Next Article in Journal
Evaluation of Cerebral Microvascular Regulatory Mechanisms with Transcranial Doppler in Fabry Disease
Previous Article in Journal
Towards Intelligent Data Analytics: A Case Study in Driver Cognitive Load Classification
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Comparative Analysis of the Permutation and Multiscale Entropies for Quantification of the Brain Signal Variability in Naturalistic Scenarios

The Thomas N. Sato BioMEC-X Laboratories, Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International (ATR), 2-2 Hikaridai Seika-cho, Kyoto 619-02, Japan
Submission received: 7 July 2020 / Revised: 4 August 2020 / Accepted: 5 August 2020 / Published: 6 August 2020

Abstract

:
As alternative entropy estimators, multiscale entropy (MSE) and permutation entropy (PE) are utilized for quantification of the brain function and its signal variability. In this context, their applications are primarily focused on two specific domains: (1) the effect of brain pathology on its function (2) the study of altered states of consciousness. As a result, there is a paucity of research on applicability of these measures in more naturalistic scenarios. In addition, the utility of these measures for quantification of the brain function and with respect to its signal entropy is not well studied. These shortcomings limit the interpretability of the measures when used for quantification of the brain signal entropy. The present study addresses these limitations by comparing MSE and PE with entropy of human subjects’ EEG recordings, who watched short movie clips with negative, neutral, and positive content. The contribution of the present study is threefold. First, it identifies a significant anti-correlation between MSE and entropy. In this regard, it also verifies that such an anti-correlation is stronger in the case of negative rather than positive or neutral affects. Second, it finds that MSE significantly differentiates between these three affective states. Third, it observes that the use of PE does not warrant such significant differences. These results highlight the level of association between brain’s entropy in response to affective stimuli on the one hand and its quantification in terms of MSE and PE on the other hand. This, in turn, allows for more informed conclusions on the utility of MSE and PE for the study and analysis of the brain signal variability in naturalistic scenarios.

1. Introduction

Recent neuroscientific findings present compelling empirical [1,2,3] and theoretical [4,5] evidence for the importance of the brain signal variability in its function. Such a variability stems from the interaction between neuronal circuits [6,7] across broad spatiotemporal scales [8,9]. This variability is further hypothesized to signify the cortical self-organized criticality [10,11,12,13] in which the brain capacity for information processing is maximized [14,15]. These observations help verify the potential role of entropy in identification of the brain signal variability [16,17,18,19,20,21] and the significance of such a variability in its function [22,23,24].
However, the entropy of biological signals can be drastically affected by such factors as long-range autocorrelation (due to reduction of entropy by correlation [25]), the signal length, and the presence of non-stationarity [26]. To mitigate these affects, a number of entropy estimators are introduced [27,28] among which multiscale entropy (MSE) [29] and permutation entropy (PE) [30] are widely used.
MSE utilizes the sample entropy [28] to quantify the signal variability at different time scales (through coarse graining process) [31,32]. It is successfully applied in the study of schizophrenia [33], depression [34], autism [35,36], and Alzheimer’s disease [37,38]. It also appears as a relibale tool for analysis of the effect of ageing on brain function [39,40] as well as the sleep cycle [41].
On the other hand, PE utilizes the reoccurrences of ordinal patterns in the signal to estimate its variability through the application of Shannon entropy [42]. PE is shown to be more robust to noise [30] and invariant under non-linear scale transformations [43]. Applications of PE include the difference in heart rate variability (HRV) between healthy individuals and the congestive heart failure (CHF) patients [44], detection of epileptic seizure [45], and the effect of anesthetic drugs on humans [46] and non-humans [47]. Zanin et al. present a review of PE applications.

1.1. The Purpose of Present Study

Within the domain of neuroscientific research, MSE and PE are primarily used in two specific scenarios: (1) the affect of brain pathology on its function [33,34,35,36,37,38,45] (2) the differential states of wakeful and unconscious brain [41,46,47,48]. This results in a lack of sufficient research on applicability of these measures in more naturalistic scenarios. Furthermore, the utility of these measures for quantification of the brain function and with respect to its entropy is not well studied. Such analyses are crucial for determining the level of correspondence between these measures on the one hand and the entropy of neurophysiological signals on the other hand. For instance, MSE utilizes several coarse-grained versions of original time series (referred to as scales) to refine the estimate of its variability [29]. In contrast, PE achieves this objective via the realization of the ordinal patterns in the time series [30]. However, the lack of comparative analysis between these measures with entropy limits their interpretability when used for quantification of the brain signal entropy.
The present study addresses these shortcomings through comparative analysis of MSE and PE for study of the human brain signal variability in response to naturalistic stimuli. To this end, it uses the Shanghai Jiao Tong University (SJTU) Emotion EEG Dataset (SEED) [49] that comprises human subjects’ sixty-two-channel EEG recordings. These EEG signals were acquired when the human subjects in the SEED study watched fifteen four-minute movie clips with positive, neutral, and negative contents.

1.2. Contributions

The contribution of the present study is threefold. First, it identifies a significant anti-correlation between MSE and entropy, whose affect is stronger in the case of negative than positive or neutral affect. Second, it verifies that MSE significantly differentiates between these affective states. In particular, it indicates that the negative affect is associated with lowest MSE, that the positive affect’s MSE is higher at the small-scale, and that the neutral affect is associated with higher MSE at the large-scale. These results highlight the level of association between the brain’s entropy in response to affective stimuli on the one hand and its quantification in terms of MSE and PE on the other hand.
These results help draw a level of association between the brain’s entropy in response to affective stimuli on the one hand and its quantification in terms of MSE and PE on the other hand. This, in turn, allows for more informed conclusions on the utility of these measures for study and analysis of the brain function.

2. Materials and Methods

The Dataset

SEED [49] includes (Figure 1B) fifteen Chinese subjects’ sixty-two-channel EEG recordings (7 males and 8 females; Mean (M) = 23.27, Standard Deviation (SD) = 2.37). These recordings were acquired during the participants’ watching of fifteen four-minute Chinese movie clips with negative, neutral, and positive affective content (five clips per affect). The participants’ selection criteria were based on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) [50] personality traits. This questionnaire analyzes the individuals’ personality along three independent temperament’s dimensions. They are neuroticism/stability, extraversion/introversion, and psychoticism/socialization.
The movie clips were selected based on three criteria: length (i.e., four-minute to prevent fatigue), content (easy to comprehend without requiring explicit explanations), and single-affect elicitation (e.g., only negative affect). To choose proper movie clips, the authors recruited twenty volunteers who were different from those who took part in SEED. These additional individuals assessed a pool of movie clips using the Likert five-point scale. The authors then used these assessments and selected 15 movie clips whose average scores were ≥3 and were also ranked among the top five in their respective affect category. The affectiveness of these selected movie clips were further verified in a follow-up study [52] that included nine participants (different from the twenty individuals who first rated them).
In each experiment, per participant, the movie clips were ordered to prevent two similar-affect clips (e.g., both having negative content) to follow one another. There was a five-second hint period before the start of each clip (i.e., similar to [52]). After watching each four-minute movie clip, the participants answered three questions that assessed their emotion-feeling states [51]. These questions included participants’ actually felt emotion while watching the clip, have they previously watched the full movie that the clip was taken from, and how well they comprehended the clip’s content. After answering these questions, the participants took a fifteen-second rest before the next movie clip started. Every participant took part in three experiments (one week apart). Every experiment used the same set of fifteen movie clips. All participants watched them in the same order of presentation.
SEED’s preprocessing steps were as follows. First, the EEG segments that corresponded to the duration of each movie clip were extracted (i.e., per EEG channel, per affect, per movie clip, and per participants). These segments were then down-sampled to 200 Hz sampling rate. Next, they were visually examined against the individuals’ accompanying electromyography (EMG) and electrooculography (EOG) recordings. Subsequently, the data that were contaminated by potential muscle- (i.e., EMG) or eye-movement (i.e., EOG) were manually removed. The EOG was further utilized to identify blink artifacts in these EEG recordings. To attenuate the noise (e.g., cardiac pulsations, respiration, etc.), the EEG signals were band-pass filtered between 0.3 to 50 Hz. Further details about SEED and its data preprocessing can be found in [49,52] and at http://bcmi.sjtu.edu.cn/~seed/seed.html.

3. Data Validation

The present study utilized the EEG recordings that pertained to the first participation of every individual and included all of their fifteen movie clips (i.e., five clips per affect) in their session (Figure 1A). For each individual, all sixty-two EEG channels were used. Prior to further analyses, the following EEG signals’ validation steps were applied.
To ensure that DE, MSE, and PE computations were not affected by the length of EEG data [32,53,54], all recordings were first confirmed to be sufficiently long (Mean (M) = 45,286.71, Standard Deviation (SD) = 2776.61, CI 95 % = [44,565.70 46,007.73], minimum = 37,000, maximum Length = 47,601) where CI 95 % stands for 95% confidence interval. These recordings were then trimmed to yield equal-length EEG time series (i.e., per channel, per affect, and per participant) as per the shortest available EEG recording (i.e., 37,000 data points).
Next, the participants’ EEG recordings were de-trended. This step was then followed by performing Augmented Dickey–Fuller (ADF) [55] and Kwiatkowski–Phillips–Schmidt–Shin (KPSS) [56] tests on them to ensure their covariance stationarity. These tests identified that one of the participant’s data did not satisfy covariance stationarity (i.e., trend-stationarity which implies mean and variance do not change over time). Consequently, this participant was excluded from further analyses. Additionally, the first session of another two participants’ EEG recordings did not also pass these tests. Therefore, their first sessions were replaced with their corresponding second and third sessions, respectively, that subsequently passed these tests.

4. Quantification of the EEG Channels’ Signal Variability Using DE, PE, and MSE

In the present study, DE, MSE, and PE were computed in terms of the brain variability across distributed regions. This approach was motivated by the findings that underlined the distributed nature of the brain function [57,58,59,60]. For instance, prior research indicated that most accurate neural signature of discrete emotions were associated with the brain’s anatomically distributed variational patterns [61]. It also identified that the use of joint activity from these regions more accurately discriminated between different emotions [62,63]. In this respect, Liu et al. [48] showed that MSE and FC were robust measures for mice spatiotemporal whole-brain variability across anesthetic and wakeful states. These results provided evidence for the involvement of large-scale cortical networks in the formation of high-level mental states and distinct emotions [64,65,66].
In line with these findings, Keshmiri et al. [67] also observed that DE of the brain’s distributed regions significantly distinguished between negative, neutral, and positive affect. Specifically, their results indicated that DE of the negative and positive affect were higher than that of neutral affect. This finding echoed the results that identified an increase in the brain’s information processing during the emotional than neutral contexts [68]. It was also in accord with the findings that indicated an increase in brain activity with attention [69,70]. Keshmiri et al. [67] also observed that DE was able to significantly differentiate between the negative, neutral, and positive affect. These results further supported the observations that considered distributed brain regions were involved in the formation of differential affect and emotions [66,71,72]. In light of these findings, the present study considered DE from [67] as a basis for evaluation of the utility of MSE and PE for quantification of the brain responses to negative, neutral, and positive affective stimuli.

4.1. The Procedure

The present study followed the same steps that were outlined in [67] to compute DE, PE, and MSE values for participants’ full-length EEG time series (i.e., 37,000 data points), per channel. For clarity, these steps are restated in this section.
For every trial, each participant’s data that pertained to each of its five movie clips (i.e., one EEG data file for each of the movie clip), per affect, were accessed, one-by-one, and their respective DE, MSE, and PE were computed. This step yielded five separate sets (i.e., one set for each of the movie clips, per affect), each of which containing DE, PE, and MSE values for each of sixty-two EEG channels, per participant. We then computed the average values of each of these entropic measures for each channel.
For example, in the case of PE computation for EEG channel FP1 (Figure 1B), there were five PEs (i.e., one PE for each of the movie clips of a given affect (e.g., negative affect): [PE m o v i e c l i p 1 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 2 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 3 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 4 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 5 F P 1 ]. These five PEs were averaged (i.e., mean([PE m o v i e c l i p 1 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 2 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 3 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 4 F P 1 , PE m o v i e c l i p 5 F P 1 ]). It is also apparent that these values’ order have no effect on their computed average PE for FP1 [73].

4.2. DE Computation

DE was computed using its non-parametric estimator [74] that is based on the nearest neighbour distance [75]. The present results utilized its implementation in [76].
In the case of DE, the computation step highlighted in Section 4 resulted in 1 × 62 vectors, per affect, per participant, where 62 refers to the number of EEG channels (Figure 1B).

4.3. PE Computation

PE computation was carried out using its original implementation by Bandt and Pompe [30]. In the case of PE, practical consideration for its use favours the choice of permutation order that is 3 m ! < < N [53,77,78] where m and N refer to permutation order and the length of the time series, respectively. Considering the length of EEG time series in the present study (i.e., 37,000 data points), the only suitable permutation orders were 3–7. Therefore, the main text reported PE results using order 3 and time lag 1 (i.e., its original implementation by Bandt and Pompe [30]). Appendices Appendix J, Appendix K and Appendix L provide the results pertinent to permutation orders 5, 6 and 7.
In the case of PE, computational steps in Section 4 resulted in 1 × 62 vectors (for all permutation orders 3, 5, 6, and 7), per affect, per participant, where 62 refers to the number of EEG channels (Figure 1B).

4.4. MSE Computation

The present study utilized the original MSE implementation by Goldberger et al. [32]. It also followed their guidelines on the choice of parameters, thereby using the scale factor 20 (MSE1 through MSE20 hereafter) and the similarity criterion 0.20 (i.e., a positive real value, typically within 10.0% to 20.0% of the time series standard deviation [32]).
MSE computation also followed the same computational steps in Section 4 (i.e., similar to DE and PE). Unlike DE and PE, MSE computation resulted in 20 × 62 matrices, per affect, per participant, where 62 refers to the number of EEG channels (Figure 1B) and 20 refers to MSE1–MSE20, per channel, per affect.

5. Analysis

For both PE and MSE, their Spearman correlations with DE, per EEG channel, per affect, were computed. This was followed by their bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) correlations at 95.0% (i.e., p < 0.05) confidence interval. This test considered the null hypothesis
Hypothesis 1.
There was no correlation between participants’ PEs/MSEs and the corresponding DEs of their EEG channels.
Hypothesis 1 was then tested against the alternative hypothesis
Hypothesis 2.
There was a significant correlation between participants’ PEs/MSEs and the corresponding DEs of their EEG channels.
We further computed the p-value of these tests as a fraction of the distribution that was more extreme than the actually observed correlation values. This was carried out using a two-tailed test in which the correlation coefficients’ absolute values were used so that both, the positive and the negative correlations were accounted for.
Next, the significance of EEG channels’ PE and MSE for each of the negative, neutral, and positive affect were compared (i.e., with respect to DE) using Kruskal–Wallis test. This was then followed by posthoc Wilcoxon rank-sum tests between every pair of affect. These results were further verified through the application of the paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval on each pair of these affective states. These paired two-sample bootstrap tests considered the null hypothesis
Hypothesis 3.
The difference between PE/MSE values of participants’ EEG channels in two different mental states was non-significant.
They tested Hypothesis 3 against the alternative hypothesis.
Hypothesis 4.
The PE/MSE values of participants’ EEG channels were significantly different in two different mental states.

Reported Results and Affect Sizes

In the case of two-tailed bootstrap tests, their mean, standard deviation, and 95.0% confidence interval are reported.
Results pertinent to MSE1 and MSE20, per participant, per affect, per EEG channel, are reported in the main text. Appendices Appendix G, Appendix H and Appendix I provide these results for MSE2–MSE19.
For PE, the main text includes its results based on permutation order 3. Subsequently, Appendices Appendix J, Appendix K and Appendix L present the results for permutation orders 5, 6, and 7.
Appendix M provide the Spearman Correlations Between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. The rand p-values associated with these figures are shown in Table A17, Table A18 and Table A19.
The effect size for the Kruskal–Wallis test was computed using r = χ 2 N [79] where N and χ 2 are the sample size and the test-statistics. For Wilcoxon tests, the effect size r = W N [80] was used. In this equation, W is the Wilcoxon statistics. All results were Bonferroni-corrected. All analyses were carried out in Matlab 2016a.
In regards to the adapted analyses by the present study, two points beg further clarification: the choice of non-parametric tests and the bootstrap test of significance. The choice of non-parametric tests were due to the fact that the computed DE, PE, and MSE, per affect, were not normally distributed (separately as well as combined, with respect to both: individuals and EEG channels). On the other hand, the use of bootstraps was motivated by the fact that the reported analyses were based on a small sample of fourteen individuals. As a result, it was necessary to ensure that the observed differences between DE, MSE, and PE were not due to a sub-sample of participants (i.e., skewed values). Applying bootstrap tests with large re-sampling successions (10,000 simulations) at 95.0% confidence interval (i.e., p < 0.05 significance level) allowed for further evaluation of the reported results.

6. Results

6.1. DE vs. PE

Figure 2 shows the correlations between participants’ DE and their corresponding PE values. PE and DE were significantly correlated in the case of negative affect (Figure 2A, r = 0.32, p < 0.001). However, such correlations were non-significant in the case of neutral (r = 0.05, p = 0.15) and positive (r = 0.05, p = 0.15) affect. These results were further supported by their corresponding bootstrap tests (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (Appendix A).
Figure 3A shows the grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE in negative, neutral, and positive affect. Kruskal–Wallis test indicated a significant difference among participants’ positive, neutral, and negative affect (p < 0.001, H(2, 2603) = 20.79, r = 0.09). Posthoc Wilcoxon test (Figure 3B) indicated higher PE values in the case of positive than neutral affect (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 4.52, r = 0.11, M P o s i t i v e = 2.56, SD P o s i t i v e = 0.04, M N e u t r a l = 2.54, SD N e u t r a l = 0.05). Similarly, the negative affect was associated with higher PEs than the neutral affect (p < 0.01, W(1734) = 3.03, r = 0.07, M N e g a t i v e = 2.55, SD N e g a t i v e = 0.05). On the other hand, the difference between the PEs of the positive and negative affect were non-significant (p = 0.21, W(1734) = 1.26). However, these significant differences did not survive the two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (Appendix B).

6.2. MSE1 vs. DE

Figure 4 shows the correlations between participants’ DEs and MSE1s. MSE1 and DE were significantly anti-correlated in the case of negative (Figure 4A, r = −0.50, p < 0.001), neutral (Figure 4B, r = −0.13, p < 0.001), and positive affect (Figure 4C, r = −0.13, p < 0.001). These results were further supported by their corresponding bootstrap tests (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (Appendix C).
Figure 5A shows the grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ MSE1s in negative, neutral, and positive affect. These subplots identify incremental patterns of MSE1 from negative to positive affect. Kruskal–Wallis test on these MSE1 values identified a significant difference between these three affective states (p < 0.001, H(2, 2603) = 160.39, r = 0.25). Posthoc Wilcoxon test (Figure 5B) indicated that MSE1 in the case of positive affect was significantly higher than neutral (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 4.51, r = 0.11, M P o s i t i v e = 1.22, SD P o s i t i v e = 0.46, M N e u t r a l = 1.13, SD N e u t r a l = 0.38) and negative (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 11.84, r = 0.28, M N e g a t i v e = 0.95, SD N e g a t i v e = 0.45) affect. It also showed that neutral affect’s MSE1 was significantly higher than MSE1 associated with the negative affect (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 8.90, r = 0.22). These results were further supported by their corresponding two-sample bootstrap tests of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (Appendix D).

6.3. MSE20 vs. DE

There were also significant anti-correlations between participants’ DEs and MSE20s (negative: Figure 6A, r = −0.56, p < 0.001, and neutral: Figure 6B, r = −0.41, p < 0.001, and positive: Figure 6C, r = −0.41, p < 0.001). These anti-correlations were stronger in the case of MSE20 than MSE1. They were further supported by their corresponding bootstrap tests (10,000 simulation runs) at the 95.0% confidence interval (Appendix E).
Figure 7A shows the grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ MSE20s in negative, neutral, and positive affect. These subplots identify a pattern in which MSE20s associated with neutral affect is higher than negative and positive affect. Kruskal–Wallis on these MSE20s identified a significant difference between participants’ three affective states (p < 0.001, H(2, 2603) = 155.37, r = 0.24). However in this case and unlike MSE1, posthoc Wilcoxon test (Figure 7B) revealed that neutral affect’s MSE20 was significantly higher than positive ( p < 0.01, W(1734) = 2.84, r = 0.07, M P o s i t i v e = 1.26, SD P o s i t i v e = 0.33, M N e u t r a l = 1.31, SD N e u t r a l = 0.31) and negative (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 9.20, r = 0.22, M N e g a t i v e = 1.09, SD N e g a t i v e = 0.38) affect. It also indicated that positive affect’s MSE20 was significantly higher (i.e., similar to MSE1) than the MSE20 of negative affect (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 11.85, r = 0.28). These results were further supported by their corresponding two-sample bootstrap tests of significance 10,000 simulation runs) at a 95.0% confidence interval (Appendix F).

7. Discussion

The present study sought to examine the utility of MSE and PE for study of the effect of naturalistic affective stimuli on brain signal variability. For this purpose, it utilized SEED [49]: a publicly available dataset of human subjects’ sixty-two-channel EEG recordings. These EEG signals were recorded while participants watched movie clips whose contents elicited negative, neutral, and positive affect. Our study was primarily motivated by two observations. First, previous research has mostly considered MSE and PE for investigation of either the effect of brain pathology on its function [33,34,35,36,37,38,45] or the altered state of consciousness [41,46,47,48]. This has left the utility of these measures for the study of brain variability in more naturalistic settings unanswered. Second, the lack of comparative analysis of MSE and PE with the brain signal entropy has further limited the scope of their interpretability when used for quantification of the brain signal entropy.
MSE appeared to complement the findings on the use of DE for quantification of the differential effect of negative, neutral, and positive affect on the brain activity. Specifically, Keshmiri et al. [67] identified that DE of the brain’s distributed regions significantly distinguished between negative, neutral, and positive affect. They also observed that DEs of negative and positive affect were higher than neutral’s DE. This finding echoed the brain’s higher information processing during the emotional, rather than neutral contexts [68]. In this respect, the present results found that positive and neutral affects were associated with higher MSE than the negative affect. They further indicated that the positive affect’s MSE was higher at the small-scale and the neutral affect was associated with higher MSE at the large-scale. These results were in accordance with the findings that maximum entropy does not necessarily promote higher signal variability [25,29]. They also extended the findings on the interplay between small and large scale MSEs [39,48,81,82] to the case of brain affective responses to naturalistic stimuli.
On the other hand, we found little evidence in support of the use of PE. For instance, the use of permutation order-3 resulted in a significant correlation between PE and DE in the case of negative affect. Although this correlation was in accord with the correspondence between PE and signal’s entropy [30,83,84], such a correlation was absent in the case of neutral and positive affect. Surprisingly, such a correlation was absent altogether in the case of higher-order permutations (Appendices Appendix J, Appendix K and Appendix L). Considering the present results, PE appeared to more readily associate with the brain variability that is characterized with distinct states (e.g., epileptic seizure [45], the affect of anesthetic drugs [46,47]) that presumably take place in a dichotomous fashion [66] than subtle changes in mental states during naturalistic settings (e.g., negative than neutral or positive affect).
A number of studies utilize PE to interpret the affect of such activities as meditation [85] and mediated social communication [86] on human subjects. These studies use PE to construe the observed changes in the brain activity in terms of relaxation and induced comfort in such settings. The present results challenged the plausibility of such interpretations. In particular, the observed insensitivity of PE to subtle changes in brain variability appeared to explain the discrepancy of the findings in the meditation literature. For instance, Vyšata et al. [85] reported a decrease in global permutation entropy during the mediation (interpreted as a sign for relaxation by the authors). On the contrary, Kakumanua et al. [87] observed an increase in PE that was only present in the case of experienced meditators. This was also the case in mediated social communication in which the lower [86] and higher [88,89] entropy was assumed to highlight such self-assessed feelings as relaxed mood, interest, and the feeling of human presence. Furthermore, the mediated social communication studies were based on a surprisingly limited number of brain sites (e.g., only two forehead sites in [86,88,89]). As a result, it was unclear whether the observed changes in entropy were due to the individuals’ interaction with the media or potentially a residue of overall brain signal variability [1,3] which may not necessarily pertain to the affect of stimuli. Most importantly, these results were inconsistent with the findings that underlined the substantial contrast between human–human and human–agent interactions (HHI and HAI, respectively) [90]. For example, Rauchbauer and colleagues [91] observed that neural markers of mentalizing [92,93] and social motivation [94] were only activated during HHI. These studies also suggested that although the brain activity within the person perception network (PPN) was not reduced during HAI (as compared to HHI), the activity within the theory-of-mind (TOM) network [95,96] was reduced [97,98]. These findings along with the insensitivity of PE to differential affect in the present study, appear to be at odds with the interpretation of decreased PE as a measure of relaxation/comfort in mediated communication [86].
Taken together, the present study implied that MSE and PE may yield different results in the case of naturalistic settings in comparison with such scenarios as the affect of brain pathology on its function [33,34,35,36,37,38,45] and altered states of consciousness [41,46,47,48]. These results help realize the level of association between entropy, on the one hand, and MSE and PE on the other hand, thereby allowing for more informed conclusions on their use for the quantification of the brain variability in response to naturalistic stimuli.
On the other hand, it is also crucial to emphasize that the present results suffer from two important limiting factors: small sample size and the lack of cultural diversity within this sample. In this regard, the accompanying bootstrap tests with large repeated re-sampling partially allowed for mitigating the potential impact that the small sample might have otherwise imposed on the analyses. However, such tests must not be interpreted as substitutes for larger sample size and therefore care must be taken to avoid improper generalization of the present findings. In the same vein, cultural diversity is the key to a thorough understanding of the full spectrum of the humans’ emotion and mental states. Although individuals’ ability to experience affect appears to be a unifying facet across cultures [99], this does not necessarily imply that all individuals experience and respond to affective stimuli in a similar way. Therefore, one must take caution to prevent unwarranted generalization of the present results across different cultures.

8. Limitations and Future Direction

An important characteristic of the brain function is the innate interactivity among its distributed regions [20]. This interplay among brain’s distributed regions implies the flow of information across its multiple areas whose information-content may concurrently contribute to a number of functional pathways [100]. The present study fell short in considering the potential effect of a such a flow of information on computed DE, PE, and MSE values. Therefore, future research to verify the possibility of such impacts [101,102] is necessary, thereby allowing for more informed conclusion on present results.
Our analyses identified significant anti-correlations between DE and MSE at different scales (i.e., MSE1–MSE20). They also indicated that such anti-correlations were stronger in the case of negative than positive and neutral affect. This differed from the results of the altered states of consciousness [48] in which positive/negative correlations were associated with the small/large scales. Although Liu et al. [48] also reported the weakening of correlations in the case of simulated brain variability, observed correlations in the present study were substantially weaker. These differences may suggest the differential behaviour of MSE in naturalistic settings compared to such scenarios as a study of the brain variability under anesthesia and wakefulness. On the other hand, the observed correlational differences might be due to the differing operational principle between DE that was used in the present study and the regional entropy (RE) [15,103] that Liu et al. [48] utilized. The comparative analysis of RE and DE is an interesting venue for future research to shed further light on their respective degree of correlation with MSE.
The Spearman correlation between DE and MSE1 appeared to suggest a degree of a nonlinear relationship between these two quantities. A potential reason behind the observed effect might be the shared information among different EEG channels with respect to their MSE1. The possibility of higher shared information between DE and MSE1 became more plausible, considering the increased anti-correlation between DE and MSE of higher scales (e.g., MSE20). This might also hint at the possibility of lower scale MSE (e.g., MSE1) to more closely reflect/capture the local information processing [39,81] at the sight of EEG channels. This, in turn, could imply that the changes in the lower scale MSE was more in line (i.e., less anti-correlated) with the signal entropy (i.e., its DE). It is apparent that these potential non-linear relations could not be captured by Spearman correlation (i.e., a linear measure). Therefore, it is crucial to further investigate the nature and potential causes of such variation in the observed anti-correlations through the use of measures that can quantify linear as well as nonlinear relations (e.g., mutual information (MI) [104]).
The small sample size is another limitation of the present work. This concerning issue [58,105,106,107] is best highlighted by Lindquist [66] that found 914 affect-related experimental contrasts included only 6827 participants (i.e., 6827/914 ≈ 7.47 individuals on average). This is further worsened by the fact that many of these studies comprise individuals from the same demographic background (however, see [108] for a small deviation). Although individuals’ ability to experience affect appears to be a unifying and common concept across cultures [109,110], the present results could be further enriched through the inclusion of larger human samples from different age groups and cultural backgrounds. This will, in turn, allow for a thorough comprehension and generalization of the utility of DE, MSE, and PE within the context of the brain variability in response to naturalistic stimuli.
Entropic measures have also been widely used as robust features for classification of the individuals’ mental [45,49]. Interestingly, Liu and colleagues [111] used a considerably large resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data from Human Connectome Project (998 individuals) to demonstrate that the information from cortical entropy profiles could effectively predict diverse facets of human subjects’ cognitive ability. Although the present results pointed at subtle differences between DE and MSE on the one hand, and PE on the other hand, they did not discard their potentials as robust features for quantification of the b374-382rain activity in such applications as brain–computer interface (BCI) [112] and neurofeedback brain training [113]. In this regard, an intriguing venue for future exploration is to consider the combination of these features to utilize their differential power for quantification of the brain variability in such applications.
The adapted non-parametric estimator of DE in the present study utilizes the nearest neighbour distance for its DE estimation [74], thereby achieving a more robust estimate of DE in comparison with histogram-based techniques [26]. It also bypasses the need for imposing an unwarranted distribution on data that is necessary when one opts for parametric (e.g., Gaussian distribution and its closed-form parametric DE computation) approaches [26]. From a computational perspective, the use of nearest neighbour distance by this approach highlights an interesting underlying similarity between DE on the one hand and PE and SE on the other hand. Specifically, DE and PE computations share a basic principle in that they both estimate the entropy of a continuous time-series through its discretization: i.e., neighbourhood formation in the case of DE estimator and the signal’s symbolic representation in the case of PE. In the same vein (although in a much-restricted sense), DE and PE both relate to SE computation through the realization that the latter is based on a discretized (a binary discretization in this case) summary statistics of a given continuous time series. In this respect, it would be interesting for future research to further examine the effect of such discretization strategies on the level of correspondence between the estimated entropy by these algorithms. Another possibility for future research that is worth investigating would be to reevaluate the use of PE for estimation of the brain variability in response to naturalistic stimuli based on its more recent variant i.e., multiscale permutation entropy (MPE) [114].

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the ethical committee of the Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International (ATR). The protocol was approved by the ATR ethical committee (approval code: 0221200707001).

Funding

This work was supported by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Number JP19K20746.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest. The funder had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A. DE vs. PE3: Bootstrap Spearman Correlation

Figure A1 shows the Spearman correlation bootstrap tests (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval between participants’ DE and their corresponding PE values. This test verified the non-significant correlation between DE and PE in the case of neutral and positive affect. Table A1 shows the values associated with Spearman correlation coefficient’s mean (yellow line) and 95.0% confidence interval (red lines) for negative (Figure A1A), neutral (Figure A1B), and positive (Figure A1C) affect. The two-tailed p-value of each test is presented in the column “p” of this table.
Figure A1. Bootstrap Spearman correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE3 values (i.e., 62 PEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. Although PE showed a significant correlation to DE in the negative mental state, such a correlation was non-significant in the case of neutral and positive states.
Figure A1. Bootstrap Spearman correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE3 values (i.e., 62 PEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. Although PE showed a significant correlation to DE in the negative mental state, such a correlation was non-significant in the case of neutral and positive states.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a1
Table A1. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between PEs (i.e., 62 PEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DE (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Table A1. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between PEs (i.e., 62 PEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DE (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
Negative0.32<0.001[0.26 0.38]
Neutral0.05=0.15[−0.018 0.11]
Positive0.05=0.15[−0.017 0.12]

Appendix B. DE vs. PE3: Bootstrap Two-Sample Test of Significance

Figure A2 show the paired two-sample bootstrap test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE3 values (i.e., 62 PE3s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. This figure verifies non-significant differences between the positive and the neutral and the negative and the neutral states. In Table A2, the values associated with mean differences (yellow line) and their corresponding 95% confidence interval for positive vs. neutral (Figure A2A), positive vs. negative (Figure A2B), and negative vs. neutral (Figure A2C) are shown. The column “SD d i f f ” in this table presents the standard deviation associated with each of these paired mean differences.
Figure A2. Paired two-sample bootstrap test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ PE3 values (i.e., 62 PE3s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between PEs of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Figure A2. Paired two-sample bootstrap test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ PE3 values (i.e., 62 PE3s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between PEs of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a2
Table A2. Paired two-sample bootstrap test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE3 values (i.e., 62 PE3s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Table A2. Paired two-sample bootstrap test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE3 values (i.e., 62 PE3s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
ConditionsM diff SD diff 95.0% CI diff
Positive vs. Neutral0.01350.009[−0.003 0.031]
Positive vs. Negative0.0070.008[−0.009 0.023]
Negative vs. Neutral−0.0070.010[−0.026 0.013]

Appendix C. DE vs. MSE1: Bootstrap Spearman Correlation

Figure A3 shows the Spearman correlations bootstrap tests between the participants’ DEs and their corresponding MSE1 (10,000 simulation runs) at the 95.0% confidence interval. Table A3 shows the values associated with Spearman correlation coefficient’s mean (yellow line) and 95.0% confidence interval (red lines) for negative (Figure A3A), neutral (Figure A3B), and positive (Figure A3C) affect. The two-tailed p-value of each test is presented in the column “p” of this table.
Table A3. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DE (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Table A3. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DE (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
Negative−0.50<0.001[−0.56 −0.43]
Neutral−0.13<0.001[−0.20 −0.05]
Positive−0.13<0.001[−0.20 −0.05]
Figure A3. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1 values (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) Positive.
Figure A3. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1 values (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) Positive.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a3

Appendix D. DE vs. MSE1: Bootstrap Two-Sample Test of Significance

The paired two-sample bootstrap test on these MSE1 values further verified that the MSE1s associated with the positive state were higher than the neutral (Figure A4A) and the negative (Figure A4B) states. It also indicated that the neutral state’s MSE1s were higher than the MSE1s in a negative state (Figure A4C). In Table A4, the values associated with mean differences (yellow line) and their corresponding 95% confidence interval for positive vs. neutral (Figure A4A), positive vs. negative (Figure A4B), and negative vs. neutral (Figure A4C) are shown. The column “SD d i f f ” in this table presents the standard deviation associated with each of these paired mean differences.
Figure A4. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSE1s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Figure A4. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSE1s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a4
Table A4. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ MSE1 values (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Table A4. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ MSE1 values (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
ConditionsM diff SD diff 95.0% CI diff
Positive vs. Neutral0.940.020[0.055 0.133]
Positive vs. Negative0.280.023[0.230 0.320]
Negative vs. Neutral−0.180.021[−0.222 −0.139]

Appendix E. DE vs. MSE20: Bootstrap Spearman Correlation

Figure A5 shows the bootstrap Spearman correlations (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. MSE20 was significantly anti-correlated with DE in the negative, neutral, and positive states. Overall, MSE20 anti-correlations were stronger than MSE1 in every mental state. Table A5 shows the values associated with Spearman correlation coefficient’s mean (yellow line) and 95.0% confidence interval (red lines) for negative (Figure A5A), neutral (Figure A5B), and positive (Figure A5C) affect. The two-tailed p-value of each test is presented in the column “p” of this table.
Figure A5. Bootstrap Spearman correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. MSE20 was significantly anti-correlated with DE in the negative, neutral, and positive states. Overall, MSE20 anti-correlations were stronger than MSE1 in every mental state.
Figure A5. Bootstrap Spearman correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. MSE20 was significantly anti-correlated with DE in the negative, neutral, and positive states. Overall, MSE20 anti-correlations were stronger than MSE1 in every mental state.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a5
Table A5. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Table A5. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
Negative−0.56<0.001[−0.47 −0.34]
Neutral−0.41<0.001[−0.47 −0.34]
Positive−0.41<0.001[−0.47 −0.34]

Appendix F. DE vs. MSE20: Bootstrap Two-Sample Test of Significance

The paired two-sample bootstrap test on these states’ MSE20 values further verified that the MSE20s associated with the neutral state were higher than the positive (Figure A6A) and the negative (Figure A6C) states. It also identified that the positive state’s MSE20s were higher than the negative state’s MSE20s (Figure A6B). In Table A6, the values associated with mean differences (yellow line) and their corresponding 95% confidence interval for positive vs. neutral (Figure A6A), positive vs. negative (Figure A6B), and negative vs. neutral (Figure A6C) are shown. The column “SD d i f f ” in this table presents the standard deviation associated with each of these paired mean differences.
Figure A6. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSE20s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Figure A6. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSE20s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a6
Table A6. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Table A6. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
ConditionsM diff SD diff CI diff
Positive vs. Neutral−0.0480.015[−0.078 −0.018]
Positive vs. Negative0.1690.017[0.134 0.202]
Negative vs. Neutral−0.2170.017[−0.249 −0.184]

Appendix G. Correlation: MSE2–MSE19 vs. DE

Left-column subplots in Figure A7, Figure A8 and Figure A9 show the Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs and their corresponding MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, the neutral, and the positive mental states. The right-column subplots in these figures correspond to the bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval in these mental states.
Table A7 (negative state), Table A8 (neutral state), and Table A9 (positive state) show the values associated with Spearman correlation coefficient’s mean (yellow line) and 95.0% confidence interval (red lines) for negative (Figure A7, right subplots), neutral (Figure A8, right subplots), and positive (Figure A9, right subplots) affect. The two-tailed p-values for each of these subplots are presented in the column “p” of their corresponding tables.
Figure A7. Negative mental state. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). The subplots in the right column correspond to bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval. The values associated with these subplots are summarized in Table A7.
Figure A7. Negative mental state. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). The subplots in the right column correspond to bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval. The values associated with these subplots are summarized in Table A7.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a7
Figure A8. Neutral mental state. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). The subplots in the right column correspond to bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval. The values associated with these subplots are summarized in Table A8.
Figure A8. Neutral mental state. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). The subplots in the right column correspond to bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval. The values associated with these subplots are summarized in Table A8.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a8
Figure A9. Positive mental state. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) values. The subplots in the right column correspond to bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval. The values associated with these subplots are summarized in Table A9.
Figure A9. Positive mental state. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) values. The subplots in the right column correspond to bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval. The values associated with these subplots are summarized in Table A9.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a9
Table A7. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative mental states.
Table A7. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
MSE1−0.50<0.001[−0.56 −0.43]
MSE2−0.5408<0.001[−0.60 −0.48]
MSE3−0.5836<0.001[−0.64 −0.52]
MSE4−0.5930<0.001[−0.65 −0.54]
MSE5−0.6073<0.001[−0.66 −0.55]
MSE6−0.6073<0.001[−0.66 −0.55]
MSE7−0.6120<0.001[−0.66 −0.56]
MSE8−0.6051<0.001[−0.65 −0.55]
MSE9−0.6053<0.001[−0.65 −0.55]
MSE10−0.6001<0.001[−0.65 −0.55]
MSE11−0.6000<0.001[−0.65 −0.55]
MSE12−0.5928<0.001[−0.64 −0.54]
MSE13−0.5900<0.001[−0.64 −0.54]
MSE14−0.5840<0.001[−0.63 −0.53]
MSE15−0.5804<0.001[−0.63 −0.53]
MSE16−0.5736<0.001[−0.62 −0.53]
MSE17−0.5724<0.001[−0.63 −0.52]
MSE18−0.5677<0.001[−0.62 −0.52]
MSE19−0.5648<0.001[−0.61 −0.51]
MSE20−0.56<0.001[−0.47 −0.34]
Table A8. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the neutral mental states.
Table A8. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the neutral mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
MSE1−0.13<0.001[−0.20 −0.05]
MSE2−0.1652<0.001[−0.25 −0.09]
MSE3−0.2646<0.001[−0.34 −0.19]
MSE4−0.3191<0.001[−0.39 −0.25]
MSE5−0.3730<0.001[−0.44 −0.30]
MSE6−0.3948<0.001[−0.46 −0.33]
MSE7−0.4226<0.001[−0.48 −0.36]
MSE8−0.4261<0.001[−0.49 −0.36]
MSE9−0.4351<0.001[−0.50 −0.37]
MSE10−0.4320<0.001[−0.49 −0.37]
MSE11−0.4386<0.001[−0.50 −0.38]
MSE12−0.4368<0.001[−0.50 −0.37]
MSE13−0.4371<0.001[−0.50 −0.37]
MSE14−0.4313<0.001[−0.49 −0.37]
MSE15−0.4311<0.001[−0.49 −0.37]
MSE16−0.4232<0.001[−0.48 −0.36]
MSE17−0.4217<0.001[−0.48 −0.36]
MSE18−0.4163<0.001[−0.48 −0.35]
MSE19−0.4185<0.001[−0.48 −0.35]
MSE20−0.41<0.001[−0.47 −0.34]
Table A9. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the positive mental states.
Table A9. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the positive mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
MSE1−0.13<0.001[−0.20 −0.05]
MSE2−0.1652<0.001[−0.24 −0.09]
MSE3−0.2646<0.001[−0.34 −0.19]
MSE4−0.3191<0.001[−0.39 −0.25]
MSE5−0.3730<0.001[−0.44 −0.31]
MSE6−0.3948<0.001[−0.46 −0.33]
MSE7−0.4226<0.001[−0.48 −0.36]
MSE8−0.4261<0.001[−0.49 −0.36]
MSE9−0.4351<0.001[−0.50 −0.37]
MSE10−0.4320<0.001[−0.49 −0.37]
MSE11−0.4386<0.001[−0.50 −0.38]
MSE12−0.4368<0.001[−0.50 −0.37]
MSE13−0.4371<0.001[−0.50 −0.37]
MSE14−0.4313<0.001[−0.49 −0.37]
MSE15−0.4311<0.001[−0.49 −0.37]
MSE16−0.4232<0.001[−0.48 −0.36]
MSE17−0.4217<0.001[−0.48 −0.36]
MSE18−0.4163<0.001[−0.48 −0.35]
MSE19−0.4185<0.001[−0.48 −0.35]
MSE20−0.41<0.001[−0.47 −0.34]

Appendix H. MSE2–MSE19 Two-Sample Bootstrap Test of Significance

Paired two-sample bootstrap test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE2–MSE10 and MSE11–MSE19 are shown in Figure A10 and Figure A11. In Table A10, the values associated with mean differences (yellow line) and their corresponding 95% confidence interval for each of these two figures are shown. The column “SD d i f f ” in these tables pertain to the standard deviation for each of the paired mean differences in Figure A10 and Figure A11.
Figure A10. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE2s (i.e., 62 MSE2s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE10s (i.e., 62 MSE10s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). In each subplot, the compared pairs of mental states are: Top: positive versus neutral, Middle: positive versus negative, and Bottom: negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSEs of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations. The values associated with these figures are summarized in Table A10.
Figure A10. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE2s (i.e., 62 MSE2s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE10s (i.e., 62 MSE10s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). In each subplot, the compared pairs of mental states are: Top: positive versus neutral, Middle: positive versus negative, and Bottom: negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSEs of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations. The values associated with these figures are summarized in Table A10.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a10
Table A10. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ MSE2s (i.e., 62 MSE2s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE19s (i.e., 62 MSE19s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) values. Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Table A10. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ MSE2s (i.e., 62 MSE2s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE19s (i.e., 62 MSE19s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) values. Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Positive vs. NeutralPositive vs. NegativeNegative vs. Neutral
M/SD/CI 95 % M/SD/CI 95 % M/SD/CI 95 %
MSE20.04/0.02/[0.004 0.074]0.23/0.02/[0.19 0.27]−0.19/0.02/[−0.23 −0.16]
MSE3−0.011/0.02/[−0.04 0.02]0.20/0.02/[0.16 0.24]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.17]
MSE4−0.03/0.017/[−0.06 −0.0002]0.18/0.019/[0.14 0.22]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE5−0.049/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.17/0.02/[0.13 0.20]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE6−0.06/0.02/[−0.09 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.12 0.20]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE7−0.06/0.02/[−0.09 −0.03]0.16/0.02/[0.12 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE8−0.06/0.02/[−0.09 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.12 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE9−0.06/0.02/[−0.09 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.12 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE10−0.06/0.02/[−0.09 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.12 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE11−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.12 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE12−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.12 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.24 −0.18]
MSE13−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.13 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE14−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.13 0.19]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE15−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.13 0.20]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE16−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.13 0.20]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE17−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.16/0.02/[0.13 0.20]−0.21/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE18−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.17/0.02/[0.13 0.20]−0.22/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
MSE19−0.05/0.02/[−0.08 −0.02]0.17/0.02/[0.13 0.20]−0.22/0.02/[−0.25 −0.18]
Figure A11. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE11s (i.e., 62 MSE11s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE19s (i.e., 62 MSE19s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). In each subplot, the compared pairs of mental states are: Top: positive versus neutral, Middle: positive versus negative, and Bottom: negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSEs of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations. The values associated with these figures are summarized in Table A10.
Figure A11. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) associated with the participants’ MSE11s (i.e., 62 MSE11s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE19s (i.e., 62 MSE19s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). In each subplot, the compared pairs of mental states are: Top: positive versus neutral, Middle: positive versus negative, and Bottom: negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSEs of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations. The values associated with these figures are summarized in Table A10.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a11

Appendix I. MSE2–MSE19 Grand Averages’ Spatial Maps

Figure A12 and Figure A13 show the grand averages of the participants’ spatial maps for MSE2–MSE19.
Figure A12. Grand averages of spatial maps of participants’ MSE2s (i.e., 62 MSE2s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE10s (i.e., 62 MSE10s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Distribution of MSEs in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one MSE value, per EEG channel).
Figure A12. Grand averages of spatial maps of participants’ MSE2s (i.e., 62 MSE2s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE10s (i.e., 62 MSE10s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Distribution of MSEs in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one MSE value, per EEG channel).
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a12
Figure A13. Grand averages of spatial maps of participants’ MSE11s (i.e., 62 MSE11s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE19s (i.e., 62 MSE19s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Distribution of MSEs in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one MSE value, per EEG channel).
Figure A13. Grand averages of spatial maps of participants’ MSE11s (i.e., 62 MSE11s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) through MSE19s (i.e., 62 MSE19s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Distribution of MSEs in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one MSE value, per EEG channel).
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a13

Appendix J. PE5

The left subplots in Figure A14 show the correlations between participants’ DE and their corresponding PE5 (i.e., order-5 permutation) values. We observed that these correlations were non-significant in all three mental states: negative (r = −0.012, p = 0.71), neutral (r = −0.053, p = 0.11), positive (r = −0.048, p = 0.16). These results were further supported by their corresponding bootstrap tests (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (Figure A14, the right column’s subplots). Table A11 shows the values associated with Spearman correlation coefficient’s mean (yellow line) and 95.0% confidence interval (red lines) for negative (Figure A14A), neutral (Figure A14B), and positive (Figure A14C) affect. The two-tailed p-value of each test is presented in the column “p” of this table.
Figure A14. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE5 values (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) Negative (B) Neutral (C) Positive. The subplots on the right column correspond to the bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at the 95.0% confidence interval.
Figure A14. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE5 values (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) Negative (B) Neutral (C) Positive. The subplots on the right column correspond to the bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at the 95.0% confidence interval.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a14
Table A11. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Table A11. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
Negative−0.0130.71[−0.08 0.05]
Neutral−0.0530.12[−0.12 0.01]
Positive−0.0480.17[−0.11 0.02]
Figure A15A shows the grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE5 in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The Kruskal–Wallis test of the participants’ PE5 showed a significant difference between their affective states (p < 0.001, H(2, 2603) = 169.56, r = 0.26). The posthoc Wilcoxon test (Figure A15B) indicated higher PE5 in the case of positive than neutral (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 12.73, r = 0.31, M P o s i t i v e = 9.07, SD P o s i t i v e = 0.35, M N e u t r a l = 8.85, SD N e u t r a l = 0.42) and negative (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 7.10, r = 0.17, M N e g a t i v e = 8.96, SD N e g a t i v e = 0.40) states. Similarly, the negative state was associated with higher PE5 than the neutral (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 6.48, r = 0.16).
However, only the difference between the positive and the neutral states passed the paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (Figure A16). In Table A12, the values associated with mean differences (yellow line) and their corresponding 95% confidence interval for positive vs. neutral (Figure A16A), positive vs. negative (Figure A16B), and negative vs. neutral (Figure A16C) are shown. The column “SD d i f f ” in this table presents the standard deviation associated with each of these paired mean differences.
Figure A15. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE5 values (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of PE5s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one PE5 value, per EEG channel) (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect).
Figure A15. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE5 values (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of PE5s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one PE5 value, per EEG channel) (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect).
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a15
Figure A16. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at the 95.0% CI associated with the participants’ PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. IX-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between PE5s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Figure A16. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at the 95.0% CI associated with the participants’ PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. IX-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between PE5s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a16
Table A12. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of the difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Table A12. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE5s (i.e., 62 PE5s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of the difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
ConditionsM diff SD diff CI diff
Positive vs. Neutral0.140.02[0.10 0.17]
Positive vs. Negative0.020.02[−0.01 0.05]
Negative vs. Neutral0.030.02[−0.01 0.07]

Appendix K. PE6

Left subplots in Figure A17 show the correlations between participants’ DE and their corresponding PE6 (i.e., order 6 permutation) values. These correlations were non-significant in all three mental states: negative (r = −0.013, p = 0.71), neutral (r = −0.053, p = 0.11), positive (r = −0.048, p = 0.16). These results were further supported by their corresponding bootstrap tests (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% a confidence interval (Figure A17, right column’s subplots). Table A13 shows the values associated with Spearman correlation coefficient’s mean (yellow line) and 95.0% confidence interval (red lines) for negative (Figure A17A), neutral (Figure A17B), and positive (Figure A17C) affect. The two-tailed p-value of each test is presented in the column “p” of this table.
Figure A17. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE6 values (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. The subplots on the right column correspond to the bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval.
Figure A17. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE6 values (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. The subplots on the right column correspond to the bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a17
Table A13. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% CI associated with the Spearman correlation between PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Table A13. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% CI associated with the Spearman correlation between PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
Negative−0.0120.70[−0.08 0.05]
Neutral−0.0530.12[−0.12 0.01]
Positive−0.0480.17[−0.11 0.02]
Figure A18A shows the grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE6 in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The Kruskal–Wallis test of the participants’ PE6 showed a significant difference between their neutral, negative, and positive affective states (p < 0.001, H(2, 2603) = 169.56, r = 0.26). The posthoc Wilcoxon test (Figure A18B) indicated higher PE6 in the case of positive than neutral (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 12.73, r = 0.31, M P o s i t i v e = 9.07, SD P o s i t i v e = 0.35, M N e u t r a l = 8.85, SD N e u t r a l = 0.42) and negative (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 7.10, r = 0.17, M N e g a t i v e = 8.96, SD N e g a t i v e = 0.40) states. Similarly, the negative state was associated with higher PE6 than the neutral (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 6.48, r = 0.16).
Figure A18. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The distribution of PE6s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one PE6 value, per EEG channel). (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect).
Figure A18. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The distribution of PE6s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one PE6 value, per EEG channel). (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect).
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a18
However, only the difference between the positive and the neutral states passed the paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (Figure A19). In Table A14, the values associated with mean differences (yellow line) and their corresponding 95% confidence interval for positive vs. neutral (Figure A19A), positive vs. negative (Figure A19B), and negative vs. neutral (Figure A19C) are shown. The column “SD d i f f ” in this table presents the standard deviation associated with each of these paired mean differences.
Figure A19. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% CI associated with the participants’ PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between PE6s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Figure A19. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% CI associated with the participants’ PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between PE6s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a19
Table A14. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Table A14. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE6s (i.e., 62 PE6s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
ConditionsM diff SD diff CI diff
Positive vs. Neutral0.140.02[0.10 0.17]
Positive vs. Negative0.020.018[−0.01 0.06]
Negative vs. Neutral0.030.02[−0.01 0.06]

Appendix L. PE7

Figure A20 left subplots show the correlations between participants’ DE and their corresponding PE7 (i.e., order-7 permutation). These correlations were non-significant in all three mental states: negative (r = −0.012, p = 0.71), neutral (r = −0.053, p = 0.11), positive (r = −0.048, p = 0.16). These results were further supported by their corresponding bootstrap tests (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (Figure A20, right column’s subplots). Table A15 shows the values associated with Spearman correlation coefficient’s mean (yellow line) and 95.0% confidence interval (red lines) for negative (Figure A20A), neutral (Figure A20B), and positive (Figure A20C) affect. The two-tailed p-value of each test is presented in the column “p” of this table.
Figure A20. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. The subplots on the right column correspond to the bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval.
Figure A20. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive. The subplots on the right column correspond to the bootstrap correlation test (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a20
Figure A21A shows the grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE7 in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The Kruskal–Wallis test of the participants’ PE7 showed a significant difference between their affective states (p < 0.001, H(2, 2603) = 169.56, r = 0.26). The posthoc Wilcoxon test (Figure A21B) indicated higher PE7 in the case of positive than neutral (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 12.73, r = 0.31, M P o s i t i v e = 9.07, SD P o s i t i v e = 0.35, M N e u t r a l = 8.85, SD N e u t r a l = 0.42) and negative (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 7.10, r = 0.17, M N e g a t i v e = 8.96, SD N e g a t i v e = 0.40) states. Similarly, the negative state was associated with higher PE7 than the neutral (p < 0.001, W(1734) = 6.48, r = 0.16).
Table A15. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DE (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Table A15. Bootstrap (10,000 simulation runs) 95.0% confidence intervals (CI) associated with the Spearman correlation between PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and DE (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in the negative, neutral, and positive mental states.
Conditionsrp (Two-Tailed)CI 95 %
Negative−0.0130.70[−0.08 0.05]
Neutral−0.0530.12[−0.12 0.01]
Positive−0.0480.16[−0.11 0.02]
Figure A21. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of PE7s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one PE7 value, per EEG channel). (B) Descriptive statistics of participants’ PE7 in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The asterisks mark the significant difference in these subplots.
Figure A21. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of PE7s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B in the main text (i.e., one PE7 value, per EEG channel). (B) Descriptive statistics of participants’ PE7 in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The asterisks mark the significant difference in these subplots.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a21
However, only the difference between the positive and the neutral states passed the paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (Figure A22). In Table A16, the values associated with mean differences (yellow line) and their corresponding 95% confidence interval for positive vs. neutral (Figure A22A), positive vs. negative (Figure A22B), and negative vs. neutral (Figure A22C) are shown. The column “SD d i f f ” in this table presents the standard deviation associated with each of these paired mean differences.
Figure A22. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% CI associated with the participants’ PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSE1s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Figure A22. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% CI associated with the participants’ PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are (A) positive versus neutral, (B) positive versus negative, and (C) negative versus neutral. X-axis: difference between average values of two affective states (e.g., positive versus negative). Blue line: non-significant difference between MSE1s of two different affective states. Red lines: 95.0% confidence interval boundaries. Yellow line: average two affetective states’ mean differences over 10,000 simulations.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a22
Table A16. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
Table A16. Paired two-sample bootstrap test of significance (10,000 simulation runs) at 95.0% confidence interval (CI) applied on the participants’ PE7s (i.e., 62 PE7s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect). Compared pairs of mental states are: positive versus neutral, positive versus negative, and negative versus neutral. M: mean difference, SD: standard deviation of such a difference. CI: 95% confidence interval for every paired affective states’ difference.
ConditionsM diff SD diff CI diff
Positive vs. Neutral0.140.02[0.10 0.17]
Positive vs. Negative0.020.02[−0.02 0.06]
Negative vs. Neutral0.030.02[−0.01 0.06]

Appendix M. Spearman Correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20

Figure A23, Figure A24 and Figure A25 show the Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. The r- and p-values associated with these figures are shown in Table A17, Table A18 and Table A19.
Figure A23. Negative affect’s Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. (A) Scatter plots of MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 (B) Spearman correlations associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1 (C) Spearman correlations’ p-values associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1.
Figure A23. Negative affect’s Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. (A) Scatter plots of MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 (B) Spearman correlations associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1 (C) Spearman correlations’ p-values associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a23
Figure A24. Neutral affect’s Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. (A) Scatter plots of MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 (B) Spearman correlations associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1 (C) Spearman correlations’ p-values associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1.
Figure A24. Neutral affect’s Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. (A) Scatter plots of MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 (B) Spearman correlations associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1 (C) Spearman correlations’ p-values associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a24
Figure A25. Positive affect’s Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. (A) Scatter plots of MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 (B) Spearman correlations associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1 (C) Spearman correlations’ p-values associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1.
Figure A25. Positive affect’s Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20. (A) Scatter plots of MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 (B) Spearman correlations associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1 (C) Spearman correlations’ p-values associated with MSE2–MSE20 and MSE1.
Brainsci 10 00527 g0a25
Table A17. Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 in negative affect.
Table A17. Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 in negative affect.
MSE1 vs.r p <
MSE20.95550.00001
MSE30.91120.00001
MSE40.85720.00001
MSE50.83110.00001
MSE60.80330.00001
MSE70.79560.00001
MSE80.78260.00001
MSE90.77610.00001
MSE100.76270.00001
MSE110.75680.00001
MSE120.74600.00001
MSE130.74190.00001
MSE140.73750.00001
MSE150.73400.00001
MSE160.72930.00001
MSE170.72980.00001
MSE180.72670.00001
MSE190.72140.00001
MSE200.71830.00001
Table A18. Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 in neutral affect.
Table A18. Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 in neutral affect.
MSE1 vs.r p <
MSE20.92200.0001
MSE30.83230.0001
MSE40.74600.0001
MSE50.70060.0001
MSE60.66050.0001
MSE70.64830.0001
MSE80.63440.0001
MSE90.63510.0001
MSE100.62560.0001
MSE110.62540.0001
MSE120.61840.0001
MSE130.61590.0001
MSE140.60870.0001
MSE150.61330.0001
MSE160.60920.0001
MSE170.60130.0001
MSE180.60170.0001
MSE190.59910.0001
MSE200.59300.0001
Table A19. Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 in positive affect.
Table A19. Spearman correlations between MSE1 versus MSE2–MSE20 in positive affect.
MSE1 vs.rp<
MSE20.95810.0001
MSE30.88030.0001
MSE40.79210.0001
MSE50.72180.0001
MSE60.66080.0001
MSE70.62430.0001
MSE80.59130.0001
MSE90.57250.0001
MSE100.55510.0001
MSE110.54580.0001
MSE120.53290.0001
MSE130.52390.0001
MSE140.51530.0001
MSE150.51050.0001
MSE160.50350.0001
MSE170.50000.0001
MSE180.50170.0001
MSE190.49220.0001
MSE200.48980.0001

References

  1. Garrett, D.D.; Kovacevic, N.; McIntosh, A.R.; Grady, C.L. The importance of being variable. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 4496–4503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Heisz, J.J.; Shedden, J.M.; McIntosh, A.R. Relating brain signal variability to knowledge representation. Neuroimage 2012, 63, 1384–1392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Garrett, D.D.; Samanez-Larkin, G.R.; MacDonald, S.W.; Lindenberger, U.; McIntosh, A.R.; Grady, C.L. Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: A next frontier in human brain mapping? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 610–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Pouget, A.; Drugowitsch, J.; Kepecs, A. Confidence and certainty: Distinct probabilistic quantities for different goals. Nat. Neurosci. 2016, 19, 366–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Friston, K. The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 127–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Stein, R.B.; Gossen, E.R.; Jones, K.E. Neuronal variability: Noise or part of the signal? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 389–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Faisal, A.A.; Selen, L.P.; Wolpert, D.M. Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 292–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Avena-Koenigsberger, A.; Misic, B.; Sporns, O. Communication dynamics in complex brain networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018, 19, 17–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Muller, L.; Chavane, F.; Reynolds, J.; Sejnowski, T.J. Cortical travelling waves: Mechanisms and computational principles. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018, 19, 255–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bak, P.; Tang, C.; Wiesenfeld, K. Self-organized criticality: An explanation of the 1 f noise. Phys. Rev. Lett. 1987, 59, 381–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Shew, W.L.; Plenz, D. The functional benefits of criticality in the cortex. Neuroscientist 2013, 19, 88–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Fagerholm, E.D.; Lorenz, R.; Scott, G.; Dinov, M.; Hellyer, P.J.; Mirzaei, N.; Leeson, C.; Carmichael, D.W.; Sharp, D.J.; Shew, W.L.; et al. Cascades and cognitive state: Focused attention incurs subcritical dynamics. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 4626–4634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Palva, S.; Palva, J.M. Roles of brain criticality and multiscale oscillations in temporal predictions for sensorimotor processing. Trends Neurosci. 2018, 41, 729–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Laughlin, S. A simple coding procedure enhances a neuron’s information capacity. Zeitschrift für Naturforschung 1981, 36, 910–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Shew, W.L.; Yang, H.; Yu, S.; Roy, R.; Plenz, D. Information capacity and transmission are maximized in balanced cortical networks with neuronal avalanches. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Miller, G.A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 1956, 63, 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Strong, S.P.; Koberle, R.; de Ruyter van Steveninck, R.R.; Bialek, W. Entropy and information in neural spike trains. Phys. Rev. Lett. 1998, 80, 197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Borst, A.; Theunissen, F.E. Information theory and neural coding. Nat. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 947–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sharpee, T.O.; Calhoun, A.J.; Chalasani, S.H. Information theory of adaptation in neurons, behavior, and mood. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014, 25, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Tononi, G.; McIntosh, A.R.; Russell, D.P.; Edelman, G.M. Functional clustering: Identifying strongly interactive brain regions in neuroimaging data. Neuroimage 1998, 7, 133–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Demertzi, A.; Tagliazucchi, E.; Dehaene, S.; Deco, G.; Barttfeld, P.; Raimondo, F.; Martial, C.; Fernández-Espejo, D.; Rohaut, B.; Voss, H.U.; et al. Human consciousness is supported by dynamic complex patterns of brain signal coordination. Sci. Adv. 2019, 5, eaat7603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Carhart-Harris, R.L. The entropic brain-revisited. Neuropharmacology 2018, 142, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Quiroga, R.Q.; Panzeri, S. Extracting information from neuronal populations: Information theory and decoding approaches. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 173–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Tononi, G.; Boly, M.; Massimini, M.; Koch, C. Integrated information theory: From consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016, 17, 450–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Zhang, Y.C. Complexity and 1/f noise. A phase space approach. J. Phys. I 1991, 1, 971–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Xiong, W.; Faes, L.; Ivanov, P.C. Entropy measures, entropy estimators, and their performance in quantifying complex dynamics: Effects of artifacts, nonstationarity, and long-range correlations. Phys. Rev. E 2017, 96, 062114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Pincus, S.M. Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1991, 88, 2297–2301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Richman, J.S.; Moorman, J.R. Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 2000, 278, H2039–H2049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Costa, M.; Goldberger, A.L.; Peng, C.K. Multiscale entropy analysis of complex physiologic time series. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2002, 89, 068102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Bandt, C.; Pompe, B. Permutation entropy: A natural complexity measure for time series. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2002, 88, 174102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Courtiol, J.; Perdikis, D.; Petkoski, S.; Mïler, V.; Huys, R.; Sleimen-Malkoun, R.; Jirsa, V.K. The multiscale entropy: Guidelines for use and interpretation in brain signal analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2016, 273, 175–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Goldberger, A.; Amaral, L.; Glass, L.; Hausdorff, J.; Ivanov, P.; Mark, P. Physiobank, physiotoolkit, and physionet: Components of a new research resource for complex physiologic signals. Circulation 2000, 88, e215–e220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Takahashi, T.; Cho, R.Y.; Mizuno, T.; Kikuchi, M.; Murata, T.; Takahashi, K.; Wada, Y. Antipsychotics reverse abnormal EEG complexity in drug-naive schizophrenia: A multiscale entropy analysis. Neuroimage 2010, 51, 173–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Okazaki, R.; Takahashi, T.; Ueno, K.; Takahashi, K.; Higashima, M.; Wada, Y. Effects of electroconvulsive therapy on neural complexity in patients with depression: Report of three cases. J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 150, 389–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Bosl, W.; Tierney, A.; Tager-Flusberg, H.; Nelson, C. EEG complexity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC Med. 2011, 9, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Ghanbari, Y.; Bloy, L.; Edgar, J.C.; Blaskey, L.; Verma, R.; Roberts, T.P. Joint analysis of band-specific functional connectivity and signal complexity in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 9, 444–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Yang, A.C.; Wang, S.J.; Lai, K.L.; Tsai, C.F.; Yang, C.H.; Hwang, J.P. Cognitive and neuropsychiatric correlates of EEG dynamic complexity in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiat. 2015, 47, 52–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Mizuno, T.; Takahashi, T.; Cho, R.Y.; Kikuchi, M.; Murata, T.; Takahashi, K.; Wada, Y. Assessment of EEG dynamical complexity in Alzheimer’s disease using multiscale entropy. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2010, 121, 1438–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. McIntosh, A.R.; Kovacevic, N.; Itier, R.J. Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2008, 4, e1000106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Yang, A.C.; Huang, C.C.; Yeh, H.L.; Liu, M.E.; Hong, C.J.; Tu, P.C.; Chen, J.F.; Huang, N.E.; Peng, C.K.; Lin, C.P.; et al. Complexity of spontaneous BOLD activity in default mode network is correlated with cognitive function in normal male elderly: A multiscale entropy analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 2013, 34, 428–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Miskovic, V.; MacDonald, K.J.; Rhodes, L.J.; Cote, K.A. Changes in EEG multiscale entropy and power? Law frequency scaling during the human sleep cycle. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019, 40, 538–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Shannon, C.E. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 1948, 27, 379–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Bandt, C.; Shiha, F. Order patterns in time series. J. Time Ser. Anal. 2007, 28, 646–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bian, C.; Qin, C.; Ma, Q.D.; Shen, Q. Modified permutation-entropy analysis of heartbeat dynamics. Phys. Rev. E 2012, 85, 021906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Cao, Y.; Tung, W.W.; Gao, J.B.; Protopopescu, V.A.; Hively, L.M. Detecting dynamical changes in time series using the permutation entropy. Phys. Rev. E 2004, 70, 046217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Olofsen, E.; Sleigh, J.W.; Dahan, A. Permutation entropy of the electroencephalogram: A measure of anaesthetic drug effect. Br. J. Anaesth. 2008, 101, 810–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Silva, A.; Cardoso-Cruz, H.; Silva, F.; Galhardo, V.; Antunes, L. Comparison of anesthetic depth indexes based on thalamocortical local field potentials in rats. Anesthesiology. J. Am. Soc. Anesthesiol. 2010, 112, 355–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Liu, M.; Song, C.; Liang, Y.; Knöpfel, T.; Zhou, C. Assessing spatiotemporal variability of brain spontaneous activity by multiscale entropy and functional connectivity. NeuroImage 2019, 198, 198–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Zheng, W.L.; Lu, B.L. Investigating critical frequency bands and channels for EEG-based emotion recognition with deep neural networks. IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 2015, 7, 162–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Eysenck, S.B.; Eysenck, H.J.; Barrett, P. A revised version of the psychoticism scale. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 1985, 6, 1170–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Philippot, P. Inducing and assessing differentiated emotion-feeling states in the laboratory. Cognit. Emot. 1993, 7, 171–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Lu, Y.; Zheng, W.L.; Li, B.; Lu, B.L. Combining eye movements and EEG to enhance emotion recognition. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Buenos Aires, Argentina, 25–31 July 2015; pp. 1170–1176. [Google Scholar]
  53. Riedl, M.; Mïler, A.; Wessel, N. Practical considerations of permutation entropy. Eur. Phys. J. Spec. Top. 2013, 222, 249–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Yang, A.C.; Tsai, S.J.; Lin, C.P.; Peng, C.K. A Strategy to Reduce Bias of Entropy Estimates in Resting-State fMRI Signals. Front. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Hamilton, J.D. Time Series Analysis; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kwiatkowski, D.; Phillips, P.C.; Schmidt, P.; Shin, Y. Testing the null hypothesis of stationarity against the alternative of a unit root. J. Economet. 1992, 54, 159–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Chen, J.; Leong, Y.C.; Honey, C.J.; Yong, C.H.; Norman, K.A.; Hasson, U. Shared memories reveal shared structure in neural activity across individuals. Nat. Neurosci. 2017, 20, 115–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Zadbood, A.; Chen, J.; Leong, Y.C.; Norman, K.A.; Hasson, U. How we transmit memories to other brains: Constructing shared neural representations via communication. Cerebral Cortex 2017, 27, 4988–5000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Shen, H. Portrait of a memory. Nature 2018, 553, 146–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Avery, E.W.; Yoo, K.; Rosenberg, M.D.; Greene, A.S.; Gao, S.; Na, D.L.; Scheinost, D.; Constable, T.R.; Chun, M.M. Distributed Patterns of Functional Connectivity Predict Working Memory Performance in Novel Healthy and Memory-impaired Individuals. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2019, 32, 241–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Saarimäki, H.; Gotsopoulos, A.; Jääskeläinen, I.P.; Lampinen, J.; Vuilleumier, P.; Hari, R.; Sams, M.; Nummenmaa, L. Discrete neural signatures of basic emotions. Cerebral Cortex 2015, 26, 2563–2573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Farroni, T.; Menon, E.; Rigato, S.; Johnson, M.H. The perception of facial expressions in newborns. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 4, 2–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Watson, D.; Tellegen, A. Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol. Bull. 1985, 98, 219–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Barrett, F.L.; Russell, J.A. Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 967–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Miller, E.K.; Nieder, A.; Freedman, D.J.; Wallis, J.D. Neural correlates of categories and concepts. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2003, 13, 198–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Lindquist, K.A.; Satpute, A.B.; Wager, T.D.; Weber, J.; Barrett, L.F. The brain basis of positive and negative affect: Evidence from a meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cerebral Cortex 2015, 26, 1910–1922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Keshmiri, S.; Shiomi, M.; Ishiguro, H. Entropy of the Multi-Channel EEG Recordings Identifies the Distributed Signatures of Negative, Neutral and Positive Affect in Whole-Brain Variability. Entropy 2019, 21, 1228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Smith, A.P.R.; Henson, R.N.A.; Dolan, R.J.; Rugg, M.D. fMRI correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts. Neuroimage 2004, 22, 868–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Haynes, J.D.; Sakai, K.; Rees, G.; Gilbert, S.; Frith, C.; Passingham, R.E. Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 323–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Paus, T.; Zatorre, R.J.; Hofle, N.; Caramanos, Z.; Gotman, J.; Petrides, M.; Evans, A.C. Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 1997, 9, 392–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Damasio, A.; Carvalho, G.B. The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 340, 143–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Kober, H.; Barrett, L.F.; Joseph, J.; Bliss-Moreau, E.; Lindquist, K.; Wager, T.D. Functional grouping and cortical/subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage 2008, 42, 998–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Spüler, M. Questioning the evidence for BCI-based communication in the complete locked-in state. PLoS Biol. 2019, 17, e2004750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Kozachenko, L.F.; Leonenko, N.N. Sample estimate of entropy of a random vector. Probl. Inf. Trans. 1987, 23, 95–101. [Google Scholar]
  75. Beirlant, J.; Dudewicz, E.J.; Györfi, L.; Van der Meulen, E.C. Nonparametric entropy estimation: An overview. Int. J. Math. Stat. Sci. 1997, 6, 17–39. [Google Scholar]
  76. Lizier, J.T. JIDT: An information-theoretic toolkit for studying the dynamics of complex systems. Front. Robot. AI 2014, 1, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Little, D.J.; Kane, D.M. Permutation entropy of finite-length white-noise time series. Phys. Rev. E 2016, 94, 022118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Cuesta-Frau, D.; Murillo-Escobar, J.P.; Orrego, D.A.; Delgado-Trejos, E. Embedded Dimension and Time Series Length. Practical Influence on Permutation Entropy and Its Applications. Entropy 2019, 21, 385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Rosenthal, R.; DiMatteo, M.R. Meta-analysis: Recent developments n quantitative methods for literature reviews. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2001, 52, 59–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Tomczak, M.; Tomczak, E. The need to report effect size estimates revisited. An overview of some recommended measures of effect size. Trends Sport Sci. 2014, 1, 19–25. [Google Scholar]
  81. Vakorin, V.A.; Lippé, S.; McIntosh, A.R. Variability of brain signals processed locally transforms into higher connectivity with brain development. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 6405–6413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. McIntosh, A.R.; Vakorin, V.; Kovacevic, N.; Wang, H.; Diaconescu, A.; Protzner, A.B. Spatiotemporal dependency of age-related changes in brain signal variability. Cerebral Cortex 2014, 24, 1806–1817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Keller, K.; Unakafov, A.M.; Unakafova, V.A. On the relation of KS entropy and permutation entropy. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 2012, 241, 1477–1481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Haruna, T.; Nakajima, K. Permutation complexity via duality between values and orderings. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 2011, 240, 1370–1377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Vyšata, O.; Schätz, M.; Kopal, J.; Burian, J.; Prochâzka, A.; Jiri, K.; Hort, J.; Valixsx, M. Non-Linear EEG measures in meditation. J. Biomed. Sci. Eng. 2014, 7, 731–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Sumioka, H.; Keshmiri, S.; Ishiguro, H. Information-theoretic investigation of impact of huggable communication medium on prefrontal brain activation. Adv. Robot. 2019, 33, 1019–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Kakumanu, R.J.; Nair, A.K.; Venugopal, R.; Sasidharan, A.; Ghosh, P.K.; John, J.P.; Mehrotra, S.; Panth, R.; Kutty, B.M. Dissociating meditation proficiency and experience dependent EEG changes during traditional Vipassana meditation practice. Biol. Psychol. 2018, 135, 65–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Keshmiri, S.; Sumioka, H.; Yamazaki, R.; Ishiguro, H. Multiscale Entropy Quantifies the Differential Effect of the Medium Embodiment on Older Adults Prefrontal Cortex during the Story Comprehension: A Comparative Analysis. Entropy 2019, 21, 199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Keshmiri, S.; Sumioka, H.; Yamazaki, R.; Ishiguro, H. Differential Effect of the Physical Embodiment on the Prefrontal Cortex Activity as Quantified by Its Entropy. Entropy 2019, 21, 875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Henschel, A.; Hortensius, R.; Cross, E.S. Social Cognition in the Age of Human-Robot Interaction. Trends Neurosci. 2020, 43, 373–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Rauchbauer, B.; Nazarian, B.; Bourhis, M.; Ochs, M.; Prévot, L.; Chaminade, T. Brain activity during reciprocal social interaction investigated using conversational robots as control condition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2020, 374, 20180033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Frith, C.D.; Frith, U. Interacting minds—A biological basis. Science 1999, 286, 1692–1695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Dennett, D.C. The Intentional Stance; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1989; p. 286. [Google Scholar]
  94. Chevallier, C.; Kohls, G.; Troiani, V.; Brodkin, E.S.; Schultz, R.T. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Neurosci. 2012, 16, 231–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  95. Koster-Hale, J.; Saxe, R. Theory of mind: A neural prediction problem. Neuron 2013, 79, 836–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  96. Saxe, R.; Wexler, A. Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 1391–1399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Klapper, A.; Ramsey, R.; Wigboldus, D.; Cross, E.S. The control of automatic imitation based on bottom-up and top-down cues to animacy: Insights from brain and behavior. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2014, 26, 2503–2513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  98. Price, C.J. A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage 2012, 26, 816–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  100. Han, Y.; Kebschull, J.M.; Campbell, R.A.A.; Cowan, D.; Imhof, F.; Zador, A.M.; Mrsic-Flogel, T.D. The logic of single-cell projections from visual cortex. Nature 2018, 556, 51–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Schreiber, T. Measuring information transfer. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2000, 85, 461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Seth, A.K.; Barrett, A.B.; Barnett, L. Granger causality analysis in neuroscience and neuroimaging. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 3293–3297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Fagerholm, E.D.; Scott, G.; Shew, W.L.; Song, C.; Leech, R.; Knopfel, T.; Sharp, D.J. Cortical entropy, mutual information and scale-free dynamics in waking mice. Cerebral Cortex 2016, 26, 3945–3952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Cover, T.M.; Thomas, J.A. Elements of Information Theory, 2nd ed.; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  105. Kragel, P.A.; Reddan, M.C.; LaBar, K.S.; Wager, T.D. Emotion schemas are embedded in the human visual system. Sci. Adv. 2019, 5, eaaw4358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Baucom, L.B.; Wedell, D.H.; Wang, J.; Blitzer, D.N.; Shinkareva, S.V. Decoding the neural representation of affective states. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 718–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Mitchell, T.M.; Shinkareva, S.V.; Carlson, A.; Chang, K.M.; Malave, V.L.; Mason, R.A.; Just, M.A. Predicting human brain activity associated with the meanings of nouns. Science 2008, 320, 1191–1195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Jääskeläinen, I.P.; Koskentalo, K.; Balk, M.H.; Autti, T.; Kauramäki, J.; Pomren, C.; Sams, M. Inter-subject synchronization of prefrontal cortex hemodynamic activity during natural viewing. Open Neuroim. J. 2008, 2, 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Osgood, C.E. The nature and measurement of meaning. Psychol. Bull. 1952, 49, 197–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  110. Wierzbicka, A. Semantics, Culture, and Cognition: Universal Human Concepts in Culture-Specific Configurations; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  111. Liu, M.; Liu, X.; Hildebrt, A.; Zhou, C. Individual cortical entropy profile: Test? Retest reliability, predictive power for cognitive ability, and neuroanatomical foundation. Cerebral Cortex Commun. 2020, 1, tgaa015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Ramadan, R.A.; Vasilakos, A.V. Brain computer interface: Control signals review. Neurocomputing 2017, 223, 26–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Sitaram, R.; Ros, T.; Stoeckel, L.; Haller, S.; Scharnowski, F.; Lewis-Peacock, J.; Weiskopf, N.; Blefari, M.L.; Rana, M.; Oblak, E.; et al. Closed-loop brain training: The science of neurofeedback. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 86–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Dávalos, A.; Jabloun, M.; Ravier, P.; Buttelli, O. On the Statistical Properties of Multiscale Permutation Entropy: Characterization of the Estimator’s Variance. Entropy 2019, 21, 450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. (A) Shanghai Jiao Tong University (SJTU) Emotion EEG Dataset (SEED) [49] paradigm. Each setting, per participant, comprised fifteen four-minute movie clips (i.e., n = 15). There was a five-second hint before the start of each clip. After each clip, participants answered (Likert 5-point scale) three questions that assessed their emotion-feeling states [51]. These questions included participants’ actually felt emotion while watching the clip, have they previously watched the fully movie that the clip corresponded to, and how well they comprehended the clip’s content. (B) Sixty-two EEG channels’ arrangement.
Figure 1. (A) Shanghai Jiao Tong University (SJTU) Emotion EEG Dataset (SEED) [49] paradigm. Each setting, per participant, comprised fifteen four-minute movie clips (i.e., n = 15). There was a five-second hint before the start of each clip. After each clip, participants answered (Likert 5-point scale) three questions that assessed their emotion-feeling states [51]. These questions included participants’ actually felt emotion while watching the clip, have they previously watched the fully movie that the clip corresponded to, and how well they comprehended the clip’s content. (B) Sixty-two EEG channels’ arrangement.
Brainsci 10 00527 g001
Figure 2. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE values (i.e., 62 permutation entropies (PE) associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive.
Figure 2. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding PE values (i.e., 62 permutation entropies (PE) associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive.
Brainsci 10 00527 g002
Figure 3. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PEs in negative, neutral, and positive affect states. These subplots identify that PEs associated with these mental states exhibited highly resembling patterns that were approximately uniformly distributed over all brain regions. (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank-sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain PEs (i.e., 62 PEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive affect states. The asterisks mark the significant differences in these subplots.
Figure 3. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ PEs in negative, neutral, and positive affect states. These subplots identify that PEs associated with these mental states exhibited highly resembling patterns that were approximately uniformly distributed over all brain regions. (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank-sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain PEs (i.e., 62 PEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive affect states. The asterisks mark the significant differences in these subplots.
Brainsci 10 00527 g003
Figure 4. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding multiscale entropy (MSE)1 values (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) Negative (B) Neutral (C) Positive.
Figure 4. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding multiscale entropy (MSE)1 values (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) Negative (B) Neutral (C) Positive.
Brainsci 10 00527 g004
Figure 5. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of MSE1s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B (i.e., one MSE1 value, per EEG channel) Differential patterns of the participants’ MSE1s that show an increase from negative to positive states is evident in these subplots. (B) Descriptive statistics of participants’ MSE1 in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The asterisks mark the significant differences in these subplots.
Figure 5. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ MSE1s (i.e., 62 MSE1s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of MSE1s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B (i.e., one MSE1 value, per EEG channel) Differential patterns of the participants’ MSE1s that show an increase from negative to positive states is evident in these subplots. (B) Descriptive statistics of participants’ MSE1 in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. The asterisks mark the significant differences in these subplots.
Brainsci 10 00527 g005
Figure 6. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive.
Figure 6. Spearman correlation between participants’ DEs (i.e., 62 DEs associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) and their corresponding MSE20 values (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in (A) negative, (B) neutral, and (C) positive.
Brainsci 10 00527 g006
Figure 7. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of MSE20s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B (i.e., one MSE20 value, per EEG channel). These subplots identify a pattern in which MSE20s associated with the neutral state is higher than the negative as well as the positive state. Similar to participants’ MSE1s, the negative state’s MSE20s are below those of the neutral and the positive states. (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank-sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect).
Figure 7. (A) Grand averages of the spatial maps of participants’ MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect) in negative, neutral, and positive mental states. Distribution of MSE20s in these spatial maps corresponds to the EEG channels’ arrangement in Figure 1B (i.e., one MSE20 value, per EEG channel). These subplots identify a pattern in which MSE20s associated with the neutral state is higher than the negative as well as the positive state. Similar to participants’ MSE1s, the negative state’s MSE20s are below those of the neutral and the positive states. (B) Descriptive statistics of the posthoc Wilcoxon rank-sum tests applied on participants’ whole-brain MSE20s (i.e., 62 MSE20s associated with 62 EEG channels of a participant, per affect).
Brainsci 10 00527 g007

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Keshmiri, S. Comparative Analysis of the Permutation and Multiscale Entropies for Quantification of the Brain Signal Variability in Naturalistic Scenarios. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 527. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10080527

AMA Style

Keshmiri S. Comparative Analysis of the Permutation and Multiscale Entropies for Quantification of the Brain Signal Variability in Naturalistic Scenarios. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(8):527. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10080527

Chicago/Turabian Style

Keshmiri, Soheil. 2020. "Comparative Analysis of the Permutation and Multiscale Entropies for Quantification of the Brain Signal Variability in Naturalistic Scenarios" Brain Sciences 10, no. 8: 527. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci10080527

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop