Next Article in Journal
Impact of Hydroxychloroquine Treatment of COVID-19 on Cardiac Conduction: The Beat Goes On
Previous Article in Journal
A Throat Lozenge with Fixed Combination of Cetylpyridinium Chloride and Benzydamine Hydrochloride Has Direct Virucidal Effect on SARS-CoV-2
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Mediating Role of Rumination in the Relationship between Loneliness and Depression in University Students during the COVID-19 Pandemic

by
Ines Luttenbacher
1,2,*,
Jamie S. Breukel
1 and
Maheen M. Adamson
2,3
1
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, 1012 WX Amsterdam, The Netherlands
2
Adamson Lab, Headache Center for Excellence, Rehabilitation Service, VA Palo Alto Health Care System, 3801 Miranda Avenue, Palo Alto, CA 94304, USA
3
Department of Neurosurgery, Stanford University School of Medicine, 300 Pasteur Drive, Palo Alto, CA 94304, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 1 September 2021 / Revised: 17 September 2021 / Accepted: 20 September 2021 / Published: 27 September 2021

Abstract

:
Introduction: While mitigation procedures are needed to prevent the continuous spread of COVID-19, they may, in turn, negatively impact individuals’ mental health. Therefore, the current study aimed to investigate the relationships between loneliness, rumination, and depression, as well as the mediating role of rumination in the relationship between loneliness and depression in university students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Methods: A cross-sectional online survey was circulated, collecting a final sample of 288 university students (Mage = 22.01, SD = 3.45, range = 18–55, 75.7% female). Study variables were measured utilizing self-report questionnaires. Results: In line with the hypotheses, rumination partially mediated the relationship between loneliness and depression. Discussion: Therefore, ruminative thoughts may be one of the key factors contributing to lonely university students’ susceptibility to depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic.

1. Introduction

Unprecedented, the current COVID-19 pandemic has revealed itself to be one of the most devastating and deadliest health crises in recent history. More than a year following the outbreak of the novel Coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2), approximately 175 million people have already contracted the disease and more than 3.8 million deaths have been reported [1]. Governments worldwide were forced to implement several mitigation procedures, including social distancing, isolation, and quarantine [2]. While these measures are needed to prevent the continuous spread of COVID-19, they contrast with the basic human need for social connection and may, therefore, contribute to the development of mental health problems.
Indeed, several early studies have revealed that COVID-19 not only directly affects people’s physical health, but that it may also have indirect, detrimental effects on their psychological well-being. For instance, an increase in depression [3,4,5,6] and feelings of loneliness [5] as a result of social isolation [4,7], has recently been reported.
Early theorists have established loneliness as an antecedent of depression. In line with other scholars, depression is defined as experiencing a low mood among other symptoms, such as feelings of worthlessness and hopelessness [8]. Loneliness can be defined as a negative emotional response to suboptimal relationships resulting from a discrepancy between one’s desired and achieved quality of social relationships [9]. Some authors have suggested that loneliness threatens an individual’s self-concept and self-worth when the self is predominately defined in terms of their suboptimal social relationships [9,10]. This, in turn, could leave such individuals more vulnerable to depression [9,10,11]. A recent meta-analysis supported this theory by showing that loneliness strongly predicts depression [12]; a finding that was further supported by studies conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic [13,14].
University students are thought to be particularly affected by the social isolation rules following the outbreak of COVID-19 [15]. Preceding COVID-19, attending university was already a stressful period in terms of maintaining the quality of social relationships and, subsequently, adjusting socially and emotionally [16]. With social isolation rules and online education now being implemented, the maintenance of such quality social relations may become more strenuous and trigger negative health consequences. Indeed, during the COVID-19 pandemic, 82% of a sample consisting of Bangladeshi university students reported mild to severe depressive symptoms in response to COVID-19-related social isolation [17]. This elucidates an increase in the prevalence of depression prior to COVID-19 [18]. Notably, a strong link between loneliness, depression, and suicide in college students has been identified [19], further illustrating the importance of investigating the relationship between loneliness and depression by elucidating its contributing factors.
From a theoretical perspective, rumination may explain the relationship between loneliness and depression. Rumination can be defined as a discrepancy-based, maladaptive coping strategy, as individuals often start ruminating when experiencing a discrepancy between their desired and achieved situations [20,21,22]. As mentioned, loneliness may arise from the discrepancy between one’s desired and achieved quality of their social relationships [9,23]. Therefore, feelings of loneliness may evoke a maladaptive coping strategy, such as rumination. Within the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, not being able to control and change the experienced discrepancy between one’s desired and achieved quality of social relationships might exacerbate rumination. Rumination, in turn, may cause people to remain fixated on negative experiences, such as those imposed by the COVID-19 pandemic, and can, in turn, impair problem solving [21,24]. According to the response styles theory, this fixation on negative experiences and the subsequent inability to diverge from a negative state of mind may give rise to depressive symptoms [21,24,25]. In summary, individuals experiencing loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic may be more likely to engage in a maladaptive coping strategy, such as rumination which, in turn, may increase their risk of experiencing depressive symptoms.
Research preceding COVID-19 has already established rumination as a partial mediator in the relationship between university students’ loneliness and depression [26]. Research during COVID-19 has also identified that rumination plays a partially mediating role in the relationship between COVID-19 stressors and stress consequences for university students [27]. The authors of these studies used scales for COVID-19 stressors and stress consequences that included items for loneliness and depression, respectively. As such, their results may be indicative of what the current study might find.
Although prior research has already found that rumination mediates the relationship between loneliness and depression in university students, whether this finding holds during a global pandemic has, heretofore, remained unclear. Therefore, the current study aims to expand existing knowledge of the aforementioned mediation model, by investigating the relationships between loneliness, rumination, and depression in a diverse sample of university students during the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic; a time characterized by increased, and forced, social isolation and, historically unique, circumstances of widespread online education. Investigating the factors contributing to depression is of the utmost importance, as findings may improve clinical practice and patient care by highlighting the most important issues requiring attention in attempts to effect change during a global pandemic. This, in turn, could allow for more targeted interventions that may help in the battle against mental health challenges that university students may face post-COVID-19.
Based on the theoretical framework and prior research presented here, five hypotheses were formed (Figure 1). The first hypothesis assumes that loneliness predicts depression (path c). Secondly, loneliness is hypothesized to predict rumination (path a), followed by the third hypothesis assuming rumination to predict depression (path b). Fourth, it is hypothesized that loneliness predicts depression if controlled for rumination indicating a partial mediation (path c′). Lastly, the fifth hypothesis assumes that rumination mediates the relationship between loneliness and depression (path a*b). The purpose of this paper was to investigate the relationships between loneliness, rumination, and depression with the intention of ultimately providing a mediation model in which loneliness serves as the predictor, rumination as the mediator, and depression as the outcome.

2. Method

2.1. Study Design

An English, cross-sectional, 10- to 15-min electronic survey was circulated through an online platform called Qualtrics (see OSF for link to survey file https://osf.io/nfre9/?view_only=9ca256578fb74d98b979130e7fcb70b4 (accessed on 2 December 2020). For this study, we collected data as part of a larger collaboration, therefore not all of the questionnaires in the survey were utilized. Survey data was collected from 2 December 2020 until 11 December 2020. The study design was strictly correlational since no variables were manipulated.

2.2. Participants

Utilizing a snowballing technique and convenience sampling, participants were recruited via the researchers’ social networks, including WhatsApp, LinkedIn, Facebook, Instagram, and the Adamson Brain Stimulation lab. Students from the University of Amsterdam (UvA) had the opportunity to earn 0.5 research credits when participating via the UvA lab. An a priori power analysis (α = 0.05, 95% power) showed that we required a sample size of 147 participants to detect medium effects [28]. A total of 410 university students partook in this study. Participants were included in the final analysis if they (a) were enrolled in a university, (b) were at least 18 years of age, (c) received a form of online education due to social isolation rules, and (d) signed the informed consent form. Participants were excluded if they (a) did not complete the entire survey, (b) failed to answer the items correctly, (c) completed the survey in under five minutes, and (d) provided invalid answers for demographics. The number of participants excluded for each criterion is outlined in Appendix A. Upon cleaning the data, a final sample of 288 university students (Mage = 22.01, SD = 3.45, range = 18–55, 75.7% female) was acquired. Table 1 displays an overview of additional demographic categories used in the current study.

3. Materials

Depressive symptoms. Depressive symptoms experienced over the past week were assessed using the Center of Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [8]. The CES-D is a 20-item self-report questionnaire consisting of four positively formulated items and 16 negatively formulated items. Each item comprises four possible response options ranging from 0 (rarely or none of the times) to 3 (most or all of the times). Statements included “I felt sad” and “My sleep was restless”. By summing up each item’s scores, a total score ranging from 0 to 60 can be calculated, with higher scores indicating greater depressive symptomatology. A score of 16 or above indicates presence of depression. Cronbach’s α   = 0.92 was calculated, suggesting a very good level of the scale’s internal consistency [29]. According to Radloff (1977) [8], the test-retest reliability is good, ranging from r = 0.45 to r = 0.7 [30].
Loneliness. Loneliness was measured using the De Jong Gierveld Loneliness scale [31]. The Loneliness scale is an 11-item self-report questionnaire measuring emotional and social loneliness. Five items are formulated positively and six negatively. Participants respond to the items on a 5-point scale (1 = yes!, 2 = yes, 3 = more or less, 4 = no, 5 = no!). Possible items include “I miss having people around me” and “I experience a general sense of emptiness”. Following correction, a total score ranging from 0 to 11 can be calculated, with higher scores indicating a more severe sense of loneliness. A score of 3 or above is interpreted as an indicator of loneliness being present. The scale’s internal consistency was calculated, with a Cronbach’s α   = 0.82 indicating a very good level [29]. Test-retest reliability ranges from r = 0.81 to r = 0.85 [32], suggesting excellent test-retest reliability [30].
Rumination. Rumination was assessed using the Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ) [33]. The PTQ is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 15 negatively formulated items to which participants respond on a scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). Possible items include “Thoughts intrude into my mind” and “I can’t stop dwelling on them”. By summing up each item’s score, a total score ranging from 0 to 60 can be calculated, with higher scores indicating higher levels of rumination. The internal consistency was calculated with a Cronbach’s α   = 0.95, indicating a very good reliability level [29]. According to Ehring and colleagues (2011) [33], test-retest reliability is r = 0.69. This is suggestive of a good test-retest reliability [30].
Demographic variables. Age, gender, nationality, ethnicity, and online education were included as demographic variables. To report the psychological factor, as opposed to the biological factor, gender, rather than sex, was quantified.

3.1. Procedure

The current study was approved by the Ethics Review Board (ERB) of the University of Amsterdam (UvA). Consent was obtained from all participating university students. Once consent was given voluntarily, participants anonymously filled out the survey. Participants from the UvA received 0.5 research credits upon finishing the survey. The possibility of receiving a debriefing and contact information was offered, upon survey completion, to participants needing psychological help.

3.2. Statistical Analyses

The software package SPSS Statistics (Version 26.0) was used for all statistical analyses. Unless otherwise specified, an alpha level of 0.05 was used to determine significance for all statistical tests. Preliminary analyses were conducted, starting with the calculation of demographics, Cronbach’s α , and intercorrelations. Intercorrelations were calculated to acquire information about whether the variables were related before proceeding with the main analysis. The rumination and loneliness variables were standardized [34], allowing for an accurate comparison between the study variables. Thereafter, the mediation assumptions were tested. Lastly, the hypotheses, including the final mediation model, were tested utilizing PROCESS macro for SPSS (Model 4) (Version 3.5) [35]. In order to test for significance of the indirect effect, bootstrap 95% confidence intervals (CI) using 5000 bootstrap samples were used [35]. Effects were deemed significant if 95% CIs did not include zero.

4. Results

4.1. Preliminary Analyses

Table 2 lists the means and intercorrelations of the study variables. The study variables showed positive intercorrelations, with depression and loneliness being strongly correlated, r = 0.571, p < 0.001; loneliness and rumination being strongly correlated, r = 0.690, p < 0.001; and rumination and depression showing a medium correlation, r = 0.425, p < 0.001. University students reported moderate loneliness (M = 5.66, SD = 3.05) heightened levels of rumination (M = 46.67, SD = 12.00), and symptoms of depression (M = 22.07, SD = 11.59).
Mediation assumptions were tested based on prior scholars’ suggestions [37,38]. All assumptions were satisfied: (1) Depression was a continuous dependent variable, (2) the relationship between depression and both loneliness and rumination, was linear, (3) the residuals were independent, (4) no influential cases biased the mediation model, (5) residuals were normally distributed, (6) multicollinearity was met, and (7) homoscedasticity was met.

4.2. Mediation Analysis

A mediation analysis using standardized predictors was performed to examine the mediating role of rumination in the relationship between loneliness and depression, by investigating the relationships between those variables in university students during the COVID-19 pandemic (Figure 2).
Hypothesis one stated that loneliness predicts depression. Hypothesis one was supported as there was a significant total effect of the loneliness score on the depression score (path c), b = 6.62, t(286) = 11.76, p < 0.001. On average, higher scores on the loneliness scale predicted higher scores on the depression scale.
Hypothesis two stated that loneliness predicts rumination. Results showed that the loneliness score significantly predicted the rumination score (path a), b = 0.43, t(286) = 7.97, p < 0.001. On average, higher scores on the loneliness scale predicted higher scores on the rumination scale. Therefore, hypothesis two was supported.
Hypothesis three stated that rumination predicts depression. This was supported as the rumination score significantly predicted the depression score (path b), b = 6.33, t(286) = 12.73, p < 0.001. On average, higher scores on the rumination scale predicted higher scores on the depression scale.
Hypothesis four stated that loneliness predicts depression if controlled for rumination. Results showed a significant direct effect of the loneliness score on the depression score if controlled for the rumination score (path c′), b = 3.92, t(286) = 7.88, p < 0.001, accounting for a partial mediation. On average, higher scores on the loneliness scale predicted higher scores on the depression scale if rumination remained constant. Therefore, hypothesis four was supported.
Hypothesis five stated that rumination mediates the relationship between loneliness and depression. This was supported as there was a significant indirect effect of the loneliness score on the depression score through the rumination score (path a*b), b = 2.70, SE = 0.40, 95% BCa CI [1.93, 3.51]. On average, higher scores on the loneliness scale predicted higher scores on the depression scale, which was explained by higher scores on the rumination scale. The mediation effect accounted for 40.76% of the total effect of loneliness on depression.

4.3. Exploratory Analysis

An exploratory analysis extending the mediation model by including gender, nationality, ethnicity and online education as covariates was performed. Results show no effects of the covariates on the mediation analysis. Table 3 shows the results of the mediation analysis including covariates. Hypothesis one was still supported showing that, on average, higher scores on the loneliness scale significantly predicted higher scores on the depression scale (path c), b = 6.56, t(282) = 11.63, p < 0.001. Hypothesis two was still supported as, on average, higher scores on the loneliness scale significantly predicted higher scores on the rumination scale (path a), b = 0.41, t(282) = 7.82, p < 0.001. Hypothesis three was still supported as results indicated that, on average, higher scores on the rumination scale significantly predicted higher scores on the depression scale (path b), b = 6.43, t(281) = 12.42, p < 0.001. Hypothesis four was supported as there was still a significant direct effect of the loneliness score on the depression score if controlled for the rumination score (path c′), b = 3.93, t(281) = 7.85, p < 0.001, accounting for a partial mediation. On average, higher scores on the loneliness scale predicted higher scores on the depression scale if rumination remained constant.
Importantly, hypothesis five was still supported even after controlling for covariates, as there was a significant indirect effect of the loneliness score on the depression score through the rumination score (path a*b), b = 2.63, SE = 0.41, 95% BCa CI [1.87, 3.47]. Those results indicate that, on average, higher scores on the loneliness scale predicted higher scores on the depression scale, which was explained by higher scores on the rumination scale. The mediation effect accounted for 40.1% of the total effect of loneliness on depression. Therefore, the mediation effect including covariates accounted for 0.66% less of the total effect of loneliness on depression than the mediation effect excluding covariates.

5. Discussion

This study has investigated the mental health of university students during the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic. To this end, the study expanded existing knowledge on the mediating role of rumination in the relationship between loneliness and depression, as well as the relationships between those variables. In line with our hypotheses, it was found that loneliness predicted both depression (1) and rumination (2). Moreover, rumination was found to be a predictor of depression (3). Additionally, the results indicated that loneliness predicted depression even when rumination was controlled for, thus indicating a partial mediation (4). Lastly, rumination mediated the relationship between loneliness and depression (5). Gender, nationality, ethnicity, and online education were included as covariates in an exploratory analysis and did not affect the conclusions drawn.

5.1. Relation to the Theoretical Framework

These findings align with the proposed theoretical framework [9,21,23,24,25]. Importantly, since the current study utilized a correlational design, it does not allow for causal inferences about the directionality of effects proposed by the theoretical framework. For instance, despite early theorists and a meta-analysis suggesting loneliness as the antecedent of depression [9,11,12,23], loneliness may also be bidirectional [39]: (1) Loneliness threatens an individual’s self-concept and self-worth, making them more vulnerable to depression [9,10,11,23], (2) Depression then causes interpersonal problems and subsequent damage to relationships which, in turn, induces more loneliness [40]. Hence, a vicious cycle between loneliness and depression may perpetuate the symptoms of both. Collectively, future research should investigate the possibility of bidirectionality and its effects on the mediating role of rumination using longitudinal data, once available.

5.2. Relation to Prior Research

As expected, the findings of the current study further support prior research by Vanhalst and colleagues (2012) [26]. Since both the study design and targeted population were comparable to the current study, similar conclusions were drawn regarding rumination being a partial mediator. The present study found that, as a partial mediator, heightened rumination only explained 40.76% of the total effect of loneliness on depression. This suggests that other mediators or moderators might play a role in accounting for this effect. In contrast to Vanhalst and colleagues (2012) [26] and the current study, Zawadzki and colleagues (2013) [41] investigated trait anxiety as an additional mediator alongside rumination. In doing so, they identified a full mediation as opposed to a partial mediation. As we continue to conduct research during the pandemic, it would be prudent to extend the current mediation model by including extra variables, such as trait anxiety.
The current study was conducted during a global pandemic, thereby providing timely and important findings. The results indicated that, compared to non-pandemic circumstances, university students during the COVID-19 pandemic experienced an increase in loneliness, rumination, and depression [42,43,44]. The increase in loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic aligns with prior research showing that social isolation acts as a risk factor for experiencing an increase in feelings of loneliness [5,7]. Concerning rumination, it may be argued that rumination imposed by the COVID-19 pandemic is unique from typical rumination, as it involves more uncertainty and uncontrollability, possibly leading to a heightened fixation on “what if” responses among those affected. As suggested by prior research, an explanation for an increase in depression during the COVID-19 pandemic is the amplified number of stressors imposed, such as limited social interactions and increased worrying [3,45]. Since meta-analyses provide more reliable findings, and information about whether means significantly differ, future researchers may benefit from a meta-analytical examination of the mediation model presented here.

6. Limitations

Although the current study revealed important and timely findings, the following limitations should be considered whilst interpreting the results. First, as briefly touched upon previously, the current study did not have access to longitudinal data, thereby limiting inferences drawn about the causality of effect. However, by utilizing a cross-sectional design during a global pandemic, the current study was able to acquire well timed insights, thereby providing an early milestone towards a better understanding of rumination’s role in the relationship between loneliness and depression. It is now up to future research to build upon these findings in a follow-up longitudinal study capable of providing more reliable and robust causal conclusions regarding rumination as the primary contributing factor in the relationship between loneliness and depression [46,47,48,49].
Second, although convenience sampling was the least time-intensive and expensive method, it may have led to an overrepresentation of certain subgroups [50]. Indeed, the current study’s sample was predominately female, possibly limiting the generalizability of the findings to males. Furthermore, it is important to note that even though investigating the current mediation model using university students was imperative, generalizability of the current results to other groups is limited. Future research may benefit from investigating the current mediation model in an older population since, for instance, the physical effects of behavioral choices (e.g., smoking, drinking) on mental health are likely to compound over time, possibly providing additional pathways that predict depressed mood among older adults.
Third, the three self-report questionnaires utilized in the survey were not counterbalanced across participants, possibly leading to an anchoring effect and, subsequently, inflated correlations. However, correlations found in the current study are in line with prior research [41]. Therefore, it was concluded that the results of the current study were not significantly influenced by the anchoring effect. Nevertheless, even though self-report questionnaires were the most appropriate means of measuring subjective and internal processes, future research should employ counterbalanced questionnaires.

7. Conclusions

Notwithstanding these limitations, the present study provided important findings by showing that ruminative thoughts are an imperative factor towards understanding the key factors that contribute to lonely university student’s greater susceptibility to depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic. Furthermore, by illustrating that loneliness predicted both rumination and depression in this study, the cumulative negative effects of loneliness on mental health became apparent. Since loneliness remained a predictor of depression even when rumination was controlled for, counselling for university students may benefit from targeting loneliness in addition to rumination. Collectively, each of the paths outlined in the mediation model was noteworthy and should be considered by health authorities, as intervention and prevention programs at multiple stages might be necessary to manage university students’ mental health. In conclusion, to avoid the negative long-term mental health consequences observed in previous pandemics [51], (Brooks et al., 2020), health counsellors should utilize the current study’s insights to help university students improve their psychological well-being during the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, I.L. and J.S.B.; Methodology, I.L. and J.S.B.; Software, I.L. and J.S.B.; Validation, I.L. and J.S.B.; Formal Analysis, I.L.; Investigation, I.L.; Data Curation, I.L. and J.S.B.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, I.L.; Writing—Review & Editing, J.S.B. and M.M.A.; Visualization, I.L.; Supervision, J.S.B. and M.M.A.; Project Administration, J.S.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

University of Amsterdam, VA Palo Alto Health Care System.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Figure A1. Participant Flowchart displaying participants excluded for each exclusion criteria specified.
Figure A1. Participant Flowchart displaying participants excluded for each exclusion criteria specified.
Covid 01 00038 g0a1

References

  1. WHO. Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Dashboard. 2020. Available online: https://covid19.who.int (accessed on 31 December 2020).
  2. Balkhair, A.A. COVID-19 pandemic: A new chapter in the history of infectious diseases. Oman Med. J. 2020, 35, e123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ettman, C.K.; Abdalla, S.M.; Cohen, G.H.; Sampson, L.; Vivier, P.M.; Galea, S. Prevalence of depression symptoms in US adults before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA Netw. Open 2020, 3, e2019686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Gardiner, C.; Laud, P.; Heaton, T.; Gott, M. What is the prevalence of loneliness amongst older people living in residential and nursing care homes? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Age Ageing 2020, 49, 748–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Van Bavel, J.J.; Baicker, K.; Boggio, P.S.; Capraro, V.; Cichocka, A.; Cikara, M.; Crockett, J.M.; Crum, A.J.; Douglas, K.M.; Druckman, J.N.; et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 460–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wang, C.; Pan, R.; Wan, X.; Tan, Y.; Xu, L.; Ho, C.S.; Ho, R.C. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Ammar, A.; Chtourou, H.; Boukhris, O.; Trabelsi, K.; Masmoudi, L.; Brach, M.; Bouaziz, B.; Bentlage, E.; How, D.; Ahmed, M.; et al. COVID-19 home confinement negatively impacts social participation and life satisfaction: A worldwide multicenter study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Blatt, S.J. Interpersonal relatedness and self-definition: Two personality configurations and their implications for psychopathology and psychotherapy. In Repression: Defense Mechanisms and Personality; Singer, J., Ed.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  10. Semmer, N.; Jacobshagen, N.; Meier, L.; Elfering, A. Occupational stress research: The stress-as-offense-to-self perspective. In Occupational Health Psychology: European Perspectives on Research, Education and Practice; McIntyre, S., Houdmont, J., Eds.; Nottingham University Press: Nottingham, UK, 2007; Volume 2, pp. 41–58. Available online: http://laurenzmeier.info/pdf/Semmer2007OHP.pdf (accessed on 4 December 2020).
  11. Cole, D.A.; Martin, J.M.; Powers, B. A competency-based model of child depression: A longitudinal study of peer, parent, teacher, and self-evaluations. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 505–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Erzen, E.; Çikrikci, Ö. The effect of loneliness on depression: A meta-analysis. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2018, 64, 427–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Lee, C.M.; Cadigan, J.M.; Rhew, I.C. Increases in loneliness among young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic and association with increases in mental health problems. J. Adolesc. Health 2020, 67, 714–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Palgi, Y.; Shrira, A.; Ring, L.; Bodner, E.; Avidor, S.; Bergman, Y.; Cohen-Fridel, S.; Keisari, S.; Hoffman, Y. The loneliness pandemic: Loneliness and other concomitants of depression, anxiety and their comorbidity during the COVID-19 outbreak. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 275, 109–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Groarke, J.M.; Berry, E.; Graham-Wisener, L.; McKenna-Plumley, P.E.; McGlinchey, E.; Armour, C. Loneliness in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic: Cross-sectional results from the COVID-19 psychological wellbeing study. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0239698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Shaver, P.; Furman, W.; Buhrmester, D. Transition to college: Network changes, social skills, and loneliness. In Understanding Personal Relationships: An Interdisciplinary Approach; SAGE: London, UK, 1985; pp. 193–219. [Google Scholar]
  17. Islam, A.; Barna, S.D.; Raihan, H.; Alam Khan, N.; Hossain, T. Depression and anxiety among university students during the COVID-19 pandemic in Bangladesh: A web-based cross-sectional survey. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0238162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ibrahim, A.K.; Kelly, S.; Adams, C.E.; Glazebrook, C. A systematic review of studies of depression prevalence in university students. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013, 47, 391–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Furr, S.R.; Westefeld, J.S.; McConnell, G.N.; Jenkins, J.M. Suicide and depression among college students: A decade later. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 2001, 32, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Martin, L.L.; Tesser, A. Some ruminative thoughts. In Ruminative Thoughts: Advances in Social Cognition; Wyer, R.S., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1996; Volume 9, pp. 1–47. [Google Scholar]
  21. Nolen-Hoeksema, S.; Wisco, B.E.; Lyubomirsky, S. Rethinking rumination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 3, 400–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Raes, F.; Schoofs, H.; Griffith, J.W.; Hermans, D. Rumination relates to reduced autobiographical memory specificity in formerly depressed patients following a self-discrepancy challenge: The case of autobiographical memory specificity reactivity. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2012, 43, 1002–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Peplau, L.A.; Perlman, D. Perspectives on loneliness. In Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research, and Therapy; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1982; pp. 1–20. [Google Scholar]
  24. Anderson, C. Attributional style, depression, and loneliness: A cross-cultural comparison of American and Chinese students. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1999, 25, 482–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Compas, B.E.; Connor-Smith, J.K.; Saltzman, H.; Thomsen, A.H.; Wadsworth, M.E. Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 87–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Vanhalst, J.; Luyckx, K.; Raes, F.; Goossens, L. Loneliness and depressive symptoms: The mediating and moderating role of uncontrollable ruminative thoughts. J. Psychol. 2012, 146, 259–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Ye, B.; Wu, D.; Im, H.; Liu, M.; Wang, X.; Yang, Q. Stressors of COVID-19 and stress consequences: The mediating role of rumination and the moderating role of psychological support. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2020, 118, 105466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kenny, D.A. Mediation. 2021. Available online: http://davidakenny.net/cm/mediate.htm#DI (accessed on 22 December 2020).
  29. Ursachi, G.; Horodnic, I.A.; Zait, A. How reliable are measurement scales? External factors with indirect influence on reliability estimators. Procedia Econ. Finance 2015, 20, 679–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Fleiss, J.L. The Design and Analysis of Clinical Experiments; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  31. De Jong-Gierveld, J.; Kamphuls, F. The development of a Rasch-type loneliness scale. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1985, 9, 289–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gierveld, J.D.J.; van Tilburg, T. The De Jong Gierveld short scales for emotional and social loneliness: Tested on data from 7 countries in the UN generations and gender surveys. Eur. J. Ageing 2010, 7, 121–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Ehring, T.; Watkins, E. Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic process. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 2008, 1, 192–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Aiken, L.S.; West, S.G.; Reno, R.R. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions; SAGE: London, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 3rd ed.; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  36. Cohen, J. The Effect Size. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988; pp. 77–83. [Google Scholar]
  37. Field, A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 5th ed.; SAGE: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  38. Tranmer, M.; Murphy, J.; Elliot, M.; Pampaka, M. Multiple Linear Regression, 2nd ed.—Cathie Marsh Institute Working Paper. 2020. Available online: http://hummedia.manchester.ac.uk/institutes/cmist/archive-publications/working-papers/2020/multiple-linear-regression.pdf (accessed on 4 January 2021).
  39. Nuyen, J.; Tuithof, M.; De Graaf, R.; Van Dorsselaer, S.; Kleinjan, M.; Have, M.T. The bidirectional relationship between loneliness and common mental disorders in adults: Findings from a longitudinal population-based cohort study. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2019, 55, 1297–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Joiner, T.E. Depression’s vicious scree: Self-propagating and erosive processes in depression chronicity. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 2000, 7, 203–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Zawadzki, M.J.; Graham, J.E.; Gerin, W. Rumination and anxiety mediate the effect of loneliness on depressed mood and sleep quality in college students. Health Psychol. 2013, 32, 212–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Caballer, A.; Belmonte, O.; Castillo, A.; Gasco, A.; Sansano, E.; Montoliu, R. Equivalence of chatbot and paper-and-pencil versions of the De Jong Gierveld loneliness scale. Curr. Psychol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Raes, F. Repetitive negative thinking predicts depressed mood at 3-year follow-up in students. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2012, 34, 497–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Van Dam, N.T.; Earleywine, M. Validation of the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale—Revised (CESD-R): Pragmatic depression assessment in the general population. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 186, 128–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Son, C.; Hegde, S.; Smith, A.; Wang, X.; Sasangohar, F. Effects of COVID-19 on college students’ mental health in the United States: Interview survey study. J. Med. Internet Res. 2020, 22, e21279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Cole, D.A.; Maxwell, S.E. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2003, 112, 558–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Cheong, J.; MacKinnon, D.P.; Khoo, S.T. Investigation of mediational processes using parallel process latent growth curve modeling. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2003, 10, 238–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Maxwell, S.E.; Cole, D.A. Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychol. Methods 2007, 12, 23–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Selig, J.; Preacher, K.J. Mediation models for longitudinal data in developmental research. Res. Hum. Dev. 2009, 6, 144–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Bornstein, M.H.; Jager, J.; Putnick, D. Sampling in developmental science: Situations, shortcomings, solutions, and standards. Dev. Rev. 2013, 33, 357–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Brooks, S.K.; Smith, L.E.; Webster, R.K.; Weston, D.; Woodland, L.; Hall, I.; Rubin, G.J. The impact of unplanned school closure on children’s social contact: Rapid evidence review. Eurosurveillance 2020, 25, 2000188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. The hypothesized mediation model illustrating the predictive paths between loneliness, rumination, and depression. Loneliness predicts depression (path c); loneliness predicts rumination (path a); rumination predicts depression (path b); loneliness predicts depression if controlled for rumination, indicating a partial mediation (c′); and rumination mediates the relationship between loneliness and depression (a*b).
Figure 1. The hypothesized mediation model illustrating the predictive paths between loneliness, rumination, and depression. Loneliness predicts depression (path c); loneliness predicts rumination (path a); rumination predicts depression (path b); loneliness predicts depression if controlled for rumination, indicating a partial mediation (c′); and rumination mediates the relationship between loneliness and depression (a*b).
Covid 01 00038 g001
Figure 2. Model of loneliness as a predictor of depression, mediated by rumination with the accompanying unstandardized regression coefficients of each path (N = 288). Number in parentheses denotes path c. ** p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Model of loneliness as a predictor of depression, mediated by rumination with the accompanying unstandardized regression coefficients of each path (N = 288). Number in parentheses denotes path c. ** p < 0.001.
Covid 01 00038 g002
Table 1. Demographic Categories (N = 288).
Table 1. Demographic Categories (N = 288).
Demographic CategoriesFrequencyValid Percentage
Nationality
Dutch12142
German5017.4
Other11740.6
Ethnicity
White22879.2
Moroccan/Turkish/North-African82.8
Black20.7
Asian206.9
Hispanic103.5
Other207
Online Education
Completely20270.1
Partially8228.5
Other 141.4
Note. 1 The Online Education category “Other” consisted of an open-ended question in which participants could state their special circumstances regarding the form of education they received during the COVID-19 pandemic.
Table 2. Univariate Statistics and Intercorrelations of the Study Variables (N = 288).
Table 2. Univariate Statistics and Intercorrelations of the Study Variables (N = 288).
VariablesMeanSD123
1. Depression22.0711.59
2. Loneliness5.663.050.571 **
3. Rumination46.6712.000.426 **0.690 **
Note. Small r = 0.1, medium r = 0.3, large r = 0.5 [36] ** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Results of Mediation Analysis Including Covariates (N = 288).
Table 3. Results of Mediation Analysis Including Covariates (N = 288).
Pathbtp95% C.I.
LowerUpper
c6.5611.63<0.001
a0.417.82<0.001
b6.4312.42<0.001
c′3.937.85<0.001
a*b2.63 1.873.47
Note. Displayed are the unstandardized regression coefficients (b), the t-values and the p-value of each path. The Lower and Upper 95% Confidence Interval (C.I) of the indirect effect (a*b) is displayed.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Luttenbacher, I.; Breukel, J.S.; Adamson, M.M. The Mediating Role of Rumination in the Relationship between Loneliness and Depression in University Students during the COVID-19 Pandemic. COVID 2021, 1, 447-457. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/covid1020038

AMA Style

Luttenbacher I, Breukel JS, Adamson MM. The Mediating Role of Rumination in the Relationship between Loneliness and Depression in University Students during the COVID-19 Pandemic. COVID. 2021; 1(2):447-457. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/covid1020038

Chicago/Turabian Style

Luttenbacher, Ines, Jamie S. Breukel, and Maheen M. Adamson. 2021. "The Mediating Role of Rumination in the Relationship between Loneliness and Depression in University Students during the COVID-19 Pandemic" COVID 1, no. 2: 447-457. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/covid1020038

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop