Next Article in Journal
Exploratory Survey on European Consumer and Stakeholder Attitudes towards Alternatives for Surgical Castration of Piglets
Next Article in Special Issue
Stimulus Control of Odorant Concentration: Pilot Study of Generalization and Discrimination of Odor Concentration in Canines
Previous Article in Journal
Serum Health Biomarkers in African and Asian Elephants: Value Ranges and Clinical Values Indicative of the Immune Response
Previous Article in Special Issue
Training Petting Zoo Sheep to Act Like Petting Zoo Sheep: An Empirical Evaluation of Response-Independent Schedules and Shaping with Negative Reinforcement
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

by
Nicole Pfaller-Sadovsky
1,*,
Camilo Hurtado-Parrado
2,3,
Daniela Cardillo
4,
Lucia G. Medina
3 and
Susan G. Friedman
5
1
School of Biological Sciences, Queen’s University Belfast, Belfast BT95AJ, UK
2
Department of Psychology, Troy University, Troy, AL 36082, USA
3
Faculty of Psychology, Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz, Bogota 110221, Colombia
4
Green Dogs, Via Dante Alighieri 7, 23814 Cremeno, Italy
5
Department of Psychology, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 14 August 2020 / Revised: 24 September 2020 / Accepted: 25 September 2020 / Published: 28 September 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Learning Theory Applied to the Welfare of Animals)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Conditioned reinforcement, for example, clicker training, has become increasingly popular in recent decades. Hence, questions about the effectiveness of the conditioned reinforcer have become prominent in the animal training arena. This article summarizes the scientific literature on conditioned reinforcement in applied animal training settings (e.g., homes). It was found that dogs and horses were the most frequently studied animals. Clickers and food were the most often used training stimuli. Effect size analysis found a medium effect of clicker training. The literature reviewed here shows that conditioned reinforcement is an effective approach to change animal behavior; however, sizable information potentially related to its effectiveness was not clearly reported in the studies (e.g., food preferences). Although this review fills in a gap in the literature, it also points to the need for more research to further the understanding of conditioned reinforcement phenomena.

Abstract

A conditioned reinforcer is a stimulus that acquired its effectiveness to increase and maintain a target behavior on the basis of the individual’s history—e.g., pairings with other reinforcers. This systematic review synthesized findings on conditioned reinforcement in the applied animal training field. Thirty-four studies were included in the review and six studies were eligible for a meta-analysis on the effectiveness of behavioral interventions that implemented conditioned reinforcement (e.g., clicks, spoken word, or whistles paired with food). The majority of studies investigated conditioned reinforcement with dogs (47%, n = 16) and horses (30%, n = 10) implementing click–food pairings. All other species (cats, cattle, fish, goats, and monkeys) were equally distributed across types of conditioned (e.g., clicker or spoken word) and unconditioned reinforcers (e.g., food, water, or tactile). A meta-analysis on the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement in behavioral interventions found a medium summary effect size (Tau-U 0.77; CI95% = [0.53, 0.89]), when comparing baseline, where no training was done, and treatment levels. Moderators of conditioned reinforcement effectiveness were species (e.g., horses) and research design (e.g., multiple-baseline designs). The small number of intervention-focused studies available limits the present findings and highlights the need for more systematic research into the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement across species.

1. Introduction

Humans have been training animals for different purposes for at least 10,000 years [1]. Animal training has traditionally been considered an artisanal skill, mostly encompassing a mix of evidence-based practice (i.e., an intervention or treatment that has been shown to be effective through high-quality and substantial scientific research [2]), personal experience, history, and superstition [3]. Various species, such as domesticated animals (e.g., dogs), insects, fish, and marine mammals are trained across a variety of environments (e.g., homes, shelters, stables, zoos [4,5,6,7,8,9]), applying various training approaches and techniques (e.g., based on positive or negative reinforcement [10]; for a review of training methods [11]). One training approach that has been shown to be applied across species and environments is conditioned positive reinforcement [12].
Historically, conditioned reinforcement builds on the notions of “conditioned reflexes”, also Type S or Type R (i.e., stimulus–stimulus pairing or response–stimulus, respectively), which were first studied and reported by Ivan Pavlov [13] and later reinterpreted by B.F. Skinner as respondent conditioning [14]. Establishing a conditioned reinforcer (e.g., a click, whistle, or spoken word immediately followed by an established reinforcer) is similar to developing a conditioned stimulus in respondent conditioning [15]. Skinner [14] described a procedure in which rats were exposed to a clicking sound paired with food. Later the rats were taught to press a lever by making the sound of the click alone contingent on the lever pressing response. However, because food was no longer delivered, the click was also a respondent extinction trial (i.e., the decline of the eliciting/evocative effect of a conditioned stimulus/reinforcer resulting from presentation of the conditioned stimulus/reinforcer without the unconditioned or well-established conditioned stimulus) [15]. Several years later, the notion of conditioned reinforcement was first formally introduced in the popular press by Skinner [16] (p. 26) in an article published in the Scientific American journal, and in an interview of B. F. Skinner featured in the LOOK magazine [17]. Skinner [16] (p. 26) described the conditioned reinforcer in this way:
“The best way to reinforce the behavior with the necessary speed is to use a ‘conditioned’ reinforcer. This is a signal which the animal is conditioned to associate with food. The animal is always given food immediately after the signal, and the signal itself then becomes the reinforcer… As soon as the dog moves, sound the cricket and give food.”
Contemporarily, conditioned reinforcement has seen an increase in popularity as a training method since its introduction to a wider animal training audience during the early 1990s—“clicker training” [18,19]. Given its popularity, questions about the effectiveness and efficiency of conditioned reinforcement have become some of the focal points of research in the animal training arena (e.g., [9,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30]). Generally, studies have either investigated the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement by teaching animals arbitrarily selected responses (e.g., sliding a lid to open a box, touching the top end of a stick or touching a cone, or spins and bows [20,24,25,26,27,28,29]) or used conditioned reinforcement to teach an alternative behavior during a behavior change program or teaching new skills (e.g., touching a target, slipping into a head halter, or releasing a toy on cue [4,8,22,23]). Different methodologies are evident in this literature. Studies focused on teaching alternative behaviors during a behavior change program (i.e., behavioral interventions) or teaching new skills have generally used clicks consistently followed by food, and each animal serves as its own control (i.e., single-case research methods [31]); in addition, subjects in these studies are often exposed to different experimental conditions (e.g., target training and generalization [4,8,9,22,30,31,32]).
Conversely, research that has investigated the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement via establishing arbitrary responses has mainly implemented group designs. Animals in the experimental groups have been exclusively exposed to pairings of the target stimulus to be conditioned (e.g., beep, clicker, or spoken word) followed by food, while animals in the comparison groups are trained with food only or conditioned reinforcement only (i.e., no pairings between the target stimulus and food are scheduled [28]). Some of these studies have also investigated resistance to extinction [24,25,26,28,33,34], which has been described in the applied and basic research literature as a procedure to demonstrate the relative effectiveness of conditioned reinforcers (i.e., extinction test [35,36,37]). In contrast, few studies have investigated the efficiency of clicker training by comparing a continuous pairing of click and food versus an intermittent pairing (for a discussion of these points see [38,39]) or click versus spoken word plus food [27,29].
A survey of the literature concerned with the application and effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement showed only two partially related literature reviews, both focused on the possible functions of the clicker (e.g., bridging, conditioned reinforcement, or marking functions [40,41]). The primary focus of this review is not determining the functional account of clicker training but rather systematically assessing the efficacy of the training procedures described as clicker training. These earlier reports lack a comprehensive systematic review and quantitative analysis (i.e., meta-analysis) of existing applied animal training studies on the application and effectiveness of the clicker. Accordingly, the purpose of the current paper was to (a) update and expand partial earlier reviews on conditioned reinforcement relevant to applied animal training (e.g., systematic searches across a wide range of databases and repositories), (b) synthesize experimental data on conditioned reinforcement across applied animal behavior settings (e.g., homes, training facilities, or stables), (c) quantitatively analyze the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement when implemented as part of interventions for undesired behavior (e.g., to teach an alternative behavior or new skills to animals). To this end, nonoverlap effect size calculations (Tau-U index) were conducted in the meta-analysis, since single-case research methods (SCRM) were exclusively implemented in the relevant literature. Based on the findings of the review and meta-analysis, potentially promising directions for future research are suggested.

2. Methods

This systematic and quantitative review followed the recommendations of Petticrew and Roberts [42] and complied with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [43,44]. A list of keywords for the systematic literature search were acquired by the “Pearl Harvesting Methodological Framework” (PHMF [45]). PHMF has been proven effective in locating the most relevant, inclusive keywords by creating a set of terms referred to as a synonym ring [45,46,47]. This strategy entails the following steps: (a) choosing a representative sample of articles; (b) extracting the relevant search keywords; (c) refining the list of search keywords; and (d) validating the essential search keywords [45,47].
A convenience sample consisting of 10 sources, including book chapters, systematic reviews, and research articles, was used for the keywords’ selection process [12,40,41,48]. The following keywords resulted from applying the PHMF and were used in combination with Boolean operators (where enabled) for database searches:
“clicker training” or “secondary reinforcers” or “secondary reinforcement” or “conditioned reinforcers” or “conditioned reinforcement” or “bridging stimulus” or “event marker” and “dogs” or “cats” or “parrots” or “cows” or “pigs” or “goats” or “horses” or “fish” or “sheep” or “primates” or “pinnipeds” or “cetaceans” and not “humans”
A wildcard, the asterisk (“*”), was used for searching databases and repositories that had such functions enabled.

2.1. Literature Search and Study Selection Process

Studies were located by systematically searching the following databases: SCOPUS, PsychINFO, Web of Knowledge, and EBSCO Open Dissertations. The latter repository was included to reduce the risk of biases in the dataset (e.g., publication bias, which refers to the issue of studies reporting statistically significant results being more likely to be published than studies with less favorable outcomes) [42]. A systematic review process was used to ensure consistency and transparency [43]. Study identification, screening, and eligibility evaluation were conducted using the PRISMA guidelines (see Figure 1) [44]. Methodological restrictions (e.g., between group designs only) and publication date limitations were not implemented. The resulting list of potential studies was screened against exclusion and inclusion criteria. First, the studies’ titles were scanned based on the clearly outlined criteria. If the titles appeared relevant to the proposed review, the abstracts of the studies were read. Second, the full texts of the remaining studies were scrutinized according to the same inclusion criteria to select for eligible papers. Studies were included, if they (a) were original research, including theses and dissertations; (b) were conducted in applied settings, e.g., domestic homes, shelters, stables, zoo enclosures, training facilities, or similar; (c) involved conditioned reinforcement (e.g., auditory, tactile, visual) as the primary independent variable; and (d) used concurrent or non-concurrent observation of animal behavior change (i.e., direct measurement of the effect of the independent variables on dependent variables).
Studies that successfully passed each phase of the screening process were included in the review and subsequent meta-analysis. Additionally, the bibliographies of eligible records and two previous reviews [40,41] were examined for studies not retrieved by database and repository searches. Finally, two journals that previously published studies on applied implementation of conditioned reinforcement (Applied Animal Behaviour Science and Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis) were hand-searched for relevant articles that met inclusion criteria. To summarize, the implemented search strategies and selection processes were conducted from September 2018 through March 2019 and yielded a total of 34 eligible studies.
To ensure the accuracy of relevance decisions throughout the screening process, a second examiner (DC) unaware of the aims of the review independently examined 35 out of 136 records’ abstracts (i.e., >25%) against exclusion and inclusion criteria. Studies appraised by the second examiner (DC) were randomly selected using the Microsoft® ExcelTM application “random function”. Each examiner’s (NPS and DC) agreements and disagreements of the selected records were compared, and an inter-examiner agreement (IEA) score was calculated by number of agreements divided by number of agreements plus number of disagreements multiplied by 100. IEA computation resulted in a 91% agreement score across both examiners. Any disagreements were resolved by discussion.

2.2. Assessment of Studies and Variable Coding

For coding the variables of interest, a specifically designed Microsoft® ExcelTM matrix was employed (see copy available at Open Science Framework (OSF), doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/V5MHF). All remaining 34 studies were coded on the following criteria: (a) bibliographic information (e.g., authors, publication date and type, and country); (b) sample size; (c) description of selection of sample (e.g., species of subjects); (d) information on procedures, such as if pairing sessions were conducted, if interstimulus intervals were measured, or details on the type and delivery of the unconditioned reinforcer (e.g., food delivered in a container; see Table 1 for definitions); (e) description of subjects’ target behavior (i.e., dependent variables, e.g., touching a cone with their muzzle); (f) description of the treatment (i.e., independent variables, e.g., click and food). Whenever more than one independent variable was implemented, additional rows were introduced to account for the different comparisons (see Table 2) [49]; (g) type of research design (e.g., between-group designs and single-case research methods); and (h) effect size indices (e.g., Tau-U for single-case research methods). If effect sizes were not given in the original studies, they were calculated by using data provided in the records.
To maintain consistency in coding, a second examiner (DC) independently coded 25% of all eligible articles (i.e., 9 out of 34). These studies were again randomly selected by employing the random function, a Microsoft® ExcelTM application. The inter-coder agreement score (ICA) was determined by applying the item-by-item calculation method (i.e., number of agreements divided by number of agreements plus number of disagreements multiplied by 100). These calculations were done for each of the 38 items (e.g., “setting, training location” or “independent” and “dependent variables”) and yielded an ICA of 92% across both coders. Any disagreements were resolved by discussion.
The overall study quality was assessed by a research assistant (DC) using a 14-item checklist adapted after Logan, Hickman, Harris, and Heriza [52]. To ensure consistency during the quality assessment process, another trained research assistant (LGM) assessed 30% of all eligible studies (i.e., 11 out of 34). The same random study selection approach and item-by-item computation method as before were used and yielded an agreement score of 97% across both reviewers. As previously, any disagreements were resolved by discussion.

2.3. Data Extraction and Meta-Statistics

Six out of seven SCRM studies reported suitable data for further analysis [9]. Given there were three or more studies allowing for comparison, they were combined according to research design (e.g., multiple-baseline across-subjects) and whether the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement was investigated or not (e.g., conditioned reinforcement was part of the intervention for an undesired behavior or to teach a new skill).
Data were extracted from graphs reported in the SCRM studies using the free DigitizeIt Version 2.4.0 [53] online software. A PNG image of each graph was pasted into the software and the coordinates, as well as data points, were plotted. The resulting digitized data of baseline and interventions for each AB contrast were exported to a Microsoft® ExcelTM file.
The Tau-U effect size index was computed for each study and for potential moderator variables. This index describes the “percent nonoverlapping data minus the percent of overlapping data” [54] (p. 285). It was selected as the index for the current analysis because it has been found to be consistent with visual analysis of SCRM data, has the ability to control for an undesired baseline trend (i.e., trend in the direction of the intervention or confounding direction), is robust enough for small data sets, and was shown to have greater statistical power and precision compared to other nonoverlap effect size indices [55,56]. Tau-U is interpretable as a continuous index of improvement and the published benchmarks should be considered relative to participants’ needs, intervention comparisons, and settings: 0 to 0.62 = small effect; 0.63 to 0.92 = medium effect; 0.93 to 1.00 = large effect [56,57]. The Tau-U indices were computed by entering baseline and intervention data into a free online Tau-U calculator [58]. An effect size value was obtained for each AB contrast (i.e., baseline versus intervention). These experiment-level effect sizes were then combined into one omnibus effect size per study by applying an inverse weighting computation that puts more weight on studies with more data points and stability [57].
During the next step, effect sizes were entered into the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis® software program (Version 3.3; [59]) to generate a summary effect size which represents all included studies. The summary effect size reflects a weighting computation that assigns relevance based on within-study and between-study variance [57].
Although neither a random nor fixed effects model is considered perfectly suitable for SCRM data, the assumed across-studies differences (e.g., subjects, outcome measures, procedures, and settings) made a random effects model the most appropriate [57]. In other words, the variance between studies was hypothesized to be due to systematic differences rather than sampling error, hence, implementation of a random effects model seemed to be appropriate for the current analysis [49,60].
Additionally, the application of a random effects model additionally allowed for assessment of the presence of moderator variables (i.e., covariates which may have an impact on the effect size; [60,61]). Moderator analyses were conducted by entering the Tau-U indices and respective standard errors for each AB contrast into the meta-statistic software as done previously in [59], yielding an effect size and additional statistical measures (e.g., standard error [SE] and p-values) for each potential moderator.

Heterogeneity Assessment

Heterogeneity or between-study variation is defined as the degree to which each study’s effect size varies within the dispersion (range) of effect sizes [61]. In other words, the variation described by heterogeneity cannot be accounted for by sampling error [62,63]. The Q- and I2-statistics were used to assess heterogeneity in this analysis [64]. Essentially, the Q-statistic is a test that assesses the null hypothesis stating that studies share a common effect size, with alpha typically set at 0.05, and p-values less than alpha leading to rejection of the null (i.e., studies do not share a common effect size [64]). The I2 index was calculated and can be described as the percentage of variance between studies [65]. A general guideline for the interpretation of the I2 index was put forward by Higgins and Green [62], stating that I2 > 75% should be considered substantial heterogeneity between study effects.

3. Results

The characteristics of all studies (n = 34) included in the systematic review are displayed in Table 2. The SCRM studies that were eligible for further meta-statistical analysis are highlighted in bold.

3.1. Systematic Review

3.1.1. Search Results and Study Characteristics

The searches yielded 136 sources from four databases and repositories, such as PsycInfo and SCOPUS. After completion of the selection process (Figure 1), 34 sources were found relevant for the current systematic review. Out of these 34 eligible studies published between 1980 and 2019 (Figure 2), the majority of studies (29%, n = 10) were published in the Applied Animal Behaviour Science journal using some type of group comparison design (“group design”; e.g., nonequivalent controlled or randomized controlled). The second largest category was theses (21%, n = 7), featuring mainly studies using SCRMs (12%, n = 4).
Overall, the majority of studies (59%, n = 20) used some type of group design, seven studies (21%) used SCRMs (e.g., multiple-baseline across-subjects design or reversal design), while another seven studies (21%) were categorized as case studies due to insufficient reporting of data and generally being more narrative in nature (see copy of respective table available at Open Science Framework (OSF), doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/V5MHF).

3.1.2. Main Findings of the Systematic Review

As shown in Table 3, almost half of all included studies (47%, n = 16) reported investigating the effects of conditioned reinforcement in dogs. Thirty-two percent (n = 11) of the studies with dogs used clickers as conditioned reinforcers and food (delivered by hand) as the unconditioned reinforcer. One study each (3%, n = 1) implemented either a spoken word and food delivered by hand or a whistle with food (presented in proximity) to the trainer. Horses followed dogs in number of studies (30%, n = 10). The majority of research with horses implemented pairings of clickers with food (24%, n = 8), with five studies (15%) delivering the food by hand and two studies (6%) presenting the food in a container. All other species (cats, cattle, fish, goats, and monkeys) were equally distributed across types of conditioned reinforcers with one study each (3%, n = 1).

Unconditioned Reinforcers (SR+) and Preference Assessments

Only two out of 34 studies (6%) conducted preference assessments (i.e., selection of alternative reinforcement options, where one alternative is selected more frequently leading to the identification of preference for a certain activity, food, or person [15,79]). One study used food delivered by hand with dogs [20], while the second study with foals used scratching as the unconditioned reinforcer [50]. To summarize, out of 30 studies using food as an unconditioned reinforcer with any form of delivery (e.g., by hand or in a container), the vast majority of studies (90%, n = 27) did not perform a preference assessment to identify their learner’s preferred reinforcers. Table 4 provides detailed information.

Schedules of Reinforcement

Learner performance depends on the schedules of reinforcement implemented; similar schedule effects have been found across organisms, types of responses, and a variety of reinforcers [15]. Two broad categories of schedules of reinforcement, namely continuous and intermittent, were coded in the identified studies. With continuous reinforcement, each response produces reinforcement (e.g., every response produces a click followed by food [27]). With an intermittent schedule of reinforcement, some responses are reinforced, while reinforcement is withheld on some occasions (i.e., conditioned or unconditioned reinforcers [80]). For instance, on a variable ratio 2 schedule, on average, every two responses produce an conditioned reinforcer followed by food [15,26]. The current review found that 85% (n = 29) of the studies delivered the unconditioned reinforcers on a continuous schedule of reinforcement, irrespective of whether a conditioned reinforcer was used, or only the unconditioned reinforcer was presented (e.g., click followed by food or food only). Overall, 24% (n = 8) of studies reported the implementation of intermittent schedules (e.g., a variable ratio schedule was implemented [26]), and 12% (n = 4) of studies did not clearly state the schedule used (see copy of respective figure available at Open Science Framework (OSF), doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/V5MHF).

Interstimulus-Intervals (ISI), Delays between Responses and Conditioned Reinforcers (R − Sr+), and Number of Pairings

The close temporal proximity between two events, i.e., contiguity, has been found to be important for stimulus–stimulus learning, and therefore has been deemed necessary for establishing a conditioned reinforcer [15,80]. Based on these methodological aspects of conditioned reinforcers, three different time intervals, as well as information on pairing sessions, were coded: (a) number of conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US) pairings (e.g., ≤20 pairings per day), (b) interstimulus interval (ISI) between CS and US during pairing sessions (i.e., subjects were only exposed to the stimuli, without reinforcement of a target response; Table 5), as well as during training (i.e., when the pairings of Sr+ and SR+ occurred naturally as part of the reinforcement of a target response), and (c) delay between target response and Sr+. Table 5, Table 6 and Table 7 report these findings in detail. Surprisingly, the majority of studies (44%, n = 15) did not clearly state whether pairings have been scheduled and the amount or the length of intervals if pairing was applied. Almost a third of the studies (29%, n = 10) reported that no explicit pairing procedure was conducted, followed by a number of studies that implemented a maximum of 20 pairings per day (27%, n = 9; Table 6).
With regard to the delay of the conditioned reinforcer (i.e., delay between the target response and the presentation of the conditioned reinforcer; R → Sr+), a third of the studies used five different descriptions (32%, n = 11), such as “immediately after the response occurred” or “shortly after”. The majority of studies (68%, n = 23), however, did not clearly state the delay between the target response and conditioned reinforcer presentation (Table 6).
Similarly, nonspecific statements have been found for reporting of the interval that elapsed between the conditioned reinforcement presentation (e.g., click; Sr+) and the delivery of the unconditioned reinforcers (e.g., food, water, or scratching; SR+). Although 15% (n = 5) of studies reported the delivery of the unconditioned reinforcer “immediately” after the conditioned reinforcer, more than half of all studies (59%, n = 20) did not clearly state the specific time (e.g., seconds) that elapsed between these two stimulus events (Table 7). To summarize, these findings lacked specific information about the measurement of interstimulus and response–reinforcer delay intervals across the applied animal training literature.

Positioning of the Experimenters and Location of the Unconditioned Reinforcers (SR+)

Based on the input received from renowned experts in the field (e.g., professional animal trainers and senior researchers), and some anecdotal observations reported in the literature [81], the positioning of the experimenter or trainer and the location of respective unconditioned reinforcers were coded (e.g., experimenter or trainer was located outside the view of the animal with food in close proximity to them but the food container not attached [23]). Table 8 displays this information by species. Consistent with the overall tendency to omit reporting of some features of the experimental set up and procedures identified so far, more than half of the studies (53%, n = 18) did not provide information on the positioning of experimenters and respective unconditioned reinforcers. Among the remaining studies, the majority described the experimenter being positioned in front of the learner (36%, n = 12), of which 24% (n = 8) were canine studies reporting reinforcers were delivered in front of the dog from a container attached to the body of the experimenter (e.g., treat pouch [76]). A less frequent variation of this arrangement (9%, n = 3) that was described in studies with fish, horses, and monkeys was the experimenter positioned in front of the learner and the reinforcer being delivered in proximity.

3.1.3. Meta-Statistical Results

For the current meta-analytic review, only SCRM designs were considered. This decision was reached because we were mainly interested in clicker training’s effectiveness within the behavioral intervention realm. Six out of seven studies reported suitable data for effect size calculations (included studies are highlighted in bold in Table 2). Three different species (dogs, horses, and goats), totaling 23 subjects across six studies, and encompassing 52 AB phase contrasts, were found. The majority of studies (66.6%, n = 4) used multiple-baseline designs (MBL), whereas one study applied either a reversal (RVD) or a changing criterion design (CCD; 33.3%). Small, medium, and large effects were equally distributed across all six studies, yielding two studies for each classification (33.3% each). Table 9 displays the studies’ characteristics and individual Tau-U effect sizes.

Effects of Clicker Training

Overall, Tau-U effect sizes for the use of clicker training to change an undesired behavior (e.g., separation-related responses in horses [23]) or to teach animals new responses (e.g., wearing head halters in goats [4]) ranged from 0.48 to 0.98, with a mean or summary effect size of Tau 0.77 (CI95% = [0.53, 0.89]) when compared to baseline levels. In other words, clicker training yielded a medium effect, regardless of animal species and type of target behavior (e.g., nose target, bowing, delivering object to hand, or slipping head into halter).
While five studies applied clicker training for teaching new skills to their animals on a continuous schedule only (i.e., each correct response resulted in click then food), Wennmacher [27] investigated the effect of click and continuous pairing of food versus click and intermittent presentation of food (i.e., each correct response resulted in a click but food was given only after every second click). Although small (Tau-U 0.48; CI95% = [0.33, 0.61]), the effect size indicates that continuous pairing with food was slightly more effective in teaching new responses to dogs than delivering food intermittently.
The majority of studies implemented MBLs, hence a separate comparison of these studies could be conducted, and a forest plot was created (Figure 3). These four studies were also those that implemented clicker training as part of their intervention to increase alternative behavior (e.g., trailer loading with horses or delivering a preferred item to hand with dogs). Overall, studies using MBLs yielded medium to large effects, with a summary effect size of Tau-U 0.90 (CI95% = [0.65, 1]). Put differently, MBL studies using clicker training as part of their behavior change interventions had a medium effect favoring the intervention.

Heterogeneity Assessment

Although the sample size of studies eligible for quantitative analysis was small, an assessment of how dissimilar the included studies are (i.e., heterogeneity assessment) was attempted. The heterogeneity assessment yielded a statistically significant result (p = 0.00), therefore the null hypothesis (i.e., all studies share a common effect size) could be rejected. Additional statistical results, namely Q = 23.83 and I2 = 79.02%, suggest that the studies are heterogeneous and that almost all of the variance may be explained by differences in study characteristics.

Moderator Analyses

Ten variables related to various study characteristics (e.g., type of conditioned reinforcer, amount of pairing sessions, or species) were examined for their potential impact on the effectiveness of clicker training. However, four variables could not be further analyzed due to insufficient data (e.g., type of conditioned and unconditioned reinforcers and schedule of reinforcement) or too small sample size (e.g., contiguity of conditioned and unconditioned reinforcers and prompts used). For the remaining six variables, moderator analyses were conducted by computing meta-regressions using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis® software [59].
Two of the potential moderators yielded statistically significant results (p < 0.05), namely learner species and study design (Table 10). These variables seemed to have influenced the effectiveness of clicker training in the current set of studies. The covariate horses yielded larger effect sizes than goats or dogs (0.943, 0.574, and 0.573, respectively). Studies implementing an MBL design had higher effects than studies using changing criterion or reversal designs (0.92, 0.34, and 0.44, respectively).
To summarize, the current analysis showed that clicker training resulted in mainly medium effects (range, 0.63 to 0.92) across a range of species (i.e., horses, dogs, and goats) and target responses taught (e.g., nose touches, spin and bow, and delivering a preferred item to hand). However, these results should be interpreted cautiously due to the very limited amount of studies that were available for inclusion in this quantitative analysis (n = 6).

4. Discussion

A total of 34 studies were found eligible after the selection process was completed (Figure 1). The majority of studies were undertaken with dogs and horses, using the sound of clickers paired with food. Studies that reported the use of conditioned reinforcement during behavioral intervention were chosen for further meta-statistical analyses. Six studies reported sufficient detail to be eligible for further analysis. These studies paired clickers with food, which yielded a medium effect when compared to baseline measures (summary effect of Tau-U 0.90; CI95% = [0.65, 1]; range from 0.63 to 0.92). These effects were found regardless of animal species (dogs or horses), type of target behavior taught during the behavior change intervention (e.g., nose target, delivering object to hand, or slipping head into a halter), and design used (e.g., MBL across-subjects design).

4.1. Systematic Review

This review found that the majority of eligible studies used some type of group design, whereas only a small number of studies implemented SCRMs. SCRMs are closely associated to the behavior analytic approach [82]. Thus, the small number of SCRM studies identified here further supports the notion that there is an overall lack of behavior-analytic research in the applied animal behavior change arena [83], including the application of conditioned reinforcement across applied settings and species. Two earlier reviews on the use of conditioned reinforcement in the animal behavior realm reached similar conclusions (e.g., clicker training [40,41]). This trend in the literature is surprising, as several authors have pointed out that reinforcement-based animal training, including conditioned reinforcement, is firmly rooted in behavior analysis [3,18,84,85], and has been shown to be successful in improving a wide range of socially relevant behaviors across settings and species (e.g., fear-related behavior in dogs and sheep [86,87,88]). Further investigation seems needed to explore and, hopefully improve, conceptual and methodological cohesiveness in the implementation of procedures with a behavior-analytic tradition in applied animal contexts [89,90].
The distribution of the identified studies across the different sources showed that the majority were either published in two clinical/applied journals (Applied Animal Behaviour Science and Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research) or were available as theses/dissertations. These findings suggest that most research related to conditioned reinforcement has had clinical/applied implications. Further, an important number of studies have not been published as journal articles. The latter finding seems to be aligned with previous reports across other disciplines about the proportion of these clinical/applied-oriented dissertations that do not get published in indexed journals [91]. An explanation of this situation goes beyond the scope of this review, but it has been noted that applied dissertations/theses often do not get published because the authors do not have expectations of careers in academia, where publications are valued [91,92].
Almost half of the identified studies were conducted with dogs, and the second most frequently studied subjects were horses; in both cases, clickers and food were the most frequently paired stimuli. The remaining studies focused on a wide range of species, including cats, goats, fish, cattle, and monkeys, and additional conditioned stimuli included beeps, whistle and buzzing sounds, and spoken words. Although the number of studies per these species and stimuli was limited, evidence shows promise of a wide generality of conditioned reinforcement phenomena and procedures across species, responses, and settings [30,93,94,95,96].
Regarding the type of unconditioned reinforcer (SR+) applied, the vast majority of studies used food. Out of these studies, only one study reported the implementation of some form of reinforcer preference assessment, though it was not systematic in nature (i.e., a variety of food rewards was presented to the owners and/or the dogs to choose from). This lack of preference assessment in the identified literature is an unexpected finding, since a growing body of literature has already highlighted the value of conducting preference assessments in animal behavior interventions across the settings, species, and stimuli examined (e.g., food or toys [79,97,98,99,100]). Overall, research shows that the delivery of a pre-established preferred stimulus as a reinforcer may improve the intervention outcome relative to the use of arbitrarily selected items with a presumed reinforcing function (e.g., assumed by the animals’ caregivers [79,99,101]).
Our findings have highlighted the lack of measurement and/or reporting of different temporal parameters, such as the ISI between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli [80] and delay between the target response and reinforcement [102]. This is important given the many formal and informal animal training and learning resources [3,103,104,105,106,107] that highlight the importance of contiguity between events on the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement in practice [15,80].
In the context of respondent (Pavlovian or classical) conditioning, contiguity is defined as the close temporal proximity between the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus during the conditioning procedure [15]. The use of this respondent procedure to “charge up the clicker” prior to the operant training of target responses is often mentioned in the animal training literature as a foundational part of clicker training with an inexperienced animal [106,107]. The extent to which the pairing is needed is itself an empirical question, not addressed in the literature identified for the present review. There does not seem to be a consensus regarding the number of pairings needed to establish the previously neutral stimulus as a conditioned stimulus [16,36,37,108,109]. Similar to Feng et al. [108], this review also found considerable variation in the number of pairings reported. For example, almost a quarter of the studies implemented a maximum of 20 pairings per day [28], fewer studies reported on up to 60 pairings per day [66]. Almost a third of the studies, however, did not report implementing extra pairings prior to the onset of the actual operant training phase [76]. This is in line with the findings of Feng et al. [108,110], who also found large variations in numbers of pairings reported across animal trainers working in the industry and applied animal behavior research papers. Taken together, these findings highlight the need for applied research that systematically manipulates, across species and settings, the number of pairings as an independent variable.
Lastly, two procedural aspects with a wide range of variation and lack of information across the relevant studies were the position of the experimenter/trainer, and the location of the unconditioned stimulus (SR+). More than half of the studies did not provide information on these two aspects. Among the remaining studies that did provide some information, the majority were canine studies that described the experimenter being positioned in front of the learner and reinforcers delivered in front of the dog from a container attached to the body of the experimenter. Similar to the temporal aspects of the presentation of the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, the spatial relation between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli seems to affect the establishment of the respondent conditioning [111]. For example, depending on the distance between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, pigeons may develop conditioned responses that include approaching the conditioned stimulus (sign tracking), approaching the site where the US is presented (goal tracking), or a combination of both sign and goal tracking [112]. The fact that several studies did not report information on the location of the experimenter and/or the place where the unconditioned stimulus was delivered suggests future research could explore how these factors affect the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcement procedures, including the emergence of unexpected responses (goal or sign tracking) that perhaps relate to spatial configurations of the conditioned and unconditioned stimulus.

4.2. Meta-Analysis

The present meta-analysis aimed to quantify the effectiveness of clicker training as part of interventions to change animal behavior in applied settings, which was not reported in the literature previously. All the studies of relevance to this aim implemented SCRMs, which most likely could be the result of these methods being best suited to experimentally assess behavior change at the individual level (i.e., the causal relations between one or more IVs and DVs [84,113]).
Overall, the Tau-U summary effect sizes demonstrated that the interventions using clicker training were effective in changing the learners’ behavior, resulting in small to large effect sizes (Tau-U range, 0.48–0.98, CI95% = [0.3, 1]), irrespective of their species and setting (e.g., dogs, goats, and horses in homes, enclosures, and stables, respectively). It has to be noted that the numerical benchmarks (e.g., 0 to 0.62 = small effect; 0.63 to 0.92 = medium effect; 0.93 to 1.00 = large effect [50,51]) should be interpreted contextually and with caution as small effects can result in important improvements on the learners’ behavior and welfare [11,56].
In agreement with published behavior-analytic research in the human behavior change arena [114,115,116,117], MBL across-subjects designs were the most frequently used type of SCRM, which yielded a medium summary effect size (Tau-U 0.90, CI95% = [0.65, 1]). A moderator analysis for MBL across-subjects designs (meta-regression; Table 10) found that this type of MBL design had an impact on the effectiveness of the interventions across all species. One factor contributing to this finding may be that the majority of analyzed studies used such designs (n = 4), while only one study implemented a changing criterion design, and another study used a reversal design. Although it is well established that MBL designs have several advantages compared to other SCRM designs, such as withdrawal of the intervention or reversal to baseline is not required [84,113], the results have to be interpreted carefully due to the overall small number of studies included in the analysis.
The meta-regressions computed for the analysis of potential effect moderators yielded two statistically significant results; namely, learner species and study design (Table 10). While the latter has been addressed in the MBL across-subjects design section above, the former is discussed in the remainder of this section. The variable learner species had moderating capabilities for the effectiveness of clicker training, yielding a larger effect for horses than for dogs and goats. Although the finding that learner species can influence the effectiveness of interventions is in line with an earlier meta-analysis investigating the effectiveness of caregiver training programs for both human and non-human animal learners [114], the current findings should be interpreted carefully. Half of the eligible studies used horses, while two studies used dogs and only one study featured goats. It is very likely that the overall small number of studies and the majority of studies using horses contributed to this outcome.

4.3. Limitations

There are several limitations which should be noted. First, dividing the dataset according to research designs was based on (a) the interest of analyzing studies aimed at changing behavior, which happened to use SCRMs, and (b) the lack of commonly accepted effect size indices that could be used to synthesize SCRM and group research [57]. Therefore, only a very small number of studies could be included in the meta-analysis, which makes the interpretation of results, especially the moderator analyses, tentative. Lastly, the application of Tau-U, or other nonparametric effect sizes (e.g., percentage nonoverlapping data; PND [118]), may lead to the loss of individual characteristics of behavior change patterns in SCRM data when averaged, which could make conclusions misleading [57,119].

5. Conclusions

Our quantitative review found that the majority of eligible studies were conducted with dogs and horses, and most studies applied clickers and food as conditioned and unconditioned reinforcers. While we identified conditioned reinforcement (i.e., clicker training) as an effective approach to change animal behavior, many of the variables that seem to affect its effectiveness could not be unambiguously retrieved from the eligible studies (e.g., contiguity and contingency). Hence, we identified several potential avenues for future research, such as systematically manipulating (a) time intervals or delays between response and the onset of the conditioned reinforcer or the end of the conditioned reinforcer and the delivery of the unconditioned stimulus, (b) type of conditioned reinforcers other than clickers and spoken words (e.g., whistles or beeps), and (c) types of unconditioned reinforcers or back-up reinforcers presented after the delivery of the conditioned reinforcer (e.g., different food items, play, or tactile interaction based on preference assessments [120,121]). Future research efforts could also focus on the replication of studies across different species (e.g., companion pigs or rodents) and settings (e.g., enclosures or domestic homes). These efforts may also provide a translational approach (i.e., the synthesis between basic and applied research leading to a bidirectional flow of information and influence [122,123]) to further the understanding of fundamental behavioral principles and processes [124].

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, N.P.-S., C.H.-P. and S.G.F.; methodology, N.P.-S., C.H.-P., D.C. and L.G.M.; software, N.P.-S.; validation, C.H.-P. and S.G.F.; formal analysis, N.P.-S., D.C. and L.G.M.; investigation, N.P.-S., C.H.-P., D.C. and L.G.M.; resources, N.P.-S., C.H.-P. and S.G.F.; data curation, N.P.-S., D.C. and L.G.M.; writing—original draft preparation, N.P.-S. and C.H.-P.; writing—review and editing, C.H.-P. and S.G.F.; visualization, N.P.-S. and C.H.-P.; supervision, C.H.-P. and S.G.F.; project administration, N.P.-S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was partially supported with funding for the APC provided by the Department of Psychology of Troy University, and Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz (grant number 9IN15151).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Diamond, J. Evolution, consequences, and future of plant and animal domestication. Nature 2002, 418, 700–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Cooper, J.O.; Heron, T.E.; Heward, W.L. Applied Behavior Analysis, 2nd ed.; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 176–224. [Google Scholar]
  3. Pryor, K.; Ramirez, K. Modern Animal Training A Transformative Technology. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Operant and Classical Conditioning, 1st ed.; Mcsweeney, E.S., Murphy, F.K., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2014; pp. 456–487. [Google Scholar]
  4. Fernandez, E.J. An 8-Step Program: Shaping and Fixed-Time Food Delivery Effects on Several Approximations and Undesired Responses in Goats. Master’s Thesis, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA, May 2003. [Google Scholar]
  5. Gillis, T.E.; Janes, A.C.; Kaufman, M.J. Positive reinforcement training in squirrel monkeys using clicker training. Am. J. Primatol. 2012, 74, 712–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Grant, R.A.; Warrior, J.R. Clicker training increases exploratory behaviour and time spent at the front of the enclosure in shelter cats: Implications for welfare and adoption rates. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 211, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Mirwan, H.B.; Mason, G.J.; Kevan, P.G. Complex operant learning by worker bumblebees (Bombus impatiens): Detour behaviour and use of colours as discriminative stimuli. Insectes Sociaux 2015, 62, 365–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Medina, L.G.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. It is mine! Using clicker training as a treatment of object guarding in 4 companion dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2017, 22, 57–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Slater, C.; Dymond, S. Using differential reinforcement to improve equine welfare: Shaping appropriate truck loading and feet handling. Behav. Process. 2011, 86, 329–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hendriksen, P.; Elmgreen, K.; Ladewig, J. Trailer-loading of horses: Is there a difference between positive and negative reinforcement concerning effectiveness and stress-related signs? J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2011, 6, 261–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ziv, G. The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2017, 19, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Ramirez, K. Basic Operant Conditioning. In Animal Training: Successful Animal Management through Positive Reinforcement, 1st ed.; Shedd Aquarium: Chicago, IL, USA, 1999; pp. 65–111. [Google Scholar]
  13. Pavlov, I.P.; Gantt, W.H. Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes: Twenty-Five Years of Objective Study of the Higher Nervous Activity (Behaviour) of Animals; Gantt, W.H., Ed.; Liverwright Publishing Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Skinner, B.F. The Behavior of Organisms; B.F. Skinner Foundation: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1938. [Google Scholar]
  15. Pierce, W.D.; Cheney, C.D. Behavior Analysis and Learning, 6th ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 1–613. [Google Scholar]
  16. Skinner, B.F. How to Teach Animals. Sci. Am. 1951, 185, 26–29. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1952-04212-001 (accessed on 8 July 2020). [CrossRef]
  17. Roddy, J. Harvard-trained dog. LOOK, 20 May 1952; pp. 17–20. [Google Scholar]
  18. Burch, M.R.; Bailey, J.S. How Dogs Learn; Wiley Publishing, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 1–177. [Google Scholar]
  19. Pryor, K. History of Clicker Training I. 2013. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/node/153 (accessed on 9 March 2020).
  20. Chiandetti, C.; Avella, S.; Fongaro, E.; Cerri, F. Can clicker training facilitate conditioning in dogs? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 184, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Feng, L.C.; Hodgens, N.H.; Woodhead, J.K.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. Is clicker training (clicker + food) better than food-only training for novice companion dogs and their owners? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 204, 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ferguson, D.L.; Rosales-Ruiz, J. Loading the problem loader: The effects of target training and shaping on trailer-loading behavior of horses. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2001, 34, 409–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Rybova, V. Target Training as a Differential Reinforcement Intervention for Separation-Induced Challenging Behaviour in Horses. Master’s Thesis, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  24. Lansade, L.; Calandreau, L.A. conditioned reinforcer did not help to maintain an operant conditioning in the absence of a primary reinforcer in horses. Behav. Process. 2018, 146, 61–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. McCall, C.A.; Burgin, S.E. Equine utilization of secondary reinforcment during response extinction and acquisition. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 253–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Smith, S.M.; Davis, E.S. Clicker increases resistance to extinction but does not decrease training time of a simple operant task in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 110, 318–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Wennmacher, P.L. Effects of Click + Continuous Food versus Click + Intermittent Food on the Maintenance of Dog Behavior. Master’s Thesis, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA, May 2007. [Google Scholar]
  28. Willson, E.K.; Stratton, R.B.; Bolwell, C.F.; Stafford, K.J. Comparison of positive reinforcement training in cats: A pilot study. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2017, 21, 64–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Wood, L. Clicker Bridging Stimulus Efficacy; Hunter College: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  30. Fernandez, E.J.; Dorey, N.R. An examination of shaping with an African Crested Porcupine (Hystrix cristata). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Johnston, J.M.; Pennypacker, H.S. Strategies and Tactics of Behavioral Research, 3rd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  32. D’Onofrio, J.M. Measuring The Efficiency of Clicker Training for Service Dogs. Master’s Thesis, Pennsylvania State University, Pennsylvania, PA, USA, December 2015. Available online: https://etda.libraries.psu.edu/files/final_submissions/11557 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
  33. Häderer, I.K.; Michiels, N.K. Successful operant conditioning of marine fish in their natural habitat. Copeia 2016, 104, 380–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Williams, J.L.; Friend, T.H.; Nevill, C.H.; Archer, G. The efficacy of a secondary reinforcer (clicker) during acquisition and extinction of an operant task in horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004, 88, 331–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Dozier, C.L.; Iwata, B.A.; Thomason-Sassi, J.; Worsdell, A.S.; Wilson, D.M. A comparison of two pairing procedures to establish praise as a reinforcer. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2012, 45, 721–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Williams, B.A. Conditioned Reinforcement: Experimental and Theoretical Issues. Behav. Anal. 1994, 17, 261–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Williams, B.A. Conditioned reinforcement: Neglected or outmoded explanatory construct? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1994, 1, 457–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Martin, S.; Friedman, S.G. Blazing Clickers. Available online: http://www.behaviorworks.org/files/journals/Blazing%20Clickers.pdf (accessed on 23 September 2020).
  39. Fernandez, E.J. Click or Treat: A trick or two in the zoo. Am. Anim. Train. Mag. 2001, 2, 41–44. [Google Scholar]
  40. Dorey, N.R.; Cox, D.J. Function matters: A review of terminological differences in applied and basic clicker training research. PeerJ 2018, 6, e5621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Feng, L.C.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. How clicker training works: Comparing Reinforcing, Marking, and Bridging Hypotheses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 181, 34–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Petticrew, M.; Roberts, H. Systematic Reviews in the Social Sciences: A Practical Guide; Blackwell Publishing: Malden, MA, USA, 2006; pp. 1–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Liberati, A.; Altman, D.G.; Tetzlaff, J.; Mulrow, C.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Ioannidis, J.P.A.; Clarke, M.; Devereaux, P.J.; Kleijnen, J.; Moher, D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. The PRISMA Group. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Sandieson, R.W.; Kirkpatrick, L.C.; Sandieson, R.M.; Zimmerman, W. Harnessing the power of education research databases with the pearl-harvesting methodological framework for information retrieval. J. Spec. Educ. 2010, 44, 161–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Morville, P.; Rosenfeld, L. Information Architecture for the World Wide Web, 3rd ed.; O’Reilly Media, Inc.: Sebastopol, CA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  47. Sandieson, R.W. Pathfinding in the Research Forest: The Pearl Harvesting Method for Effective Information Retrieval. Educ. Train. Dev. Disabil. 2006, 41, 401–409. Available online: https://0-www-jstor-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/stable/23879666 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
  48. McGreevy, P.D.; Boakes, R. Learning Theory and Positive Reinforcement. In Carrots and Sticks: Principles of Animal Training; McGreevy, P.D., Boakes, R., Eds.; Sydney University Press: Sydney, Australia, 2011; pp. 24–64. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lipsey, M.W.; Wilson, D.B. Practical Meta-Analysis, 1st ed.; Sage Publications Ltd.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2001; pp. 1–247. [Google Scholar]
  50. Martinez de Andino, E.V.; McDonnell, S.M. Evaluation of operant learning in young foals using target training. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 193, 67–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Langbein, J.; Siebert, K.; Nuernberg, G.; Manteuffel, G. The impact of acoustical secondary reinforcement during shape discrimination learning of dwarf goats (Capra hircus). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 103, 35–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Logan, L.R.; Hickman, R.R.; Harris, S.R.; Heriza, C.B. Single-subject research design: Recommendations for levels of evidence and quality rating. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 50, 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Bormann, I. DigitizeIt: Digitizer Software-Digitize Scanned Graph or Chart into (x,y)-Data. Available online: https://www.digitizeit.de (accessed on 20 May 2019).
  54. Parker, R.I.; Vannest, K.J.; Davis, J.L.; Sauber, S.B. Combining nonoverlap and trend for Single-Case Research: Tau-U. Behav. Ther. 2011, 42, 284–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Parker, R.I.; Vannest, K.J.; Davis, J.L. Effect size in single-case research: A review of nine nonoverlap techniques. Behav. Modif. 2011, 35, 303–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Vannest, K.J.; Ninci, J. Evaluating intervention effects in single-case research designs. J. Couns. Dev. 2015, 93, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Morin, K.L.; Ganz, J.B.; Vannest, K.J.; Haas, A.N.; Nagro, S.A.; Peltier, C.J.; Ura, S.K. A Systematic review of Single-Case Research on video analysis as professional development for special educators. J. Spec. Educ. 2019, 53, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Vannest, K.J.; Parker, R.I.; Gonen, O.; Adiguzel, T. Single-Case Research: Web-Based Calculators for SCR Analysis (Version 2.0) [Web-Based Application]; Texas A&M University: College Station, TX, USA, 2016; Available online: https://www.singlecaseresearch.org (accessed on 10 September 2019).
  59. Biostat, Inc. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis©, version 3.0; Computer Software; Biostat, Inc.: Englewood, NJ, USA, 2019; Available online: https://www.metaanalysis.com/index.php?cart=BMNJ1648138 (accessed on 12 June 2019).
  60. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. Regression in Meta-Analysis. 2015. Available online: https://www.meta-analysis.com/downloads/MRManual.pdf (accessed on 9 January 2020).
  61. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. Introduction to Meta-Analysis; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  62. Higgins, J.P.; Green, S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions; The Cochrane Collaboration: London, UK, 2011; Available online: http://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org (accessed on 25 January 2020).
  63. Littell, J.H.; Corcoran, J.; Pillai, V. Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 1–193. [Google Scholar]
  64. Borenstein, M.; Higgins, J.P.T.; Hedges, L.V.; Rothstein, H.R. Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Res. Synth. Methods 2017, 8, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Higgins, J.P.T.; Thompson, S.G. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1539–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Batt, L.; Batt, M.; Baguley, J.; McGreevy, P. The effects of structured sessions for juvenile training and socialization on guide dog success and puppy-raiser participation. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2008, 3, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Blandina, A.G. To Click or Not to Click: The Impact of Positive Reinforcement Methods on the Acquisition of Behavior. Bachelor’s Thesis, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  68. Fernström, A.-L.; Fredlund, H.; Spangberg, M.; Westlund, K. Positive reinforcement training in Rhesus Macaques—Training progress as a result of training frequency. Am. J. Primatol. 2009, 71, 373–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Fjellanger, R.; Andersen, E.K.; McLean, I.G. A Training Program for Filter-Search Mine Detection Dogs. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2002, 15, 278–287. Available online: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9xw413g4 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
  70. Flannery, B. Relational discrimination learning in horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1997, 54, 267–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Flynn, K.K. Conditioned Reinforcement with an Equine Subject. Master’s Thesis, North Texas State University, Denton, TX, USA, May 1980. [Google Scholar]
  72. Fugazza, C.; Miklosi, Á. Social learning in dog training: The effectiveness of the Do as I do method compared to shaping/clicker training. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Guerrero-Flores, H.; Apresa-Garcia, T.; Garay-Villar, O.; Sanchez-Perez, A.; Flores-Villegas, D.; Bandera-Calderon, A.; Garcia-Palacios, R.; Rojas-Sanchez, T.; Romero-Morelos, P.; Sanches-Albor, V.; et al. A non-invasive tool for detecting cervical cancer odor by trained scent dogs. BMC Cancer 2017, 17, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Meyer, I.; Ladewig, J. The relationship between number of training sessions per week and learning in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 111, 311–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Strychalski, J.; Gugołek, A.; Konstantynowicz, M. Clicker Training Efficiency in Shaping the Desired Behaviour in the Following Dog Breeds: Boxer, Chow Chow and Yorkshire Terrier. Pol. J. Nat. Sci. 2015, 30, 235–243. Available online: http://www.uwm.edu.pl/polish-journal/sites/default/files/issues/articles/strychalski_et_al_2015_0.pdf (accessed on 8 July 2020).
  76. Thorn, J.M.; Templeton, J.J.; Van Winkle, K.M.M.; Castillo, R.R. Conditioning shelter dogs to sit. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2006, 9, 25–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Whistance, L.K.; Sinclair, L.A.; Arney, D.R.; Phillips, C.J.C. Trainability of eliminative behaviour in dairy heifers using a secondary reinforcer. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 117, 128–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Willis, C.; Church, S.M.; Guest, C.M.; Cook, W.A.; McCarthy, N.; Bransbury, A.J.; Church, M.R.T.; Church, J.C.T. Olfactory detection of human bladder cancer by dogs: Proof of principle study. BMJ 2004, 329, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Fernandez, E.J.; Dorey, N.J.; Rosales-Ruiz, J. A two-choice preference assessment with five cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2004, 7, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Chance, P. Learning and Behavior; Wadsworth Thomson Learning: Belmont, CA, USA, 2013; pp. 1–442. [Google Scholar]
  81. Guest, T. How to Practice Clicker Mechanics. 2012. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/how-to-practice-clicker-mechanics (accessed on 5 May 2020).
  82. Hurtado-Parrado, C.; López-López, W. Single-Case Research Methods: History and Suitability for a Psychological Science in Need of Alternatives. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2015, 49, 323–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Arnott, G.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. Using principles from applied behaviour analysis to address an undesired behaviour: Functional analysis and treatment of jumping up in companion dogs. Animals 2019, 9, 1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  84. Alligood, C.A.; Dorey, N.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Leighty, K.A. Applying behavior-analytic methodology to the science and practice of environmental enrichment in zoos and aquariums. Zoo Biol. 2017, 36, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Farhoody, P. Behavior Analysis. The Science of Training. Vet. Clin. N. Am.-Exot. Anim. Pract. 2012, 15, 361–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Fernandez, E.J. Training petting zoo sheep to act like petting zoo sheep: An empirical evaluation of response-independent schedules and shaping with negative reinforcement. Animals 2020, 10, 1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Feuerbacher, E.N.; Muir, K.L. Using owner return as a reinforcer to operantly treat separation-related problem behavior in dogs. Animals 2020, 10, 1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Protopopova, A.; Kisten, D.; Wynne, C. Evaluating a humane alternative to the bark collar: Automated differential reinforcement of not barking in a home-alone setting. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2016, 49, 735–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Poling, A.; Weetjens, B.; Cox, C.; Beyene, N.; Durgin, A.; Mahoney, A. Tuberculosis detection by Giant African Pouched Rats. Behav. Anal. 2011, 34, 47–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Schlinger, H.D. The Importance of Analysis in Applied Behavior Analysis. Behav. Anal. Res. Pract. 2017, 17, 334–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Mayir, B.; Bilecik, T.; Çakir, T.; Doǧan, U.; Gündüz, U.R.; Aslaner, A.; Oruç, M.T. Analysis of the publishing rate and the number of citations of general surgery dissertations. Turk. J. Surg. 2017, 33, 33–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Evans, S.C.; Amaro, C.M.; Herbert, R.; Blossom, J.B.; Roberts, M.C. “Are you gonna publish that?” Peer-reviewed publication outcomes of doctoral dissertations in psychology. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  93. Branch, M.N.; Pennypacker, H.S. Generality And Generalization of Research Findings. In APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis; Madden, G.J., Ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Volume 1, pp. 151–175. [Google Scholar]
  94. Johnston, J.M. On the relation between generalization and generality. Behav. Anal. 1979, 2, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Dorey, N.R.; Blandina, A.; Udell, M.A.R. Clicker training does not enhance learning in mixed-breed shelter puppies (Canis familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. 2020, 39, 57–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Fernandez, E.J.; Rosales-Ruiz, J. A comparison of fixed-time food schedules and shaping involving a clicker for halter behavior in a petting zoo goat. Psychol. Rec. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Martin, A.L.; Franklin, A.N.; Perlman, J.E.; Bloomsmith, M.A. Systematic assessment of food item preference and reinforcer effectiveness: Enhancements in training laboratory-housed rhesus macaques. Behav. Process. 2018, 157, 445–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Protopopova, A.; Brandifino, M.; Wynne, C.D.L. Preference assessments and structured potential adopter-dog interactions increase adoptions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 176, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Vicars, S.M.; Miguel, C.F.; Sobie, J.L. Assessing preference and reinforcer effectiveness in dogs. Behav. Process. 2014, 103, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Vitale-Shreve, K.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Udell, M.A.R. Social interaction, food, scent or toys? A formal assessment of domestic pet and shelter cat (Felis silvestris catus) preferences. Behav. Process. 2017, 141, 322–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Gaalema, D.E.; Perdue, B.M.; Kelling, A.S. Food preference, keeper ratings, and reinforcer effectiveness in exotic animals: The value of systematic testing. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2011, 14, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Gallistel, C.R.; Craig, A.R.; Shahan, T.A. Contingency, contiguity, and causality in conditioning: Applying Information Theory and Weber’s Law to the assignment of credit problem. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 126, 761–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. McGreevy, P.D. Learning. In Equine Behavior: A Guide for Veterinarians and Equine Scientists, 2nd ed.; McGreevy, P.D., Ed.; Saunders Elsevier: Sydney, Australia, 2012; pp. 83–117. [Google Scholar]
  104. Mills, D.S. Training And Learning Protocols. In BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Behaviour, 3rd ed.; Horwitz, D.F., Mills, D.S., Eds.; British Small Animal Veterinary Association: Quedgeley, UK, 2010; pp. 49–64. [Google Scholar]
  105. Ramirez, K. Is a Clicker Necessary? 2017. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/is-a-clicker-necessary (accessed on 5 May 2020).
  106. Pryor, K.W. Charging the Clicker. 2006. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/node/824 (accessed on 5 May 2020).
  107. Ryan, T.; Mortensen, K. Outwitting Dogs: Revolutionary Techniques for Dog Training That Work! The Lyons Press: Guildford, CT, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  108. Feng, L.C.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. Comparing trainers’ reports of clicker use to the use of clickers in applied research studies: Methodological differences may explain conflicting results. Pet Behav. Sci. 2017, 3, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Pryor, K.W. Getting Started: Clicker Training for Dogs; Sunshine Books, Inc.: Waltham, MA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  110. Feng, L.C.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. Practices and perceptions of clicker use in dog training: A survey-based investigation of dog owners and industry professionals. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2018, 23, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Pear, J.J. The Science of Learning, 2nd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 1–489. [Google Scholar]
  112. Silva, F.J.; Silva, K.M.; Pear, J.J. Sign- versus goal-tracking: Effects of conditioned-stimulus- to-unconditioned-stimulus distance. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1992, 57, 17–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  113. Ledford, J.R.; Barton, E.E.; Severini, K.E.; Zimmerman, K.N. A primer on Single-Case Research Designs: Contemporary use and analysis. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2019, 124, 35–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Medina, L.; Dillenburger, K.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. We don’t train in vain: A systematic review and meta-analysis of human and canine caregiver training. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2019, 23, 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Hammond, D.; Gast, D.L. Descriptive Analysis of Single Subject Research Designs: 1983–2007. Educ. Train. Autism Dev. Disabil. 2010, 45, 187–202. Available online: https://0-www-jstor-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/stable/23879806 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
  116. Ledford, J.R.; King, S.; Harbin, E.R.; Zimmerman, K.N. Antecedent Social Skills Interventions for Individuals with ASD: What Works, for Whom, and Under What Conditions? Focus Autism Other Dev. Disabil. 2018, 33, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  117. Pustejovsky, J.E.; Hedges, L.V. Design-comparable effect sizes in Multiple Baseline Designs: A general modeling framework. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 2014, 39, 368–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Scruggs, T.E.; Mastopieri, M.A.; Casto, G. The quantitative synthesis of Single-Subject Research: Methodology and validation. Remedial Spec. Educ. 1987, 8, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Burns, M.K.; Zaslofsky, A.F.; Kanive, R.; Parker, D.C. Meta-Analysis of Incremental Rehearsal Using Phi Coefficients to Compare Single-Case and Group Designs. J. Behav. Educ. 2012, 21, 185–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D.L. Shut up and pet me! Domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single-alternative choice procedures. Behav. Process. 2015, 110, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D.L. Most domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer food to petting: Population, context, and schedule effects in concurrent choice. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2014, 101, 385–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Hake, D.F. The basic-applied continuum and the possible evolution of human operant social and verbal research. Behav. Anal. 1982, 5, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. McIlvane, W.J. Translational behavior analysis: From laboratory science in stimulus control to intervention with persons with neurodevelopmental disabilities. Behav. Anal. 2009, 32, 273–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Dube, W.V. Translational Research in Behavior Analysis. In APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis; Madden, G.J., Dube, W.V., Hackenberg, T.D., Hanley, G.P., Lattal, K.A., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; pp. 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the separate stages during the selection process for identification of studies eligible for further analysis (adapted after PRISMA guidelines [43,44]).
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the separate stages during the selection process for identification of studies eligible for further analysis (adapted after PRISMA guidelines [43,44]).
Animals 10 01757 g001
Figure 2. Numbers of publications per year across all studies included in the review.
Figure 2. Numbers of publications per year across all studies included in the review.
Animals 10 01757 g002
Figure 3. Forest plot featuring the studies MBL studies with respective Tau-U effect sizes. The diamond-shaped data point at the bottom of the plot represents the mean or summary effect size across all four studies. Note: BSL = baseline; INT = intervention.
Figure 3. Forest plot featuring the studies MBL studies with respective Tau-U effect sizes. The diamond-shaped data point at the bottom of the plot represents the mean or summary effect size across all four studies. Note: BSL = baseline; INT = intervention.
Animals 10 01757 g003
Table 1. Details on the delivery of the SR+ and respective definitions as reported in the eligible studies.
Table 1. Details on the delivery of the SR+ and respective definitions as reported in the eligible studies.
Details on SR+ DeliveryDefinitions
Food by handFood was brought to the animals’ muzzles by hand (e.g., dogs, and horses) or was presented by hand in a manner that the animals could reach for the SR+ with a forelimb (e.g., rhesus macaques or squirrel monkeys).
Food remotelyFood was delivered through an automated feeder which was remotely controlled by the experimenter.
Food containerFood was made available to the animal in a container (e.g., feedbox for horses or a food bowl for dogs).
Food proximityThe food was consistently delivered in the same place in close proximity to the experimenter (e.g., after the animal performed the task, the experimenter delivered the conditioned reinforcer and placed the food near to him/her on the floor [20]).
ScratchingScratching (e.g., chest neck or rump) was used as an alternative SR+ to food for foals because they were still nursing during the experiment [50].
WaterWater was available as an SR+ in a computer-controlled learning device located in a separate compartment of the home pen (stable) of the goats. The animals had 24 h access to the device, but only one goat could enter it at a time [51].
No SR+Only the Sr+ (conditioned reinforcer) was presented or reinforcement was entirely withheld (i.e., extinction).
Not clearly statedType of SR+ and mode of delivery were not clearly stated.
Table 2. Characteristics of all eligible studies.
Table 2. Characteristics of all eligible studies.
Doc IDStudy (Year)CountryPublication TypeLearner SpeciesLearner SexLearner AgeType of Conditioned ReinforcerSetting, Training LocationType of StudyLearner Target BehaviorDependent VariablesIndependent Variables
1/1Batt et al. (2008) [66]AustraliaPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% male intact≤4 monthsClickerCommunity hallGroup designAttention, leave it, stand, loose-leash walkingRe-adoption of subsequent puppiesConditioned Reinforcement and food
1/2Batt et al. (2008) [66]AustraliaPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% male intact≤4 monthsNot applicableCommunity hallGroup designAttention, leave it, stand, loose-leash walkingRe-adoption of subsequent puppiesControl (treatment as usual)
2/1Blandina (2010) [67]USABachelor thesisDog (Canis familiaris)Not clearly stated≤6 monthsClickerShelter roomGroup designStayDurationConditioned Reinforcement and food
2/2Blandina (2010) [67]USABachelor thesisDog (Canis familiaris)Not clearly stated≤6 monthsSpoken wordShelter roomGroup designStayDurationConditioned Reinforcement and food
2/3Blandina (2010) [67]USABachelor thesisDog (Canis familiaris)Not clearly stated≤6 monthsNot applicableShelter roomGroup designStayDurationFood only
3/1Chiandetti et al. (2016) [20]ItalyPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% female intact1–5 yearsClickerHomeGroup designOpen box by pushing up the lid with noseNumber of attempts over timeConditioned Reinforcement and food
3/2Chiandetti et al. (2016) [20]ItalyPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% female intact1–5 yearsSpoken wordHomeGroup designOpen box by pushing up the lid with noseNumber of attempts over timeConditioned Reinforcement and food
3/3Chiandetti et al. (2016) [20]ItalyPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% female intact1–5 yearsNot applicableHomeGroup designOpen box by pushing up the lid with noseNumber of attempts over timeFood only
4/1D’Onofrio (2015) [32]USAMaster’s thesisDog (Canis familiaris)50% female1–9 yearsClickerHomeSingle-case methodRetrieve medicine bag and pick up walletFrequencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
5/1Feng et al. (2018) [21]AustraliaPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% male neutered1–5 yearsClickerHomeGroup designSpin, hand target, object target, on your matFrequencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
5/3Feng et al. (2018) [21]AustraliaPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% male neutered1–5 yearsNot applicableHomeGroup designSpin, hand target, object target, on your matFrequencyControl (waiting list)
6/1Ferguson et al. (2001) [22]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Female intact5–18 yearsClickerOutdoor enclosureSingle-case methodNose touch to a cloth potholderLatencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
6/2Ferguson et al. (2001) [22]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Female intact5–18 yearsClickerOutdoor enclosureSingle-case methodNose touch to a cloth potholderParts of horse enteringConditioned Reinforcement and food
7/1Fernandez (2003) [4]USAMaster’s thesisGoat (Capra hircus)Male neutered<12 monthsClickerOutdoor enclosureSingle-case methodSlipping head into halterShaping steps completedConditioned Reinforcement and food
8/1Fernström et al. (2009) [68]SwedenPeer-reviewed paperRhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta)Female intact1–5 yearsClickerIndoor enclosureGroup designTarget, cooperative behavior, box and injectionsNumber of sessions to completionTreatment intensities
9/1Fjellanger et al. (2002) [69]NorwayPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)>50% male intact1–5 yearsWhistleTraining facilityCase studyScent discrimination (explosives)Proportion correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and INT food
10/1Flannery (1997) [70]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Not clearly stated12–24 yearsClickerStableCase studyTouching labeled stimulus cards (discrimination task)Percent correct trialsConditioned Reinforcement and food
10/2Flannery (1997) [70]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Not clearly stated12–24 yearsClickerStableCase studyHigher-order discrimination taskPercent correct trialsConditioned Reinforcement and INT food
11/1Flynn (1980) [71]USAMaster’s thesisHorse (Equus caballus)Female intact>15 yearsClickerStableGroup designWalk around cone, open mouth, stepping diagonally, still for 60 sec, noddingCount correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and INT food
12/1Fugazza et al. (2015) [72]ItalyPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)Not clearly stated1–12 yearsClickerTraining facilityGroup designOpen a sliding door, jumping in the airLatencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
12/2Fugazza et al. (2015) [72]ItalyPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)Not clearly stated1–12 yearsNot applicableTraining facilityGroup designOpen a sliding door, jumping in the airLatencyModeling the response
13/1Gillis et al. (2012) [5]USAPeer-reviewed paperSquirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis)Male intact1–5 yearsClickerLaboratoryCase studyTarget and duration training, glove desensitization, chain and pole, injection trainingTime to criterionConditioned Reinforcement and food
14/1Grant et al. (2019) [6]UKPeer-reviewed paperCat (Cattus syslvestris)>50% male neuteredNot clearly statedClickerShelter roomGroup designDuration exploratory behaviorDurationConditioned Reinforcement and food
15/1Guerrero-Flores et al. (2017) [73]MexicoPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)Male intact1–5 yearsClickerLaboratoryCase studyIndicate target odor by sitting in front of sampleFrequencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
16/1Häderer et al. (2016) [33]FrancePeer-reviewed paperFish (Tripterygion tripteronotum)MixedNot clearly statedClickerNatural environment/home rangeGroup designTarget black/white chip with muzzleCount correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and food
16/2Häderer et al. (2016) [33]FrancePeer-reviewed paperFish (Tripterygion tripteronotum)MixedNot clearly statedNot applicableNatural environment/home rangeGroup designTarget black/white chip with muzzleCount correct responsesFood only
17/1Hendriksen et al. (2011) [10]DenmarkPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Mixed7–20 yearsClickerStableGroup designStepping into trailer on cueTime to criterionConditioned Reinforcement and food
17/2Hendriksen et al. (2011) [10]DenmarkPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Mixed7–20 yearsNot applicableStableGroup designStepping into trailer on cueTime to criterionNegative reinforcement
18/1Langbein et al. (2007) [51]GermanyPeer-reviewed paperGoat (Capra hircus)Male intact≤12 monthsBeepStableGroup designShape discriminationNumber of sessions to completionConditioned Reinforcement and water
18/2Langbein et al. (2007) [51]GermanyPeer-reviewed paperGoat (Capra hircus)Male intact≤12 monthsNot applicableStableGroup designShape discriminationNumber of sessions to completionControl (water only)
19/1Lansade et al. (2018) [24]FrancePeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)>50% female intact≤12 monthsSpoken wordStableGroup designNose touch coneCount correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and food
19/2Lansade et al. (2018) [24]FrancePeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)>50% female intact≤12 monthsNot applicableStableGroup designNose touch coneCount correct responsesFood only
20/1McCall et al. (2002) [25]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Not clearly stated6 months–15 yearsBuzzing soundStableGroup designPushing a leverMean training timeConditioned Reinforcement and food (Phase I)
20/2McCall et al. (2002) [25]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Not clearly stated6 months–15 yearsNot applicableStableGroup designPushing a leverMean training timeFood only (Phase I)
21/1Meyer et al. (2008) [74]DenmarkPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)50% female1–5 yearsClickerLaboratoryGroup designPaw target mouse padNumber of sessions to completionTreatment intensities
21/2Meyer et al. (2008) [74]DenmarkPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)50% female1–5 yearsClickerLaboratoryGroup designPaw target mouse padNumber of sessions to completionTreatment intensities
22/1Pfaller-Sadovsky et al. (2017) [8]AustriaPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)50% female1–9 yearsClickerHomeSingle-case methodDelivering PI to handPercent trialsConditioned Reinforcement and food
23/1Smith et al. (2008) [26]USAPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)Mixed1–9 yearsClickerHomeGroup designNose touch coneLatencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
23/2Smith et al. (2008) [26]USAPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)Mixed1–9 yearsClickerHomeGroup designNose touch coneLatencyConditioned Reinforcement and INT food
23/3Smith et al. (2008) [26]USAPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)Mixed1–9 yearsNot applicableHomeGroup designNose touch coneLatencyControl (food only)
24/1Strychalski et al. (2015) [75]PolandPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)50% female>4 yearsClickerHomeGroup designAround conesNumber of sessions to completionConditioned Reinforcement and food
25/1Thorn et al. (2006) [76]USAPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)50% femaleNot clearly statedSpoken wordShelter roomCase studySit (Experiment 1)LatencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
25/2Thorn et al. (2006) [76]USAPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)50% femaleNot clearly statedSpoken wordOutdoor enclosureGroup designSit (Experiment 2)LatencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
25/3Thorn et al. (2006) [76]USAPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)50% femaleNot clearly statedClickerOutdoor enclosureGroup designSit (Experiment 2)LatencyConditioned Reinforcement and food
26/1Wennmacher (2007) [27]USAMaster’s thesisDog (Canis familiaris)Mixed1–5 yearsClickerHomeSingle-case methodSpin, bowCount correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and food
26/2Wennmacher (2007) [27]USAMaster’s thesisDog (Canis familiaris)Mixed1–5 yearsClickerHomeSingle-case methodSpin, bowCount correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and INT food
27/1Whistance et al. (2009) [77]UKPeer-reviewed paperCattle (Bos taurus)Female intact12–24 yearsClickerStableCase studyEliminate in designated areaCount correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and food
28/1Williams et al. (2004) [34]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)>50% male neutered>4 yearsClickerStableGroup designNose touch coneNumber of sessions to completionConditioned Reinforcement and food
28/2Williams et al. (2004) [34]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)>50% male neutered>4 yearsClickerStableGroup designNose touch coneNumber of sessions to completionConditioned Reinforcement and INT food
28/3Williams et al. (2004) [34]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)>50% male neutered>4 yearsNot applicableStableGroup designNose touch coneNumber of sessions to completionFood only
29/1Willis et al. (2004) [78]UKPeer-reviewed paperDog (Canis familiaris)MixedNot clearly statedClickerTraining facilityCase studyScent discrimination (healthy/ill)Count correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and food
30/1Willson et al. (2017) [28]New ZealandPeer-reviewed paperCat (Cattus syslvestris)>50% male neutered6 months–15 yearsBeepShelter roomGroup designNose touching a targetShaping steps completedConditioned Reinforcement and food
30/2Willson et al. (2017) [28]New ZealandPeer-reviewed paperCat (Cattus syslvestris)>50% male neutered6 months–15 yearsNot applicableShelter roomGroup designNose touching a targetShaping steps completedFood only
30/3Willson et al. (2017) [28]New ZealandPeer-reviewed paperCat (Cattus syslvestris)>50% male neutered6 months–15 yearsBeepShelter roomGroup designNose touching a targetNumber of attempts over timeConditioned Reinforcement only
30/4Willson et al. (2017) [28]New ZealandPeer-reviewed paperCat (Cattus syslvestris)>50% male neutered6 months–15 yearsBeepShelter roomGroup designNose touching a targetNumber of attempts over timeConditioned Reinforcement only
31/1Wood (2007) [29]USAMaster’s thesisDog (Canis familiaris)>50% male neutered1–5 yearsClickerShelter roomGroup designNose touching a freestanding targetRequired Rs to reach criterionConditioned Reinforcement and food
31/2Wood (2007) [29]USAMaster’s thesisDog (Canis familiaris)>50% male neutered1–5 yearsSpoken wordShelter roomGroup designNose touching a freestanding targetRequired Rs to reach criterionConditioned Reinforcement and food
32/1Rybova (2018) [23]New ZealandMaster’s thesisHorse (Equus caballus)50% female7–28 yearsClickerStableSingle-case methodNose-touch target stickCount correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and food
33/1Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)50% female≤6 monthsSpoken wordOutdoor enclosureGroup designTouching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone)LatencyConditioned Reinforcement and tactile
33/2Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)50% female≤6 monthsSpoken wordOutdoor enclosureGroup designTouching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone)Count correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and tactile
33/3Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)50% female≤6 monthsSpoken wordOutdoor enclosureGroup designTouching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone)Correct response to verbal prompt rateConditioned Reinforcement and tactile
33/4Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)50% female≤6 monthsSpoken wordOutdoor enclosureGroup designTouching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone)LatencyConditioned Reinforcement and tactile
33/5Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)50% female≤6 monthsSpoken wordOutdoor enclosureGroup designTouching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone)Count correct responsesConditioned Reinforcement and tactile
33/6Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50]USAPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)50% female≤6 monthsSpoken wordOutdoor enclosureGroup designTouching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone)Correct response to verbal prompt rateConditioned Reinforcement and tactile
34/1Slater et al. (2011) [9]UKPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Male neutered5–18 yearsClickerOutdoor enclosureSingle-case methodLoading trailerLoading steps completedConditioned Reinforcement and food
34/2Slater et al. (2011) [9]UKPeer-reviewed paperHorse (Equus caballus)Male neutered5–18 yearsClickerStableSingle-case methodLifting hoofDurationConditioned Reinforcement and INT food
Table 3. Count and percentage of studies by species and type of conditioned reinforcers (Sr+), across types of unconditioned reinforcers (SR+).
Table 3. Count and percentage of studies by species and type of conditioned reinforcers (Sr+), across types of unconditioned reinforcers (SR+).
Learner SpeciesType of Sr+FoodScratchingWaterNo SR+Not clearly StatedTotal
By HandRemotelyContainerProximity
Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%
Cat (Cattus syslvestris)Beep00%13%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Clicker13%00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Cattle (Bos taurus)Clicker13%00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Dog (Canis familiaris)Clicker1132%00%13%13%00%00%00%13%1441%
Spoken word13%00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Whistle00%00%00%13%00%00%00%00%13%
Fish (Tripterygion tripteronotum)Clicker00%00%13%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Goat (Capra hircus)Beep00%00%00%00%00%13%00%00%13%
Clicker13%00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Horse (Equus caballus)Buzzing sound00%00%13%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Clicker515%00%26%00%0000%00%00%721%
Spoken word00%00%00%00%13%00%13%00%26%
Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta)Clicker13%00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis)Clicker13%00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Total2265%13%515%26%13%13%13%13%34100%
Note: For the case of food (SR+), the mode of delivery is specified (i.e., by hand, by remotely controlled feeder, presented in a container, or delivered in proximity), see Table 1 for details. Percentages may not total 100% due to rounding.
Table 4. Implementation of preference assessment displayed by type of unconditioned reinforcers (SR+).
Table 4. Implementation of preference assessment displayed by type of unconditioned reinforcers (SR+).
Type of SR+Preference AssessmentCount%
FoodFood by handNo2059%
Yes13%
Not clearly stated13%
Food remotelyNo13%
Food containerNo515%
Food proximityNo13%
Not clearly stated13%
ScratchingYes13%
WaterNot applicable13%
No SR+Not applicable13%
Not clearly statedNot clearly stated13%
Total34100%
Note: Percentages may not total 100% due to rounding.
Table 5. Number of pairings and respective interstimulus intervals (ISI) between conditioned (CS) and unconditioned stimuli (US) reported across eligible studies that reported separate pairings.
Table 5. Number of pairings and respective interstimulus intervals (ISI) between conditioned (CS) and unconditioned stimuli (US) reported across eligible studies that reported separate pairings.
CS-US Pairing InformationInterstimulus Interval ISICount%
>20 pairings (no further info)30 s–3 min13%
Not clearly stated13%
≤20 pairings/dayApproximately 1 s (trace)26%
Approximately 10 s13%
No delay (simultaneous)13%
Not clearly stated515%
20–40 pairings/dayImmediately13%
No delay (simultaneous)13%
Not clearly stated26%
60 pairings/dayNot clearly stated13%
Pairings implemented (no further info)Immediately13%
No delay (simultaneous)13%
Not clearly stated39%
No pairing sessionsNot applicable1029%
Not clearly statedNot clearly stated39%
Total34100%
Note: Percentages may not total 100% due to rounding.
Table 6. Approximations of the delays between the display of the target responses (R; e.g., nose touch) and the onset of the conditioned reinforcer (Sr+; e.g., click or spoken word) reported across all eligible studies.
Table 6. Approximations of the delays between the display of the target responses (R; e.g., nose touch) and the onset of the conditioned reinforcer (Sr+; e.g., click or spoken word) reported across all eligible studies.
Delay R → Sr+BeepBuzzing SoundClickerSpoken WordWhistleTotal
Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%
Approximately 1 s00%00%13%00%00%13%
Immediately00%00%39%13%00%412%
Immediately after00%00%13%00%00%13%
Simultaneous00%00%39%13%00%412%
Shortly after00%00%13%00%00%13%
Not clearly stated26%13%1853%13%13%2368%
Total26%13%2779%39%13%34100%
Note: Percentages may not total 100% due to rounding.
Table 7. Information extracted from the reviewed studies on the interval between the conditioned reinforcer (Sr+) and the delivery of the unconditioned reinforcement (SR+) reported across all eligible studies.
Table 7. Information extracted from the reviewed studies on the interval between the conditioned reinforcer (Sr+) and the delivery of the unconditioned reinforcement (SR+) reported across all eligible studies.
Interval Sr+ → SR+FoodScratchingWaterNo SR+Not Clearly StatedTotal
Food by HandFood RemotelyFood ContainerFood Proximity
Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%
Approximately 1 s13%00%13%00%00%00%00%00%26%
Following26%00%13%00%00%00%00%00%39%
Immediately39%00%13%13%00%00%00%00%515%
Shortly after13%00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
No delay (simultaneous)00%00%00%00%13%13%00%00%26%
Not clearly stated1544%13%26%13%00%00%00%13%2059%
Not applicable *00%00%00%00%00%00%13%00%13%
Total2265%13%515%26%13%13%13%13%34100%
Note: Percentages may not total 100% due to rounding. * = the Sr+ was not followed by the SR+.
Table 8. Positioning of experimenters and trainers with respective reinforcers displayed by learner species.
Table 8. Positioning of experimenters and trainers with respective reinforcers displayed by learner species.
Learner SpeciesIn Front, SR+ AttachedIn Front, SR+ ProximityIn Front, No SR+Outside View, SR+ ProximityPeripheral View, SR+ AttachedPeripheral View, SR+ ProximityNot ApplicableNot Clearly StatedTotal
Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%Count%
Cat (Cattus syslvestris)00%00%00%00%00%00%00%26%26%
Cattle (Bos taurus)00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%13%
Dog (Canis familiaris)824%00%00%00%00%00%00%824%1647%
Fish (Tripterygion tripteronotum)00%13%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Goat (Capra hircus)00%00%00%00%00%00%13%13%26%
Horse (Equus caballus)00%13%13%13%13%13%00%515%1029%
Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta)00%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%13%
Squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis)00%13%00%00%00%00%00%00%13%
Total824%39%13%13%13%13%13%1853%34100%
Note: Percentages may not total 100% due to rounding.
Table 9. Study characteristics and effect sizes per study.
Table 9. Study characteristics and effect sizes per study.
Author (Year)Species#SubjectsDesignReinforcersTau-U (CI95%)ContrastsEffectDesign Quality
Ferguson and Rosales-Ruiz (2001) [21]Horse5MBLClick and food0.95 (0.78, 1)15LargeStrong
Fernandez * (2003) [4]Goat3CCDClick and food0.51 (0.3, 0.71)6SmallStrong
Pfaller-Sadovsky et al. (2017) [8]Dog4MBLClick and food0.77 (0.49, 1)7MediumStrong
Rybova (2018) * [30]Horse4MBLClick and food0.90 (0.59, 1)8MediumModerate
Wennmacher * (2007) [25]Dog2RVDClick and food versus Click-click food0.48 (0.33, 0.61)12SmallModerate
Slater and Dymond (2011) [9]Horse5MBLClick and food0.98 (0.73, 1)4LargeStrong
Note: CCD = changing criterion design; CI = confidence interval; MBL = multiple-baseline design; RVD = reversal design. * = Master’s theses.
Table 10. Effect sizes and standard errors by potential moderators.
Table 10. Effect sizes and standard errors by potential moderators.
Moderator VariablesNumber of CovariatesRegression ES and (SE) Qp-ValuesR2 (%)
Learner species3 20.570.000 *1
  Dogs 0.573 (0.067)
  Goats 0.574 (0.11)
  Horses 0.94 (0.09)
Pairing sessions4 1.420.700
  >20 pairing sessions 0.94 (0.23)
  20–40 pairings/day 0.76 (0.28)
  60 pairings/day 0.90 (0.35)
  No pairings 0.63 (0.29)
Position of trainer with Sr+3 1.280.530
  In front of animal w/Sr+ attached 0.63 (0.16)
  Not clearly stated 0.82 (0.20)
  Within peripheral view w/Sr+ attached 0.90 (0.29)
Study design3 26.860.0001 *1
  MBL 0.92 (0.06)
  Changing criterion 0.34 (0.10)
  Reversal/withdrawal 0.44 (0.09)
Target behavior by4 2.590.4612
  Capturing w/movement restrictions 0.57 (0.20)
  Capturing w/o movement restrictions 0.94 (0.28)
  Shaping w/prompts 0.67 (0.25)
  Shaping w/o prompts 0.88 (0.26)
Trainer type3 0.110.940
  Experimenter 0.80 (0.15)
  Mixed 0.72 (0.25)
  Owner 0.78 (0.32)
Note: ES = effect size; SE = standard error; statistically significant results are highlighted with an asterisk.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Hurtado-Parrado, C.; Cardillo, D.; Medina, L.G.; Friedman, S.G. What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Animals 2020, 10, 1757. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ani10101757

AMA Style

Pfaller-Sadovsky N, Hurtado-Parrado C, Cardillo D, Medina LG, Friedman SG. What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Animals. 2020; 10(10):1757. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ani10101757

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pfaller-Sadovsky, Nicole, Camilo Hurtado-Parrado, Daniela Cardillo, Lucia G. Medina, and Susan G. Friedman. 2020. "What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Animals 10, no. 10: 1757. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ani10101757

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop