valdespino-Hayden, Z.; Rizvydeen, M.; Kimura, M.; Pollack, M.H.; Hobfoll, S.E.; Rajan, K.B.; Zalta, A.K.; Burns, J.W. Do the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire and Munich ChronoType Questionnaire Change After Morning Light Treatment? Sleep Sci. Pract. 2018, 2, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Roenneberg, T.; Keller, L.K.; Fischer, D.; Matera, J.L.; Vetter, C.; Winnebeck, E.C. Human activity and rest in situ. In Methods Enzymol; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2015; Volume 552, pp. 257–283. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Frequency distributions of DLMO (n = 71), MEQ score (n = 72), and MSFsc (n = 57).
Figure 1. Frequency distributions of DLMO (n = 71), MEQ score (n = 72), and MSFsc (n = 57).
Clockssleep 03 00021 g001
Figure 2. Scatter plots of MEQ score versus DLMO (n = 71), MSFsc versus DLMO (n = 57), and MSFsc versus MEQ score (n = 57).
Figure 2. Scatter plots of MEQ score versus DLMO (n = 71), MSFsc versus DLMO (n = 57), and MSFsc versus MEQ score (n = 57).
Clockssleep 03 00021 g002
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and ranges of DLMO, MEQ Score and MSFsc for early, intermediate and late DLMO-CT categories.
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and ranges of DLMO, MEQ Score and MSFsc for early, intermediate and late DLMO-CT categories.
DLMO-CT CategorynDLMO (hh:mm)
M (SD), Range
MEQ Score
M (SD), Range
MSFsc (hh:mm)
M (SD), Range
Early1920:25 (0:46), 19:12–21:1354.8 (11.1), 35–7104:01 (1:15), 02:22–06:42
Intermediate1921:33 (0:10), 21:16–21:5349.6 (6.5), 39–6004:46 (0:47), 03:20–06:40
Late1923:03 (0:53), 22:04–00:4749.2 (10.1), 27–6305:07 (1:13), 03:22–07:45
Overall5721:40 (1:16), 19:12–00:4751.2 (9.7), 27–7104:38 (1:11), 02:22–07:45
Table 2. (a) Means, standard deviations, and ranges of MEQ Score for MEQ-CT categories; (b) Means, standard deviations and ranges of MSFsc for MSFsc-CT categories.
Table 2. (a) Means, standard deviations, and ranges of MEQ Score for MEQ-CT categories; (b) Means, standard deviations and ranges of MSFsc for MSFsc-CT categories.
(a) MEQ-CT(b) MSFsc-CT
MEQ-CT CategoryMEQ Score
n, M (SD), Range
MSFsc-CT CategoryMSFsc (hh:mm)
n, M (SD), Range
Early22, 60.5 (5.3), 56–71Early19, 03:23 (0:34), 02:22–04:15
Intermediate15, 51.4 (2.9), 47–55Intermediate19, 04:37 (0:12), 04:17–04:52
Late20, 40.8 (5.0), 27–46Late19, 05:55 (0:48), 04:54–07:45
Overall57, 51.2 (9.7), 27–71Overall57, 04:38 (1:11), 02:22–07:45
Table 3. Part A: Number of participants in each DLMO-CT and MEQ-CT category combination and each DLMO-CT and MSFsc-CT category combination. Part B: Number (percentage) of concordances, 1-step errors and 2-step errors between DLMO-CT and MEQ-CT categorisations and between DLMO-CT and MSFsc-CT categorisations.
Table 3. Part A: Number of participants in each DLMO-CT and MEQ-CT category combination and each DLMO-CT and MSFsc-CT category combination. Part B: Number (percentage) of concordances, 1-step errors and 2-step errors between DLMO-CT and MEQ-CT categorisations and between DLMO-CT and MSFsc-CT categorisations.
Part A
DLMO-CT CategoryMEQ-CT CategoryMSFsc-CT Category
EarlyIntermediateLateTotalEarlyIntermediateLateTotal
Early9551984719
Intermediate65819105419
Late75719110819
Total2215205719191957
Part B
Concordance 19 + 5 + 7 = 21 (37%)8 + 5 + 8 = 21 (37%)
1-step errors 25 + 6 + 8 + 5 = 24 (42%)4 + 10 + 4 + 10 = 28 (49%)
2-step errors 35 + 7 = 12 (21%)7 + 1 = 8 (14%)
Total57 (100%)57 (100%)
1 Concordance: Agreement between DLMO-CT categorisation and subjective measure categorisation. 2 1-step error: One category difference between DLMO-CT categorisation and subjective measure categorisation (i.e., early or late DLMO-CT categorisation, and intermediate categorisation based on the other measure, and vice versa). 3 2-step error: Two category difference between DLMO-CT category and subjective measure category (i.e., early (late) DLMO-CT categorisation and late (early) categorisation based on the other measure and vice versa).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Reiter, A.M.; Sargent, C.; Roach, G.D. Concordance of Chronotype Categorisations Based on Dim Light Melatonin Onset, the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire, and the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire. Clocks & Sleep 2021, 3, 342-350. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/clockssleep3020021

AMA Style

Reiter AM, Sargent C, Roach GD. Concordance of Chronotype Categorisations Based on Dim Light Melatonin Onset, the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire, and the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire. Clocks & Sleep. 2021; 3(2):342-350. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/clockssleep3020021

Chicago/Turabian Style

Reiter, Andrew M., Charli Sargent, and Gregory D. Roach. 2021. "Concordance of Chronotype Categorisations Based on Dim Light Melatonin Onset, the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire, and the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire" Clocks & Sleep 3, no. 2: 342-350. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/clockssleep3020021

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop