Next Article in Journal
COVID-19 Neuromuscular Involvement in Post-Acute Rehabilitation
Next Article in Special Issue
Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers
Previous Article in Journal
Surgical Aspects of Corpus Callosotomy
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Math Anxiety Is Related to Math Difficulties and Composed of Emotion Regulation and Anxiety Predisposition: A Network Analysis Study

by
Lital Daches Cohen
1,2,
Nachshon Korem
1,3 and
Orly Rubinsten
1,2,*
1
Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center for the Study of Learning Disabilities, Haifa 3498838, Israel
2
Department of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa 3498838, Israel
3
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 29 October 2021 / Revised: 2 December 2021 / Accepted: 3 December 2021 / Published: 5 December 2021

Abstract

:
Current evidence suggests emotion regulation is an important factor in both math anxiety and math performance, but the interplay between these constructs is unexamined. Given the multicomponent structure of math anxiety, emotion regulation, and math performance, here, we aimed to provide a comprehensive model of the underlying nature of the links between these latent variables. Using the innovative network analysis approach, the study visualized the underlying links between directly observable and measurable variables that might be masked by traditional statistical approaches. One hundred and seventeen adults completed a battery of tests and questionnaires on math anxiety, emotion regulation, and math performance. The results revealed: (1) state math anxiety (the emotional experience in math-related situations), rather than trait math anxiety, was linked to anxiety predisposition, subjective valence of math information, and difficulties in emotion regulation; (2) the link between state math anxiety and math performance partialed out the link between trait math anxiety and performance. The study innovatively demonstrates the need to differentiate between traits and tendencies to the actual emotional experience and emotion regulation used in math anxiety. The results have important implications for the theoretical understanding of math anxiety and future discussions and work in the field.

1. Introduction

Math anxiety is a common phenomenon [1] characterized by negative attitudes toward math [2,3], as well as feelings of stress, frustration, and fear when thinking about or engaging in situations involving numerical information [3,4,5]. Researchers have reported inconsistent results for the direction of the link between math anxiety and performance, e.g., [6,7,8]. A possible explanation often cited by intervention studies suggests that emotion regulation can explain the ability of some math-anxious individuals to perform well, e.g., [9,10,11]; however, see [12]. An additional factor that can explain the inconsistent results for the link between math anxiety and performance is that most studies have assessed a single math-anxiety variable, when, in fact, there are multidimensional interrelationships between many heterogeneous components [13], such as state and trait disposition [14].
Against this background, we wished to provide a comprehensive model of the underlying nature of the links between math anxiety, emotion regulation, and math performance. Given the multicomponent structure of math anxiety (i.e., state and trait math anxiety), emotion regulation (i.e., daily and spontaneous use of emotion-regulation strategies in math contexts), and math performance (i.e., distinct forms of math, such as calculation and fluency) [15], the innovative network analysis is a highly appropriate approach. Network analysis, a new framework for analyzing complex and abstract constructs, has recently gained traction in the field of mental disorders [16]. Our aims were twofold: (1) to reveal the relative and unique contribution of each measurable score/test to the complex network of interacting components that might be masked by traditional statistical approaches or interpreted in terms of broad, unobserved latent variables [17], and (2) to visualize the dynamics among constituent elements in the network [18].

1.1. State and Trait Math Anxiety

Although related to other anxiety subtypes, including general [19,20] and test anxiety [21,22], math anxiety is a unique anxiety disorder with a specific profile [21,23,24] and a distinct pattern of neural activity, e.g., [25]. Math anxiety was found to be linked to avoidance of math-related careers [3,26,27,28], increased health costs [29], reduced financial literacy [30], and low socioeconomic status [31]. The causes of math anxiety may be both intrinsic and environmental [32]. Intrinsic factors include brain malfunctions [33], genetic roots [34], and a tendency toward anxiety in general [19,20,35]. The proposed environmental causes include adverse pedagogical and social math-related events with teachers [36] and parents [37].
In line with the State–Trait Anxiety model [38], trait math anxiety refers to an acquired and sustained individual tendency to perceive math-related situations as threatening, whereas state math anxiety is a temporary anxiety response to specific situations which involves increased arousal of the autonomic nervous system. Self-report questionnaires that typically assess anxiety levels in hypothetical/retrospective math-related situations are the primary method for evaluating trait math anxiety [39]. In contrast, state math-anxiety assessments include real-time self-reports on the actual affective experience (i.e., current anxiety) in specific situations [40]. An additional indicator of state math anxiety may be the degree of pleasant and unpleasant feelings triggered by a math task (i.e., subjective valence) [41].
Although more vulnerable, people with trait anxiety do not necessarily experience state anxiety [42,43]. Accordingly, subjective self-report questionnaires do not always converge with objective unconscious cognitive constructs of math anxiety [37]. Thus, our study included self-reports on anxiety states in hypothetical/retrospective math-related situations (i.e., trait math anxiety) and real-time reports on current anxiety in situations involving numerical information (i.e., state math anxiety).

1.2. Anxiety Predispositions in Math-Anxious Individuals

The literature suggests the predisposition to general anxiety disorder is a risk factor for developing math anxiety [19,20,35]. For example, math-anxious individuals demonstrated general-anxiety-related neuro-physiological responses [44] and maladaptive threat-related attentional bias [45,46]. Moreover, genetic studies have found that general anxiety explained 9% of the total variance in math anxiety [34,47], and general and math anxiety shared about 20% of their genetic variance [48]. In a similar vein, a longitudinal path model showed general anxiety disorder was a significant predictor of math performance [44]. Yet, the etiology of math anxiety and the role general anxiety is playing in math anxiety are still missing [49].

1.3. Math Anxiety and Performance

Math anxiety has consistently been shown to be negatively related to math performance [50,51] within and across countries in PISA tests [52,53], with a stronger link in females [22]. This relationship is stable even when controlling for cognitive abilities, e.g., [3], working memory capacity [54], processing speed [55], and general anxiety [56]. In addition, no significant relationships between math anxiety and domain-remote skills such as reading, were evident [3]. Recent evidence suggests that state (or state-like) math anxiety, e.g., [6,44,57,58,59] is more associated with math performance than trait math anxiety, e.g., [40,58,60,61,62,63], and the link between math anxiety and math performance is stronger in more complex math tests [64,65,66]. However, only a small number of studies used multiple measures of math performance [67].
Findings on the direction of causality between math anxiety and performance suggest a reciprocal link [57,68]. Math anxiety predicts decreased achievements [44,64], which, in turn, predict the development of math anxiety [44,69,70]. This reciprocal relationship between math anxiety and performance may be driven by an avoidance behavior which increases emotional [71,72] and learning problems [57,73,74]. However, this vicious cycle can be interrupted [6,7,8], a phenomenon that may be explained by emotion-regulation processes [7,10,33]. For example, increased activation in the fronto-parietal network associated with cognitive control and emotion regulation prior to a math task leads to higher achievement among highly math-anxious individuals [75]. Given the interesting findings on the role of emotion regulation in coping with math anxiety, we wished to further investigate the interplay between math anxiety, emotion regulation, and math performance while considering general anxiety.

1.4. Links between Emotion Regulation and Math Anxiety

Emotion regulation is a mental process that consciously or unconsciously [76,77] affects the type, duration, intensity, and expression of emotions [78,79] in order to accommodate environmental demands [79]. Two widely studied emotion-regulation strategies are cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression [79,80]. Cognitive reappraisal constitutes an antecedent-focused strategy that aims to modify thoughts and beliefs about a stimulus or situation in a way that alters the emotional response to it [79]. A habitual use of reappraisal leads to a decrease in the subjective experience of negative emotions and an increase in adaptive responses to emotionally evocative events [79,81]. Expressive suppression, in contrast, is a response-focused strategy in which the individual attempts to conceal his/her feelings, behaviors, and physiological activity. People who tend to use suppression in daily life are reported to have decreased subjective positive effect [79]. Not surprisingly, then, suppression is consistently associated with anxiety disorders, whereas reappraisal has been linked to increased resilience to the development of anxiety disorders [82,83,84,85].
Previous studies have indicated that the ability to regulate emotions predicts the severity of anxiety-related symptoms up to five years [86] and also predicts subsequent achievements [87], suggesting it may play a central role in the relations between math anxiety and math performance. Indeed, math anxiety involves difficulties in emotion regulation [7,33,88,89,90]. For example, in one study, highly math-anxious individuals demonstrated decreased activation in areas necessary for solving math problems (i.e., the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and increased activation in competing networks related to emotional processing (i.e., the default mode network) [25].
By manipulating emotion regulation, previous studies showed that reappraisal can reduce math-anxiety reactions [8,91,92] and improve math performance [8,9,10,11,91,93]; however, see [12].People implement multiple regulatory strategies in any given emotional episode [94], a phenomenon termed emotion polyregulation [95].
In our study, we innovatively investigated the way trait (daily) and state (actual use of) emotion-regulation strategies during a math test and trait and state math anxiety interacted with each other simultaneously and affected performance in calculation accuracy and math fluency tests.

1.5. The Current Study

The construct of math anxiety has received increasing attention in recent years [23] because of its close relations with math performance [3,57] and its far-reaching consequences, e.g., [3,27,30]. Emotion regulation, particularly reappraisal, has been shown to be effective in reducing math-anxiety reactions [8,91,92] and in improving math performance [8,9,10,11,91,93]; however, see [12]. To strengthen the effectiveness of interventions, it is necessary to ecologically describe the drivers of the link between the latent variables, and to assess the different contributions of trait and state measures of math anxiety, emotion regulation, general anxiety, and math performance to the network.
Through ecological descriptive research and the use of a powerful statistical approach, we investigated the correlations between state and trait math anxiety, state and trait emotion regulation, and performance in distinct forms of math, and analyzed the unique contribution of each observable measure to the larger network. We also assessed and visualized the dynamics among constituent elements of this complex network [18]. The findings complement those of classic models that collapsed these complex traits into abstract variables [96] and provide a comprehensive model of the underlying nature of the links between math anxiety, emotion regulation, general anxiety, and math performance.
In line with the reviewed literature, we hypothesized that: (1) math anxiety would be associated with difficulties in emotion regulation [7,8,25,33,88,89,90] and with state and trait general anxiety [35,44,48]; (2) the link between math anxiety and performance would be stronger for state, e.g., [6,44,58] rather than trait math anxiety, e.g., [40,58]; (3) math performance would be positively linked to reappraisal [8,9,10,11,91,93]; however, see [12].

2. Method

2.1. Participants

We calculated sample size according to the previously reported correlation between state math anxiety and math achievements [97]. Based on r = −0.27, power = 0.8, and a significance level of 0.05, we concluded 105 subjects would be sufficient for our analysis. Participants included 134 adults (95 females; M = 28.38 years, SD = 4.35). Participants had different seniority in university studies. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of neurologically based impairments, such as ADHD, or learning disabilities (e.g., dyslexia and dyscalculia). Prior to data collection, participants signed a consent form approved by the University of Haifa ethics committee (429/17, date of approval: 11 June 2019).

2.2. Measures

The questionnaires were translated by the author into Hebrew (forward translation) and from Hebrew back to English (back translation) to ensure the validity of the translations.

2.2.1. Trait Math Anxiety

We used the Hebrew-translated versions of the Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS) [98] to measure trait math anxiety. The AMAS is a nine-item self-report questionnaire found to be as effective as the longer Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale [98]. Each item consists of a statement describing an event; participants indicate how anxious it would make them on a five-point Likert scale from never to always. Scores on the AMAS range from 9 to 45, with a higher score indicating a higher level of math anxiety. Cronbach’s alpha for the AMAS in our sample was 0.92.

2.2.2. State Math Anxiety

State anxiety during a math test was assessed by the state-Mathematics Anxiety Questionnaire (state-MAQ) [97]. The questionnaire was developed from the State Anxiety Inventory [99]. Using a four-point Likert scale ranging from not at all to very much, participants indicate whether an emotional state applies to them immediately before a math test (pre-test; seven items) or has done so during a math test (post-test; seven items). The total score was obtained by summing each item rating. To control other influential factors, participants were instructed to fill out the questionnaire only in the light of the upcoming/completed math test and not consider other circumstances. The reliability was α = 0.92.

2.2.3. Subjective Valence Ratings of Math Information

Similar to previous research [100], we asked participants to rate how they felt on a visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from very bad to very good using the mouse immediately after a math test. While response bias is inevitable in scientific research, we used the reliable VAS [101] to capture small yet persistent effects in subjective emotional states [101,102,103,104].

2.2.4. State/Trait Anxiety

The Spielberger’s State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [99] was used to measure and differentiate between anxiety as a trait and a state. The STAI state scale consists of 20 statements asking people to describe how they feel at a particular moment in time (e.g., calm, tense) rated on a four-point intensity scale from not at all to very much. The STAI trait scale consists of 20 statements describing how people generally feel (e.g., confident), rated on a four-point frequency scale from almost never to almost always. The total score of each subscale was obtained by summing each item rating. Internal consistencies for the state scale scores ranged from 0.83 to 0.92 for male and female high-school and college students; for the trait-scale scores, coefficients of internal consistency ranged from 0.86 to 0.92 [99]. In our sample, the coefficient alpha for the state and the trait scales were 0.92 and 0.91, respectively.

2.2.5. Daily Use of Reappraisal and Suppression

The well-known Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) [105] was used to measure the frequency of the habitual use of reappraisal and suppression. The ERQ has acceptable validity and reliability [105]. It includes 10 items, six of which measure reappraisal frequency (e.g., “I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in”) and four expressive suppression frequency (e.g., “I control my emotions by not expressing them”). Items are rated on a seven-point Likert-type response scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Higher scores on an item indicate greater use of the corresponding strategy. We used the average score of the relevant subscale to create the total score of the frequency of the use of each strategy (appraisal, suppression). The coefficient alpha for the ERQ in our sample was 0.83.

2.2.6. Spontaneous Use of Reappraisal and Suppression

We administered a six-item scale based on the ERQ [105] to measure spontaneous use of reappraisal (three items) and suppression (three items) [106], with responses ranging from not at all to very much. Participants indicated which emotion-regulation behavior they used during a math task (post-test). The coefficient alpha for the scale in our sample was 0.70.

2.2.7. Difficulties in Emotion Regulation

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation scale (DERS) [107] is a 36-item scale developed to assess multiple facets of emotion regulation, on a five-point Likert scale ranging from almost never to almost always. Higher scores indicate greater difficulties in emotion regulation. The DERS has high internal consistency, good test–retest reliability, and adequate construct and predictive validity [107]. The coefficient alpha for the DERS in our sample was 0.92.

2.2.8. Math Performance

We used the Math Fluency and Calculation subtests of the Woodcock–Johnson III Tests of Achievement [108] to measure math performance. Math Fluency assesses automaticity with basic arithmetic facts. In this subtest, participants are required to quickly and accurately complete simple arithmetic problems within a three-minute time limit. Arithmetic computations include simple addition, subtraction, and multiplication operations. The Calculation subtest measures the ability to perform increasingly difficult math computations, from simple addition to advanced geometry and trigonometry, with no time limit. The number of correctly completed computations was totaled in each subtest.

2.3. Procedure

Due to COVID-19 restrictions in Israel, most participants (~70%) were recruited through iPanel (iPanel.co.il; iPanel, Bnei-Brak, Israel), an online Israeli pooling service, from June to September 2021, and a small proportion of participants (~30%) were recruited through invitations posted on various student internet groups and forums [109,110,111]. iPanel can deliver a representative sample of the adult Jewish population of Israel while adhering to the stringent standards of the European Society for Opinion and Marketing Research (ESOMAR). In addition, iPanel was evaluated by the Applied Statistical Laboratory of the Hebrew University of Jerusalem and found to be highly accurate [112]. Participants who registered in iPanel who met the inclusion criteria and participants recruited through the Internet received an invitation to participate and gave their consent. Participants who registered in iPanel received vouchers from the survey company in exchange for their participation. Participants recruited through the Internet received monetary compensation of a sum equivalent to USD 10.
The study was administered via E-Prime Go Software. First, the AMAS questionnaires were randomly presented, followed by the state-MAQ (pre-test) and the fluency and calculation subtests in random order. In the fluency and calculation subtests, arithmetic problems and math computations appeared one after the other when the participant pressed the space button to move on to the next one. A previous study found no significant differences between an online, remote administration procedure and traditional, in-person administration of the Woodcock–Johnson IV cognitive ability and academic achievement tests [112]. Then, the state-MAQ (post-test) and spontaneous use of reappraisal and suppression questionnaires were randomly administered. Next, the STAI, ERQ, and DERS questionnaires were presented in random order. The state-MAQ addressed an emotional state before (pre-test) or during (post-test) the math tests, whereas the STAI referred to general emotional states. Throughout the study, general questions (e.g., gender, age, time) were presented in order to ensure the validity of responses. Note that this was part of an ongoing larger study including additional cognitive tasks and emotional questionnaires.

2.4. Network Analysis

The entire network analysis procedure was based on Epskamp and Fried [113]. We used the R-package qgraph to estimate and visualize the network [114]. We estimated Gaussian graphical models (GGMs). In these models, each test is represented by a node in the network, and the edges represent partial correlation coefficients between the nodes using a color code (red for negative and blue for positive relations), with edge thickness representing the strength of a direct interaction between two nodes [115]. Based on Epskamp and Fried’s [113] guidelines, we computed the network using the least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO). LASSO is used to reduce false-positive edges and to deal with problems such as multicollinearity [116]. It identifies the edges that differ significantly from zero and most accurately reveal the underlying network (i.e., every edge that survives the GLASSO regression is significant). The tuning parameter (gamma) for the GLASSO estimation in our experiment was 0.5. A tuning parameter is chosen to minimize the extended Bayesian Information Criterion parameter and has been shown to accurately recover underlying network structures [117]. We determined node placement using the Fruchterman and Reingold’s [118] algorithm, which places connected nodes closer to each other and more connected nodes closer to the center.

3. Results

The final sample consisted of 117 adults (85 females; M = 28.38 years, SD = 4.35). A total of 7 males and 10 females were removed from analysis because their RTs or accuracy rates were outliers (below or above 1 SD) and/or due to missing values. Descriptive statistics of research variables are presented in Table 1. The correlation matrix is presented in Table 2. The results replicate previous findings by showing: (1) positive correlations between math and general anxiety [19,20]; (2) positive correlations between state- and trait-anxiety measures [42,43]; (3) negative correlations between math anxiety and math performance, e.g., [3,57]; (4) positive correlations between reappraisal and anxiety measures, e.g., [8,92]; (5) negative correlations between reappraisal and math performance, e.g., [8,10]. As the correlation matrix in Table 2 shows, anxiety and performance “in cluster” correlations were significant (i.e., within traditional latent constructs).
The GGM network is visualized in Figure 1. The network is composed of the following five theoretically assumed clusters: general anxiety (i.e., state and trait anxiety), state math anxiety and valence rating, math performance (i.e., math fluency and calculation), trait emotion regulation (i.e., daily and spontaneous use of and difficulties in emotion regulation; note that trait suppression was partial out of the network), and state emotion regulation. Of the possible 78 edges (i.e., links), only 21 were retained (for 95% CI around the edge, see Supplementary Figure S1). Predictability (i.e., variance of a node explained by its neighbors) ranged from 0% in the trait-suppression scale (i.e., daily use of suppression measure) to 74.3% in the state anxiety, and average predictability was 34%.
Four results are noteworthy. First, state math anxiety edges to performance partialed out the trait math-anxiety edges. Second, state math anxiety was positively linked to both state and trait anxiety. Third, spontaneous use of reappraisal during math tests was positively linked to subjective valence ratings of math information and calculations, whereas trait reappraisal had no direct links to state or trait math anxiety or math performance. Fourth, both state and trait math-anxiety questionnaires were linked to valence ratings of math information.
To sum up, our results clearly show that state math anxiety (i.e., a temporary and math-situation-related anxiety reaction) consists of predisposition to anxiety in general, negative subjective valence of math information, difficulties in emotion regulation, and is associated with low math performance.

4. Discussion

The primary purpose was to provide a comprehensive model of the nature of the links between math anxiety, emotion regulation, general anxiety, and math performance. Our powerful statistical approach enabled us to distinguish between the unique contribution of each observable variable [96] and visualize the dynamics among constituent elements in the network [18]. Here, we show that the emotional experience during math-related situations is tightly linked to the tendency toward anxiety in general, the degree of pleasant and unpleasant feelings triggered by a math task (i.e., subjective valence) [41], and difficulties in emotion regulation.
Importantly, the link between state math anxiety and math performance partialed out the link between trait math anxiety and performance. These results are consistent with previous findings of stable negative correlations between state (or state-like) math anxiety and math performance, e.g., [6,44,57,58,59]. They are also consistent with findings of a lack of correlations between fear of failure in math (i.e., trait math anxiety) and performance, e.g., [40,58,60,61,62,63]. However, math anxiety is commonly described as a trait [39]. Although related, state math anxiety, which refers to a temporary and math situation-related anxiety reaction, has been found to be less subject to bias [119] and more related to attitudes and motivation [58]. Note that in contrast to a small number of studies [64,65,66], we did not find differences in the math anxiety–performance link with various aspects of math (calculation and fluency). More research is needed to comprehensively test these relations [67].
Innovatively, we showed the spontaneous use of reappraisal in math-related situations, but not the tendency to use reappraisal in daily life, was positively linked to subjective valence ratings of math information and math performance. These findings are consistent with evidence of difficulties in emotion regulation in math anxiety [7,25,33,88,89,90] and explain the success of focused and brief reappraisal interventions in reducing math-anxiety reactions [8,91,92] and improving math performance [8,9,10,11,91,93]; however, see [12]. They are also consistent with the emotion-polyregulation phenomenon [95], according to which people implement various and multiple regulatory strategies in a given emotional episode [94].
Taken together, the study highlights for the first time the need to differentiate between general traits (i.e., general anxiety and trait math anxiety) and tendencies (i.e., daily use of emotion-regulation strategies), and the actual emotional experience (i.e., state math anxiety and subjective valence ratings) and emotion-regulation use in specific math contexts (i.e., spontaneous use of emotion-regulation strategies during a math test). From a practical perspective, the combined examination of general-anxiety predispositions, the subjective valence of math information, and difficulties in emotion regulation can help psychologists and educators optimally tailor an intervention plan to a specific case and maximize its outcomes. Importantly, the current study highlights the central role of emotion regulation in educational processes and suggests providing educators practical tools ways to promote the use of adaptive emotion-regulation strategy (i.e., reappraisal) in pedagogical programs. Finally, the study indicates the need to focus on promoting the use of reappraisal in specific math-related situations to reduce anxiety levels and improve achievement among people who experience a heightened negative emotional reaction in these contexts.

5. Limitations

The study makes an important first step towards an understanding of the interplay between math anxiety, emotion regulation, and math performance and highlights the need to distinguish between general traits and tendencies and the actual emotional experience in math-related situations. However, there are several limitations. First, recruiting participants via the Internet and social networks makes it difficult to further generalize the results and may threaten the findings’ reliability and validity, e.g., [120]. In many studies, however, internet-based data were shown to have high reliability, replicability, and theoretical consistency comparable to data gathered in a traditional lab setting, e.g., [121,122]. In this study, the coefficient alphas for the self-report questionnaires ranged from 0.70 in the spontaneous use of reappraisal and suppression questionnaire to 0.92 in the AMAS, state-MAQ, state-anxiety scale, and DERS.
Second, only adults with different seniority in university studies participated, with twice as many females as males. Future studies will have to test the model in different age groups, as well as in the general (non-student) population, with more participants and a balanced number of females and males. A recent meta-analysis [51] suggests the math anxiety–performance link is stronger among senior high-school students, followed by junior high-school, university, and elementary students. These differences might influence the edges between math anxiety, emotion regulation, and math performance.
Third, the network approach lacks directionality even though it expands our understanding of the origins of the links between latent variables. Future research should address causal inferences and consider assessing state math anxiety through implicit measures, such as Skin Conductance Response and Heart Rate Variability, e.g., [123], to assess inaccessible cognitive and emotional structures that are processed automatically [124].

6. Conclusions

The research visualizes the interplay between math anxiety, emotion regulation, and math performance. As we opted to use partial correlations between observable tests instead of latent models, our findings demonstrate, first, that the temporary and math-situation-related anxiety reaction consists of a predisposition to general anxiety, negative subjective valence of math information, and difficulties in emotion regulation. Second, the state math anxiety edge to math performance partialed out the edge between trait math anxiety and performance. Third, instead of focusing on general traits (i.e., general anxiety and trait math anxiety) and tendencies (i.e., daily use of emotion regulation), more emphasis should be put on the actual emotional experience (i.e., state math anxiety and subjective valence ratings) and emotion-regulation use in specific math contexts (e.g., spontaneous use of emotion regulation during a math test). Future research should carefully consider the relationships between directly measurable variables of math anxiety and emotion regulation that might be masked by traditional statistical approaches or interpreted in terms of unobserved latent variables.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/article/10.3390/brainsci11121609/s1, Figure S1: Bootstrapped confidence intervals of the edge weights in the network.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.D.C. and O.R.; methodology, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; software, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; validation, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; formal analysis, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; investigation, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; resources, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; data curation, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; writing—original draft preparation, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; writing—review and editing, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; visualization, L.D.C., N.K. and O.R.; supervision, O.R.; project administration, L.D.C.; funding acquisition, not relevant. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Haifa (429/17, date of approval: 11 June 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data and materials are available at https://osf.io/hj56d/ (accessed on 30 November 2021).

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the ISRAEL SCIENCE FOUNDATION (grant No 733/19).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Hart, S.A.; Ganley, C.M. The nature of math anxiety in adults: Prevalence and correlates. J. Numer. Cogn. 2019, 5, 122–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Dowker, A.; Bennett, K.; Smith, L. Attitudes to mathematics in primary school children. Child Dev. Res. 2012, 2012, 124939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kucian, K.; McCaskey, U.; Tuura, R.O.; von Aster, M. Neurostructural correlate of math anxiety in the brain of children. Transl. Psychiatry 2018, 8, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Beilock, S.L.; Maloney, E.A. Math anxiety a factor in math achievement not to be ignored. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2015, 2, 4–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ramirez, G.; Shaw, S.T.; Maloney, E.A. Math anxiety: Past research, promising interventions, and a new interpretation framework. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 53, 245–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Devine, A.; Hill, F.; Carey, E.; Szűcs, D. Cognitive and emotional math problems largely dissociate: Prevalence of developmental dyscalculia and mathematics anxiety. J. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 110, 431–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Pizzie, R.G.; Kraemer, D.J. Avoiding math on a rapid timescale: Emotional responsivity and anxious attention in math anxiety. Brain Cogn. 2017, 118, 100–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Pizzie, R.G.; Kraemer, D.J. The association between emotion regulation, physiological arousal, and performance in math anxiety. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Brooks, A.W. Get excited: Reappraising pre-performance anxiety as excitement. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014, 143, 1144–1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Pizzie, R.G.; McDermott, C.L.; Salem, T.G.; Kraemer, D.J. Neural evidence for cognitive reappraisal as a strategy to alleviate the effects of math anxiety. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2020, 15, 1271–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Sarkar, A.; Dowker, A.; Kadosh, R.C. Cognitive enhancement or cognitive cost: Trait-specific outcomes of brain stimulation in the case of mathematics anxiety. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 16605–16610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Passolunghi, M.C.; De Vita, C.; Pellizzoni, S. Math anxiety and math achievement: The effects of emotional and math strategy training. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Wang, Z.; Shakeshaft, N.; Schofield, K.; Malanchini, M. Anxiety is not enough to drive me away: A latent profile analysis on math anxiety and math motivation. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Orbach, L.; Fritz, A. A Latent Profile Analysis of Math Anxiety and Core Beliefs towards Mathematics among Children. PsyArXiv 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Fuchs, L.S.; Gilbert, J.K.; Powell, S.R.; Cirino, P.T.; Fuchs, D.; Hamlett, C.L.; Seethaler, P.M.; Tolar, T.D. The role of cognitive processes, foundational math skill, and calculation accuracy and fluency in word-problem solving versus prealgebraic knowledge. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 2085–2098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Beard, C.; Millner, A.J.; Forgeard, M.J.; Fried, E.I.; Hsu, K.J.; Treadway, M.T.; Leonard, C.V.; Kertz, S.J.; Björgvinsson, T. Network analysis of depression and anxiety symptom relationships in a psychiatric sample. Psychol. Med. 2016, 46, 3359–3369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Fried, E.I.; van Borkulo, C.D.; Cramer, A.O.; Boschloo, L.; Schoevers, R.A.; Borsboom, D. Mental disorders as networks of problems: A review of recent insights. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2017, 52, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Costantini, G.; Epskamp, S.; Borsboom, D.; Perugini, M.; Mõttus, R.; Waldorp, L.J.; Cramer, A.O. State of the aRt personality research: A tutorial on network analysis of personality data in R. J. Res. Personal. 2015, 54, 13–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ashcraft, M.H.; Moore, A.M. Mathematics anxiety and the affective drop in performance. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2009, 27, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Hembree, R. The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. J. Res. Math. Educ. 1990, 21, 33–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Cipora, K.; Szczygieł, M.; Willmes, K.; Nuerk, H.-C. Math anxiety assessment with the Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale: Applicability and usefulness: Insights from the Polish adaptation. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Devine, A.; Fawcett, K.; Szűcs, D.; Dowker, A. Gender differences in mathematics anxiety and the relation to mathematics performance while controlling for test anxiety. Behav. Brain Funct. 2012, 8, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Dowker, A.; Sarkar, A.; Looi, C.Y. Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years? Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Suárez-Pellicioni, M.; Núñez-Peña, M.I.; Colomé, À. Math anxiety: A review of its cognitive consequences, psychophysiological correlates, and brain bases. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 16, 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Pletzer, B.; Kronbichler, M.; Nuerk, H.C.; Kerschbaum, H.H. Mathematics anxiety reduces default mode network deactivation in response to numerical tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Ahmed, W. Developmental trajectories of math anxiety during adolescence: Associations with STEM career choice. J. Adolesc. 2018, 67, 158–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Eidlin Levy, H.; Fares, L.; Rubinsten, O. Math anxiety affects females’ vocational interests. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2021, 210, 105214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Maloney, E.A.; Beilock, S.L. Math anxiety: Who has it, why it develops, and how to guard against it. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 404–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Woloshin, S.; Schwartz, L.M.; Moncur, M.; Gabriel, S.; Tosteson, A.N. Assessing values for health: Numeracy matters. Med Decis. Mak. 2001, 21, 382–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Skagerlund, K.; Lind, T.; Strömbäck, C.; Tinghög, G.; Västfjäll, D. Financial literacy and the role of numeracy–How individuals’ attitude and affinity with numbers influence financial literacy. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2018, 74, 18–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ritchie, S.J.; Bates, T.C. Enduring links from childhood mathematics and reading achievement to adult socioeconomic status. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1301–1308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Daches Cohen, L.; Rubinsten, O. The complex pathways toward the development of math anxiety and links with achievements. In Heterogeneous Contributions to Numerical Cognition; Fias, W., Henik, A., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2021; pp. 311–326. [Google Scholar]
  33. Young, C.B.; Wu, S.S.; Menon, V. The neurodevelopmental basis of math anxiety. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 492–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Malanchini, M.; Rimfeld, K.; Shakeshaft, N.G.; Rodic, M.; Schofield, K.; Selzam, S.; Dale, P.S.; Petrill, S.A.; Kovas, Y. The genetic and environmental aetiology of spatial, mathematics and general anxiety. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 42218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. O’Leary, K.; Fitzpatrick, C.L.; Hallett, D. Math anxiety is related to some, but not all, experiences with math. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 2067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Novak, E.; Tassell, J.L. Studying preservice teacher math anxiety and mathematics performance in geometry, word, and non-word problem solving. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2017, 54, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Daches Cohen, L.; Rubinsten, O. Mothers, intrinsic math motivation, arithmetic skills, and math anxiety in elementary school. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Spielberger, C.D. Anxiety: Current Trends in Theory and Research: I; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Luttenberger, S.; Wimmer, S.; Paechter, M. Spotlight on math anxiety. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2018, 11, 311–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Orbach, L.; Herzog, M.; Fritz, A. Math Anxiety during the transition from primary to secondary school. In Inclusive Mathematics Education; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 419–447. [Google Scholar]
  41. Bliss-Moreau, E.; Williams, L.A.; Santistevan, A.C. The immutability of valence and arousal in the foundation of emotion. Emotion 2020, 20, 993–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Eysenck, H.J. Cicero and the state-trait theory of anxiety: Another case of delayed recognition. Am. Psychol. 1983, 38, 114–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Spielberger, C.D. Anxiety: Current Trends in Theory and Research; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  44. Cargnelutti, E.; Tomasetto, C.; Passolunghi, M.C. How is anxiety related to math performance in young students? A longitudinal study of Grade 2 to Grade 3 children. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 31, 755–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Rubinsten, O.; Eidlin, H.; Wohl, H.; Akibli, O. Attentional bias in math anxiety. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Suárez-Pellicioni, M.; Núñez-Peña, M.I.; Colomé, À. Attentional bias in high math-anxious individuals: Evidence from an emotional Stroop task. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Wang, Z.; Hart, S.A.; Kovas, Y.; Lukowski, S.; Soden, B.; Thompson, L.A.; Plomin, R.; McLoughlin, G.; Bartlett, C.W.; Lyons, I.M.; et al. Who is afraid of math? Two sources of genetic variance for mathematical anxiety. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 1056–1064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Malanchini, M.; Rimfeld, K.; Wang, Z.; Petrill, S.A.; Tucker-Drob, E.M.; Plomin, R.; Kovas, Y. Genetic factors underlie the association between anxiety, attitudes and performance in mathematics. Transl. Psychiatry 2020, 10, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Rubinsten, O.; Marciano, H.; Eidlin Levy, H.; Daches Cohen, L. A framework for studying the heterogeneity of risk factors in math anxiety. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Barroso, C.; Ganley, C.M.; McGraw, A.L.; Geer, E.A.; Hart, S.A.; Daucourt, M.C. A meta-analysis of the relation between math anxiety and math achievement. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 147, 134–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Zhang, J.; Zhao, N.; Kong, Q.P. The relationship between math anxiety and math performance: A meta-analytic investigation. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Lee, J. Universals and specifics of math self-concept, math self-efficacy, and math anxiety across 41 PISA 2003 participating countries. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2009, 19, 355–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development (OECD). PISA 2012 Results: Ready to Learn: Students’ Engagement, Drive and Self-Beliefs (Volume III): Preliminary Version; OECD: Paris, France, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  54. Skagerlund, K.; Östergren, R.; Västfjäll, D.; Träff, U. How does mathematics anxiety impair mathematical abilities? Investigating the link between math anxiety, working memory, and number processing. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0211283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Passolunghi, M.C.; Cargnelutti, E.; Pellizzoni, S. The relation between cognitive and emotional factors and arithmetic problem-solving. Educ. Stud. Math. 2019, 100, 271–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Wu, S.S.; Willcutt, E.G.; Escovar, E.; Menon, V. Mathematics achievement and anxiety and their relation to internalizing and externalizing behaviors. J. Learn. Disabil. 2014, 47, 503–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Gunderson, E.A.; Park, D.; Maloney, E.A.; Beilock, S.L.; Levine, S.C. Reciprocal relations among motivational frameworks, math anxiety, and math achievement in early elementary school. J. Cogn. Dev. 2018, 19, 21–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Orbach, L.; Herzog, M.; Fritz, A. Relation of state-and trait-math anxiety to intelligence, math achievement and learning motivation. J. Numer. Cogn. 2019, 5, 371–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Sorvo, R.; Koponen, T.; Viholainen, H.; Aro, T.; Räikkönen, E.; Peura, P.; Dowker, A.; Aro, M. Math anxiety and its relationship with basic arithmetic skills among primary school children. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 87, 309–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Haase, V.G.; Julio-Costa, A.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Oliveira, L.D.F.S.; Micheli, L.R.; Wood, G. Math self-assessment, but not negative feelings, predicts mathematics performance of elementary school children. Child Dev. Res. 2012, 2012, 982672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Krinzinger, H.; Kaufmann, L.; Willmes, K. Math anxiety and math ability in early primary school years. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2009, 27, 206–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Thomas, G.; Dowker, A. Mathematics anxiety and related factors in young children. In Proceedings of the British Psychological Society Developmental Section Conference, Bristol, UK, 14–17 September 2000. [Google Scholar]
  63. Wood, G.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Julio-Costa, A.; Micheli, L.R.; Krinzinger, H.; Kaufmann, L.; Willmes, K.; Haase, V.G. Math anxiety questionnaire: Similar latent structure in Brazilian and German school children. Child Dev. Res. 2012, 2012, 610192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Ching, B.H.H. Mathematics anxiety and working memory: Longitudinal associations with mathematical performance in Chinese children. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 51, 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Vukovic, R.K.; Kieffer, M.J.; Bailey, S.P.; Harari, R.R. Mathematics anxiety in young children: Concurrent and longitudinal associations with mathematical performance. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 38, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Vukovic, R.K.; Roberts, S.O.; Green Wright, L. From parental involvement to children’s mathematical performance: The role of mathematics anxiety. Early Educ. Dev. 2013, 24, 446–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Caviola, S.; Toffalini, E.; Giofrè, D.; Ruiz, J.M.; Szűcs, D.; Mammarella, I.C. Math Performance and Academic Anxiety Forms, from Sociodemographic to Cognitive Aspects: A Meta-analysis on 906,311 Participants. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Pekrun, R.; Lichtenfeld, S.; Marsh, H.W.; Murayama, K.; Goetz, T. Achievement emotions and academic performance: Longitudinal models of reciprocal effects. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 1653–1670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Ma, X.; Xu, J. The causal ordering of mathematics anxiety and mathematics achievement: A longitudinal panel analysis. J. Adolesc. 2004, 27, 165–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Wang, Z.; Rimfeld, K.; Shakeshaft, N.; Schofield, K.; Malanchini, M. The longitudinal role of mathematics anxiety in mathematics development: Issues of gender differences and domain-specificity. J. Adolesc. 2020, 80, 220–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Blalock, J.A.; Joiner, T.E. Interaction of cognitive avoidance coping and stress in predicting depression/anxiety. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2000, 24, 47–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Spinhoven, P.; Drost, J.; de Rooij, M.; van Hemert, A.M.; Penninx, B.W. A longitudinal study of experiential avoidance in emotional disorders. Behav. Ther. 2014, 45, 840–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Ashcraft, M.H. Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 11, 181–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Hirvonen, R.; Tolvanen, A.; Aunola, K.; Nurmi, J.E. The developmental dynamics of task-avoidant behavior and math performance in kindergarten and elementary school. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2012, 22, 715–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Lyons, I.M.; Beilock, S.L. Mathematics anxiety: Separating the math from the anxiety. Cereb. Cortex 2012, 22, 2102–2110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Aldao, A.; Nolen-Hoeksema, S.; Schweizer, S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 30, 217–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Rottenberg, J.; Gross, J.J. When emotion goes wrong: Realizing the promise of affective science. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 2003, 10, 227–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Gross, J.J. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1998, 2, 271–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inq. 2015, 26, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Bigman, Y.E.; Sheppes, G.; Tamir, M. When less is more: Effects of the availability of strategic options on regulating negative emotions. Emotion 2017, 17, 993–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Mauss, I.B.; Cook, C.L.; Cheng, J.Y.; Gross, J.J. Individual differences in cognitive reappraisal: Experiential and physiological responses to an anger provocation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2007, 66, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Dryman, M.T.; Heimberg, R.G. Emotion regulation in social anxiety and depression: A systematic review of expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 65, 17–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Kivity, Y.; Huppert, J.D. Are individuals diagnosed with social anxiety disorder successful in regulating their emotions? A mixed-method investigation using self-report, subjective, and event-related potentials measures. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 236, 298–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Schäfer, J.Ö.; Naumann, E.; Holmes, E.A.; Tuschen-Caffier, B.; Samson, A.C. Emotion regulation strategies in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: A meta-analytic review. J. Youth Adolesc. 2017, 46, 261–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Zilverstand, A.; Parvaz, M.A.; Goldstein, R.Z. Neuroimaging cognitive reappraisal in clinical populations to define neural targets for enhancing emotion regulation. A systematic review. Neuroimage 2017, 151, 105–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Wirtz, C.M.; Hofmann, S.G.; Riper, H.; Berking, M. Emotion regulation predicts anxiety over a five-year interval: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Depress. Anxiety 2014, 31, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Howse, R.B.; Calkins, S.D.; Anastopoulos, A.D.; Keane, S.P.; Shelton, T.L. Regulatory contributors to children’s kindergarten achievement. Early Educ. Dev. 2003, 14, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Artemenko, C.; Daroczy, G.; Nuerk, H.C. Neural correlates of math anxiety–an overview and implications. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Lyons, I.M.; Beilock, S.L. When math hurts: Math anxiety predicts pain network activation in anticipation of doing math. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e48076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Suárez-Pellicioni, M.; Núñez-Peña, M.I.; Colomé, A. Mathematical anxiety effects on simple arithmetic processing efficiency: An event-related potential study. Biol. Psychol. 2013, 94, 517–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Jamieson, J.P.; Mendes, W.B.; Blackstock, E.; Schmader, T. Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 46, 208–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  92. Jamieson, J.P.; Peters, B.J.; Greenwood, E.J.; Altose, A.J. Reappraising stress arousal improves performance and reduces evaluation anxiety in classroom exam situations. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2016, 7, 579–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Ramirez, G.; Beilock, S.L. Writing about testing worries boosts exam performance in the classroom. Science 2011, 331, 211–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Aldao, A.; Nolen-Hoeksema, S. One versus many: Capturing the use of multiple emotion regulation strategies in response to an emotion-eliciting stimulus. Cogn. Emot. 2013, 27, 753–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Ford, B.Q.; Gross, J.J.; Gruber, J. Broadening our field of view: The role of emotion polyregulation. Emot. Rev. 2019, 11, 197–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Schmittmann, V.D.; Cramer, A.O.; Waldorp, L.J.; Epskamp, S.; Kievit, R.A.; Borsboom, D. Deconstructing the construct: A network perspective on psychological phenomena. New Ideas Psychol. 2013, 31, 43–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Orbach, L.; Herzog, M.; Fritz, A. State-and trait-math anxiety and their relation to math performance in children: The role of core executive functions. Cognition 2020, 200, 104271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Hopko, D.R.; Mahadevan, R.; Bare, R.L.; Hunt, M.K. The abbreviated math anxiety scale (AMAS) construction, validity, and reliability. Assessment 2003, 10, 178–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Spielberger, C.D. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
  100. Daniela, T.; Alessandro, C.; Giuseppe, C.; Fabio, M.; Cristina, M.; Michele, F. Lack of sleep affects the evaluation of emotional stimuli. Brain Res. Bull. 2010, 82, 104–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Wewers, M.E.; Lowe, N.K. A critical review of visual analogue scales in the measurement of clinical phenomena. Res. Nurs. Health 1990, 13, 227–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Bond, A.; Lader, M. The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br. J. Med Psychol. 1974, 47, 211–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Hayes, M.H.; Patterson, D. Experimental development of the graphics rating method. Physiol. Bull. 1921, 18, 98–99. [Google Scholar]
  104. Monk, T.H. A visual analogue scale technique to measure global vigor and affect. Psychiatry Res. 1989, 27, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Gross, J.J.; John, O.P. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 348–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Egloff, B.; Schmukle, S.C.; Burns, L.R.; Schwerdtfeger, A. Spontaneous emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: Associations with negative affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion 2006, 6, 356–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Gratz, K.L.; Roemer, L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2004, 26, 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Woodcock, R.W.; McGrew, K.S.; Mather, N. Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement; Riverside: Itasca, IL, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  109. Roy, D.; Tripathy, S.; Kar, S.K.; Sharma, N.; Verma, S.K.; Kaushal, V. Study of knowledge, attitude, anxiety perceived mental healthcare need in Indian population during COVID-19 pandemic. Asian J. Psychiatry 2020, 51, 102083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Wang, A.H.; Lee, C.T.; Espin, S. Undergraduate nursing students’ experiences of anxiety-producing situations in clinical practicums: A descriptive survey study. Nurse Educ. Today 2019, 76, 103–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Zsido, A.N.; Arato, N.; Inhof, O.; Janszky, J.; Darnai, G. Short versions of two specific phobia measures: The snake and the spider questionnaires. J. Anxiety Disord. 2018, 54, 11–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Wright, A.J. Equivalence of Remote, Online Administration and Traditional, Face-to-Face Administration of Woodcock-Johnson IV Cognitive and Achievement Tests; Letter from the applied statistic laboratory; Department of Statistics, Hebrew University (Jerusalem) to the iPanel Company; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  113. Epskamp, S.; Fried, E.I. A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol. Methods 2018, 23, 617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Epskamp, S.; Cramer, A.O.; Waldorp, L.J.; Schmittmann, V.D.; Borsboom, D. qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  115. De Nooy, W.; Mrvar, A.; Batagelj, V. Exploratory Social Network Analysis with Pajek: Revised and Expanded Edition for Updated Software; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2018; Volume 46. [Google Scholar]
  116. Tibshirani, R. Regression Shrinkage and Selection Via the Lasso. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodol.) 1996, 58, 267–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Foygel, R.; Drton, M. Bayesian model choice and information criteria in sparse generalized linear models. arXiv 2011, arXiv:1112.5635, Preprint. [Google Scholar]
  118. Fruchterman, T.M.; Reingold, E.M. Graph drawing by force-directed placement. Softw. Pract. Exp. 1991, 21, 1129–1164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Bieg, M. Trait and State Academic Emotions: Two Sides of the Same Coin? Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  120. Gosling, S.D.; Vazire, S.; Srivastava, S.; John, O.P. Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet questionnaires. Am. Psychol. 2004, 59, 93–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  121. Germine, L.; Nakayama, K.; Duchaine, B.C.; Chabris, C.F.; Chatterjee, G.; Wilmer, J.B. Is the Web as good as the lab? Comparable performance from Web and lab in cognitive/perceptual experiments. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 19, 847–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  122. Nosek, B.A.; Banaji, M.R.; Greenwald, A.G. E-research: Ethics, security, design, and control in psychological research on the Internet. J. Soc. Issues 2002, 58, 161–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Qu, Z.; Chen, J.; Li, B.; Tan, J.; Zhang, D.; Zhang, Y. Measurement of high-school students’ trait math anxiety using neurophysiological recordings during math exam. IEEE Access 2020, 8, 57460–57471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Rubinsten, O. Link between cognitive neuroscience and education: The case of clinical assessment of developmental dyscalculia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Regularized partial correlations network. Edge thickness represents the strength of a direct interaction between two nodes. Blue and red edges indicate positive and negative relations, respectively. The black square represents the focus of the study. The orange nodes represent the general-anxiety subscales (the STAI subscales). The green node represents the trait math-anxiety questionnaire (AMAS questionnaire). The magenta nodes represent state math-anxiety measures (State MA = state-MAQ questionnaire, Valence = valence ratings of math information). Cyan nodes represent math tests. The yellow nodes represent daily use of emotion-regulation strategies (the ERQ subscales) and difficulties in emotion regulation (DERS questionnaire). White nodes represent spontaneous use of reappraisal and suppression strategies during math tests. The painted area in the rings around the nodes depicts predictability.
Figure 1. Regularized partial correlations network. Edge thickness represents the strength of a direct interaction between two nodes. Blue and red edges indicate positive and negative relations, respectively. The black square represents the focus of the study. The orange nodes represent the general-anxiety subscales (the STAI subscales). The green node represents the trait math-anxiety questionnaire (AMAS questionnaire). The magenta nodes represent state math-anxiety measures (State MA = state-MAQ questionnaire, Valence = valence ratings of math information). Cyan nodes represent math tests. The yellow nodes represent daily use of emotion-regulation strategies (the ERQ subscales) and difficulties in emotion regulation (DERS questionnaire). White nodes represent spontaneous use of reappraisal and suppression strategies during math tests. The painted area in the rings around the nodes depicts predictability.
Brainsci 11 01609 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of research variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of research variables.
Latent VariableTestMean (SD)Min–Max
Emotion regulationDaily use of emotion regulationERQ reappraisal scale4.62 (1.27)1–7
ERQ suppression scale3.41 (1.50)1–7
Difficulties in emotion regulationDERS86.37 (21.28)40–149
Spontaneous use of emotion regulation during math testsSpontaneous reappraisal3.26 (0.97)0.33–5
Spontaneous suppression2.99 (1.02)0.33–5
AnxietyGeneral anxietyState anxiety41.73 (11.91)20–73
Trait anxiety40.65 (10.93)20–76
Trait math anxietyAMAS24.22 (8.49)9–44
State math anxietyState-MAQ2.31 (1.25)0–5.71
Valence ratings13.27 (3.70)2–18
Math performance Fluency100.30 (22.02)54–159
Calculation20.39 (6.23)8–40
Table 2. Correlation matrix of research variables.
Table 2. Correlation matrix of research variables.
Variable1234567891011
1. ERQ reappraisal
2. ERQ suppression0.13
3. DERS−0.43 ***0.09
4. Spontaneous reappraisal 0.15−0.02−0.06
5. Spontaneous suppression0.14−0.10−0.060.37 ***
6. State anxiety−0.41 ***−0.050.55 ***−0.23 *−0.29 **
7. Trait anxiety−0.39 ***−0.010.63 ***−0.19 *−0.180.83 ***
8. AMAS−0.08−0.100.14−0.15−0.160.42 ***0.36 ***
9. State-MAQ−0.34 ***−0.000.46 ***−0.14−0.010.56 ***0.55 ***0.36 ***
10. Valence0.37 ***16−0.26 **0.24 **0.08−0.46 ***−0.42 ***−0.44 ***−0.46 ***
11. Fluency0.02−0.01−0.110.130.41−0.19 *−0.17−0.25 **−0.38 ***0.29 **
12. Calculation0.030.11−0.060.19 *−0.12−0.09−0.06−0.22 *−0.32 ***0.26 **0.34 ***
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Daches Cohen, L.; Korem, N.; Rubinsten, O. Math Anxiety Is Related to Math Difficulties and Composed of Emotion Regulation and Anxiety Predisposition: A Network Analysis Study. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1609. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci11121609

AMA Style

Daches Cohen L, Korem N, Rubinsten O. Math Anxiety Is Related to Math Difficulties and Composed of Emotion Regulation and Anxiety Predisposition: A Network Analysis Study. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(12):1609. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci11121609

Chicago/Turabian Style

Daches Cohen, Lital, Nachshon Korem, and Orly Rubinsten. 2021. "Math Anxiety Is Related to Math Difficulties and Composed of Emotion Regulation and Anxiety Predisposition: A Network Analysis Study" Brain Sciences 11, no. 12: 1609. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci11121609

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop