Next Article in Journal
Electroacupuncture at Fengchi(GB20) and Yanglingquan(GB34) Ameliorates Paralgesia through Microglia-Mediated Neuroinflammation in a Rat Model of Migraine
Previous Article in Journal
Collateral Circulation and BNP in Predicting Outcome of Acute Ischemic Stroke Patients with Atherosclerotic versus Cardioembolic Cerebral Large-Vessel Occlusion Who Underwent Endovascular Treatment
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Positive Touch Deprivation during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Effects on Anxiety, Stress, and Depression among Italian General Population

by
Francesco Bruno
1,2,3,*,
Carlotta Tagliaferro
4,
Sonia Canterini
5,
Valentina Laganà
2,
Marianna Contrada
6,
Chiara Fioravanti
7,
Natalia Altomari
8,
Rebecca Pistininzi
9,
Francesca Tarantino
9,
Alessia Placanica
9,
Ersilia Martina Greco
9,
Francesca Capicotto
9,
Saverio Spadea
9,
Anna Maria Coscarella
9,
Martina Bonanno
9,
Federica Scarfone
9,
Sara Luchetta-Mattace
9,
Alessia Filice
9,
Annamaria Pettinato
9,
Ana Avramovic
10,
Chloe Lau
11,
Georgia Marunic
12 and
Francesca Chiesi
12
add Show full author list remove Hide full author list
1
Department of Primary Care, Regional Neurogenetic Centre (CRN), ASP CZ, 88046 Lamezia Terme, Italy
2
Association for Neurogenetic Research (ARN), 88046 Lamezia Terme, Italy
3
Academy of Cognitive Behavioral Sciences of Calabria (ASCoC), 88046 Lamezia Terme, Italy
4
School of Psychology, University of Florence, 50135 Florence, Italy
5
Division of Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University La Sapienza, 00158 Rome, Italy
6
S. Anna Institute and Research in Advanced Neurorehabilitation (RAN), 88900 Crotone, Italy
7
Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie -Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie, Zürcherstrasse, 8852 Altendorf, Switzerland
8
Department of Mathematics and Computer Science, University of Calabria, 87036 Rende, Italy
9
Scholar at Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, Degree Course in Science and Techniques of Cognitive Psychology, Magna Graecia University of Catanzaro, 88100 Catanzaro, Italy
10
Faculty of Philosophy, Laboratory for Experimental Psychology, University of Belgrade, 11102 Belgrade, Serbia
11
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London ON N6A 3K7, Canada
12
Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug, and Child’s Health (NEUROFARBA), Section of Psychology, University of Florence, 50135 Florence, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 17 February 2023 / Revised: 11 March 2023 / Accepted: 21 March 2023 / Published: 24 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Psychiatric Diseases)

Abstract

:
Physical distancing due to the COVID-19 Pandemic has limited the opportunities for family members, friends, and significant others to show physical affection (i.e., hugs, kisses, caresses, holding hands) during social interactions. The present study investigated the effects of positive touch and psychological distress in 991 Italian participants (Mage = 34.43, SD = 14.27). Results showed the frequency of hugs with the cohabiting partner significantly decreased the symptoms of depression (β = −1.187, p = 0.018, eβ = 0.30, 95% CI = 0.11–0.82), whereas the frequency of caresses with cohabiting relatives predicted the symptoms of anxiety (β = 0.575, p = 0.034, eβ = 1.78, 95% CI = 1.04–3.03). The frequency of hugs (β = −0.609, p = 0.049, eβ = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.30–1.00), and kisses (β = 0.663, p = 0.045, eβ = 1.94, 95% CI = 1.01–3.71) with non-cohabiting relatives predicted the symptoms of anxiety (χ2 = 1.35, df = 5, p = 0.93). These results suggest the importance of positive touch on psychological well-being in the social context.

1. Introduction

The Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) Pandemic, caused by the SARS-CoV-2 virus was first reported in December 2019 in Wuhan China and subsequently documented in more than 210 countries around the world, represented an emergency for international public health [1] of both the general population [2,3,4] and vulnerable groups [5,6,7].
In many countries, various measures have been adopted in attempt to contain the spread of this infection, including social or physical distancing. The goals of physical distancing were to decrease the contacts of infected people with uninfected, minimize the virus’s transmission, and decrease associated morbidity and mortality [8].
Physical distancing has limited the opportunities for family members, friends, and colleagues to show physical affection during social interactions. Positive touch (i.e., hugs, kisses, caresses, holding hands) represents a fundamental element of the human experience, as it constitutes a primary component of socio-emotional, cognitive, physical, and neurological development [9,10]. Several studies indicate that this important form of non-verbal communication allows humans to express and transmit social support and a wide range of primary (i.e., happiness and sadness) and pro-social (i.e., love, gratitude, sympathy) emotions [10]. Moreover, positive touch is associated with the activation of the neuroendocrine system with the consequent release of oxytocin [11,12]. In turn, oxytocin may influence the levels of expression of dopamine, serotonin, and endorphins, all of which are associated with positive emotions [10]. On a psychological level, these effects result in the reduction of interpersonal conflicts, increased attachment and bonding between people, improved recognition of emotions and social memory, and greater levels of empathy, sincerity, and fidelity [10,11,12]. Furthermore, several studies have suggested that the social support and emotions transmitted through positive touch are able to mitigate the stress response by reducing cortisol levels. In turn, lowered levels of perceived stress have a protective effect on both cardiovascular and immune systems [10,11,12,13].
Although social distancing represents an indispensable measure to contain the spread of the COVID-19 Pandemic, it limits the individual’s ability to maintain emotional and social connections and thus could have dramatic consequences on mental health. Public health regulations of the social distancing due to the COVID-19 Pandemic was associated with a higher desire for touch, which in turn was related to an increased perceived pleasantness of observing touch [14]. Moreover, the deprivation of intimate touch (e.g., kiss, hugs, caress from partner or close family) due to the COVID-19-related restrictions was associated with anxiety and loneliness [15]. Its associations between psychological distress and both personal distancing behavior [16] and touch deprivation [17] were observed during the U.S. COVID-19 lockdown. Conversely, the frequency of both in-person social and sexual connections were generally associated with lower levels of depressive symptoms [18].
To our knowledge, no study has separately examined the frequency reduction of each type of positive touch (i.e., kisses, hugs, caresses, holding hands) depending on the type of relationship with cohabitants and non-cohabiting people as well as the weight that this could have on psychological distress during the COVID-19 Pandemic.
Thus, the aims of this study were to: (i) estimate the frequency of the reduction of positive touch (i.e., hugs, kisses, caresses, holding hands) both with cohabiting (i.e., partners, children, relatives or friends/roommates) and non-cohabiting (i.e., partners, children, relatives and friends) people due to the COVID-19 Pandemic; (ii) understand the impact of the frequency reduction of positive touch both with cohabiting and non-cohabiting people on psychological distress (i.e., symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

The questionnaire, in electronic format, was proposed to a convenience sample and distributed widely over a period of COVID-19 restrictions between May and June 2021. Sampling was based on the ‘snowball’ method [19], in which non-graduate students of a psychology course were invited to participate in a study with an online questionnaire and were encouraged to recruit their acquaintances and relatives as well; in addition, the questionnaire was also shared on social network groups to increase sample diversity. In all, 3003 questionnaires were collected from the general Italian population. From these, approximately one third were randomly drawn to constitute the sample for this study, more precisely, 991 Italian participants (57% female, age range from 18 to 85 years, Mage = 34.43, SD = 14.27).
Most participants have completed high school (52.7%), lived with their family members (41.1%), and were engaged to a romantic partner (37.6%). All participants were aware that participation in the study was voluntary, and data were collected anonymously for research purposes. All participants consented to participate under these terms and debriefed upon completion of the study. No compensation or incentives were provided. Approval for this study was obtained from the Ethical Committee of Calabria Region (Catanzaro, Italy; Prot. n. 52098, 16 April 2021).

2.2. Measures

The frequency of four physical positive touch exposures (i.e., hugging, kissing, caressing, holding hands) with reference to the period before the COVID-19 Pandemic and during the last month was assessed. Each response category had four options: not at all, once or a few times, 1–3 times a week, and almost every day. Social connections were measured by asking participants to refer both to touch with cohabiting persons (partners/children/relatives/friends) and to positive touch with non-cohabiting persons (children/relatives/friends). Each type of physical positive touch was correlated with measures of symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress by administering the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21) [20], Italian version [21]. This scale is a self-report questionnaire with 21-items measuring symptoms of depression, stress, and anxiety (seven items for each subscale) based on a four-point rating scale (with anchors labeled 0 = did not apply to me at all and 3 = applied to me much, or most of the time). A high score on each subscale indicates elevated symptoms of depression, anxiety, or stress. In the current sample, Cronbach’s Alpha for Stress and Depression subscales were excellent (α = 0.93 and α = 0.92, respectively) and good for the Anxiety subscale (α = 0.88).

2.3. Statistical Analysis

To account for any differential nonresponse, low amounts of missing data (<5% for all variables) were considered a complete case. We calculated descriptive estimates of the prevalence of the four physical touch before the pandemic and during the last month for each condition of cohabitation or non-cohabitation. To compare the exposures during the two timeframes, the Wilcoxon test for correlated samples was used. Log-binomial regression with adjusted odds ratios and 95% CIs were computed to estimate the associations between each of the four exposures and the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress). To calculate the regression outcomes, depression, anxiety, and stress were transformed into binary variables. The scale cutoff of greater than or equal to 7 was used to identify those likely experiencing significant depressive symptoms. A scale cutoff of 6 and 10 was used to identify significant anxiety and stress symptoms, respectively. In the binomial logistic regression, the Hosmer and Lemeshow tests were used to evaluate the goodness of fit of the models (p > 0.05) [22]. All analyses were conducted using SPSS 24.0 statistical software.

3. Results

The valid percentage of moderate/extremely severe depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms reported in the past seven days were 37.4%, 24.9%, and 33.3%, respectively. In Table 1, the frequencies of physical positive touch with cohabiting people are reported. Persons living with partners (N = 561), children (N = 248), relatives (N = 453), and friends (N = 61) seem to report a statistically significant reduction in physical touch between before the pandemic and during the last month according to the Wilcoxon test (p < 0.001). The frequencies of physical positive touch with non-cohabiting people are presented in Table 2. Similarly, people not living with their children (N = 87) and relatives and friends (N = 991) seem to report a statistically significant reduction in physical touch between before the pandemic and in the last month according to the Wilcoxon test (p < 0.001). From these findings, the adequacy of the models was checked by using as predictors the types of positive physical contact and as outcomes the levels of self-assessed depressive symptoms for both cohabiting (i.e., partners, children, relatives, or friends/roommates) and non-cohabiting (i.e., children, relatives, and friends) people. Different models were evaluated and all were found to be non-significant, allowing the data within them to be interpreted. However, only certain types of positive physical contact were found to predict significant levels of self-assessed depressive, anxiety, and stress symptoms during the pandemic. All types of physical positive touch were not significant (p > 0.05) predictors of self-assessed anxiety, stress, and depression symptoms for cohabiting and non-cohabiting children and for cohabiting and non-cohabiting friends (full data are available in the Supplementary Material). In the current study, the model that associates the frequency of hugs with the partner and the decreased levels of depression (β = −1.187, p = 0.018, eβ = 0.30, 95% CI = 0.11–0.82) showed the Hosmer and Lemeshow test (χ2 = 3.13, df = 4, p = 0.54) was not significant. This indicated good fit with a significance level greater than 0.05, and the estimated variance explained by Nagelkerke R2 of 0.05. Similarly, the frequency of caresses with cohabiting relatives predicted the levels of anxiety (β = 0.575, p = 0.034, eβ = 1.78, 95% CI = 1.04–3.03) and the Hosmer–Lemeshow test result confirmed that the model had a good fit (χ2 = 1.67, df = 6, p = 0.95), with a Nagelkerke R2 of 0.02. The frequency of hugs (β= −0.609, p = 0.049, eβ = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.30–1.00) and kisses (β= 0.663, p = 0.045, eβ = 1.94, 95% CI= 1.01–3.71) with non-cohabiting relatives predicted anxiety level (χ2 = 1.35, df = 5, p = 0.93), with a Nagelkerke R2 value of 0.01 (Table 3).

4. Discussion

The general aim of this study was to estimate the frequency of the reduction of positive touch (i.e., hugs, kisses, caresses, holding hands) both with cohabiting (i.e., partners, children, relatives, or friends/roommates) and non-cohabiting (i.e., children, relatives, and friends) people due to the COVID-19 Pandemic in the Italian general population. In addition, we were interested in the impact of the frequency reduction of positive touch both with cohabiting and non-cohabiting people on psychological distress (i.e., symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress).
As expected, compared to before the pandemic, we found a significant and general reduction in the frequency of positive touch (hugs, kisses, caresses, holding hands) both with cohabiting (i.e., partners, children, relatives, or friends/roommates) and non-cohabiting (i.e., partners, children, relatives, and friends) people. These results are consistent with the social distancing measures provided by the Italian Ministry of Health, such as maintaining a distance of at least 1 m from other people and wearing the ffp2 mask in open and closed places to limit the spread of COVID-19 [23]. In line with our findings, other studies reported a general reduction of positive touch (i.e., hugs, kisses, and caresses) from partners (von Mohr et al., 2021; Field et al., 2020), children (Field et al., 2020), close family members (von Mohr et al., 2021), friends, and work colleagues or carers [15] during the first wave of the COVID-19 Pandemic and due to COVID-19-related restrictions.
Moreover, we reported that the frequency of hugs with the cohabiting partner significantly decreased the symptoms of depression. This finding is in line with the results of Field et al. (2020) who reported that the frequency of touch with partners was related to lower levels of depressive symptoms during the U.S. COVID-19 lockdown. This effect is likely attributable to the well-documented increase in oxytocin levels that occurs when more hugs are received from the partner [10,11]. In addition, another line of research suggests that high and low levels of oxytocin are associated with high and low levels of depressive symptoms, respectively (for a review see: McQuaid et al., 2014). On the other hand, we found that the frequency of caresses with cohabiting relatives and the frequency of hugs and kisses with non-cohabiting relatives predicted symptoms of anxiety (Figure 1).
These effects are likely due to the fact that, compared to the partner, there is less trust and knowledge of the social networks frequented and of the physical contacts exchanged by co-habitants and non-cohabitant relatives. As a consequence, having positive physical contacts with them could increase the fear of being infected with SARS-CoV-2, given that both kisses and caresses are means of contracting it. However, other studies are needed to better investigate these aspects.
There are considerable limitations to this research that can be helpful for future studies. First, it is important to recognize that sampling used is not as effective as true random sampling; nonetheless, it allowed us to overcome specific disadvantages connected with true random sampling, such as being overly expensive and time-consuming. Second, self-reported measures were administered to assess the dimensions of this study. Future research should take into consideration different methods (e.g., clinician-ratings, peer-ratings) to reduce the influence of self-report bias. Finally, an individual’s retrospective report of physical touch prior to the COVID-19 Pandemic is dependent on their memory. Future studies should employ diary studies to address this limitation.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, in this study, we reported a general reduction in the frequency of positive touch (i.e., hugs, kisses, caresses, holding hands) between before the COVID-19 Pandemic and in the last month, both with cohabiting and non-cohabiting people, in line with the social distancing measures adopted by the Italian government. Interestingly, the frequency of hugs with the cohabiting partner significantly decreased the symptoms of depression, whereas the frequency of caresses with cohabiting relatives and the frequency of hugs and kisses with non-cohabiting relatives predicted anxiety levels. This may be due to social networks and frequency of physical contacts exchanged by the relative are less known than those of the cohabiting partner. Further studies are needed to better investigate these aspects.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/article/10.3390/brainsci13040540/s1, Table S1. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch with cohabiting partner and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress). Table S2. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch with cohabiting children and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress). Table S3. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch with cohabiting relatives and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress). Table S4. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch with cohabiting friends and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress). Table S5. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch with not-cohabiting children and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress). Table S6. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch with not-cohabiting relatives and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress). Table S7. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch with not-cohabiting friends and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, F.B. and F.C. (Francesca Chiesi); methodology, F.B. and F.C. (Francesca Chiesi); Data collection, F.B., S.C., V.L., M.C., C.F., N.A., R.P., F.T., A.P. (Alessia Placanica), E.M.G., F.C. (Francesca Capicotto), S.S., A.M.C., M.B., F.S., S.L.-M., A.F., A.P. (Annamaria Pettinato) and A.A.; data curation, F.B., C.T., C.L. and G.M.; Statistical analysis, C.T., G.M., F.C., F.B. and S.C.; writing—original draft preparation, F.B., C.T., C.L., G.M. and S.C.; writing—review and editing, F.C. (Francesca Chiesi). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee Ethical Committee of Calabria Region (Catanzaro, Italy) (Prot. n. 52098, 16 April 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants for their cooperation and Desiree Franconieri, Arianna De Vito, Francesco Costantino, Emanuela Pagliaro, Maria Pia Rizzo and Rosazzurra Minervini for support in data collection, and Taylor Swindall for English-language editing.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Wang, C.; Pan, R.; Wan, X.; Tan, Y.; Xu, L.; Ho, C.S.; Ho, R.C. Immediate Psychological Responses and Associated Factors during the Initial Stage of the 2019 Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Epidemic among the General Population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Bruno, F.; Vozzo, F.; Arcuri, D.; Maressa, R.; La Cava, E.; Malvaso, A.; Lau, C.; Chiesi, F. The Longitudinal Association between Perceived Stress, PTSD Symptoms, and Post-Traumatic Growth during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Role of Coping Strategies and Psychological Inflexibility. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Mioni, G.; Cellini, N.; Romola, M.; Bruno, F. The Association between Balanced Time Perspective, Personality Traits, Subjective Well-Being, and Distress during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Timing Time Percept. 2022, 1, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Di Maggio, I.; Romola, M.; Lacava, E.; Maressa, R.; Bruno, F. Dealing with COVID-19 Stressful Events, Stress and Anxiety: The Mediating Role of Emotion Regulation Strategies in Italian Women. Health Psychol. Rep. 2023, 11, 70–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bruno, F.; Malvaso, A.; Chiesi, F.; Laganà, V.; Servidio, R.; Isella, V.; Ferrarese, C.; Gottardi, F.; Stella, E.; Agosta, F.; et al. COVID-19 Vaccine Uptake among Family Caregivers of People with Dementia: The Role of Attitudes toward Vaccination, Perceived Social Support and Personality Traits. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 923316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Chiesi, F.; Vizza, D.; Valente, M.; Bruno, R.; Lau, C.; Campagna, M.R.; Lo Iacono, M.; Bruno, F. Positive Personal Resources and Psychological Distress during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Resilience, Optimism, Hope, Courage, Trait Mindfulness, and Self-Efficacy in Breast Cancer Patients and Survivors. Support. Care Cancer 2022, 30, 7005–7014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Servidio, R.; Malvaso, A.; Vizza, D.; Valente, M.; Campagna, M.R.; Iacono, M.L.; Martin, L.R.; Bruno, F. The Intention to Get COVID-19 Vaccine and Vaccine Uptake among Cancer Patients: An Extension of the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB). Support. Care Cancer 2022, 30, 7973–7982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Greenstone, M.; Nigam, V. Does Social Distancing Matter? SSRN Electron. J. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hertenstein, M.J.; Weiss, S.J. The Handbook of Touch; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2011; ISBN 9780826121929. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bruno, F.; Canterini, S. La Scienza degli Abbracci: Alla Scoperta del Nostro Cervello Socio-Emotivo; Edizioni FrancoAngeli: Milan, Italy, 2018; ISBN 9788891771520. [Google Scholar]
  11. Light, K.C.; Grewen, K.M.; Amico, J.A. More Frequent Partner Hugs and Higher Oxytocin Levels Are Linked to Lower Blood Pressure and Heart Rate in Premenopausal Women. Biol. Psychol. 2005, 69, 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Grewen, K.M.; Girdler, S.S.; Amico, J.; Light, K.C. Effects of Partner Support on Resting Oxytocin, Cortisol, Norepinephrine, and Blood Pressure before and after Warm Partner Contact. Psychosom. Med. 2005, 67, 531–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Cohen, S.; Janicki-Deverts, D.; Turner, R.B.; Doyle, W.J. Does Hugging Provide Stress-Buffering Social Support? A Study of Susceptibility to Upper Respiratory Infection and Illness. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 26, 135–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Meijer, L.L.; Hasenack, B.; Kamps, J.C.C.; Mahon, A.; Titone, G.; Dijkerman, H.C.; Keizer, A. Affective Touch Perception and Longing for Touch during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 3887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. von Mohr, M.; Kirsch, L.P.; Fotopoulou, A. Social Touch Deprivation during COVID-19: Effects on Psychological Wellbeing and Craving Interpersonal Touch. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2021, 8, 210287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Marroquín, B.; Vine, V.; Morgan, R. Mental Health during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Effects of Stay-At-Home Policies, Social Distancing Behavior, and Social Resources. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 293, 113419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Field, T.; Poling, S.; Mines, S.; Bendell, D.; Veazey, C. Touching and Touch Deprivation during a COVID-19 Lockdown. Int. J. Psychol. Res. Rev. 2020, 3, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Rosenberg, M.; Luetke, M.; Hensel, D.; Kianersi, S.; Fu, T.; Herbenick, D. Depression and Loneliness during April 2020 COVID-19 Restrictions in the United States, and Their Associations with Frequency of Social and Sexual Connections. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2021, 56, 1221–1232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Biernacki, P.; Waldorf, D. Snowball Sampling: Problems and Techniques of Chain Referral Sampling. Sociol. Methods Res. 1981, 10, 141–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Henry, J.D.; Crawford, J.R. The Short-Form Version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): Construct Validity and Normative Data in a Large Non-Clinical Sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 44, 227–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Bottesi, G.; Ghisi, M.; Altoè, G.; Conforti, E.; Melli, G.; Sica, C. The Italian Version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21: Factor Structure and Psychometric Properties on Community and Clinical Samples. Compr. Psychiatry 2015, 60, 170–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Hosmer, D.W.; Lemeshow, S.; Netlibrary, I. Applied Logistic Regression; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2000; ISBN 9780471654025. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ministero della Salute. 2021. Available online: https://www.gazzettaufficiale.it/eli/id/2021/04/29/21A02641/sg (accessed on 24 July 2022).
Figure 1. Effects of positive touch on psychological distress during the COVID-19 Pandemic.
Figure 1. Effects of positive touch on psychological distress during the COVID-19 Pandemic.
Brainsci 13 00540 g001
Table 1. Frequency of reporting physical positive touch before pandemic and in the last month with cohabiting people.
Table 1. Frequency of reporting physical positive touch before pandemic and in the last month with cohabiting people.
Before Pandemic
N (%)
In the Last Month
N (%)
p-Value
How often have you hugged your partner?Almost everyday376 (67.0)309 (55.1)<0.001
1–3 times a week150 (26.7)169 (30.1)
Once or a few times31 (5.5)69 (12.3)
Not at all4 (0.7)14 (2.5)
How often have you kissed your partner?Almost everyday373 (66.5)305 (54.4)<0.001
1–3 times a week150 (26.7)169 (30.1)
Once or a few times33 (5.9)70 (12.5)
Not at all5 (0.9)17 (3.0)
How often have you caressed your partner?Almost everyday373 (66.5)309 (55.1)<0.001
1–3 times a week148 (26.4)163 (29.1)
Once or a few times35 (6.2)73 (13.0)
Not at all5 (0.9)16 (2.9)
How often have you held your partner’s hands?Almost everyday364 (64.9)302 (53.8)<0.001
1–3 times a week153 (27.3)159 (28.3)
Once or a few times40 (7.1)83 (14.8)
Not at all 4 (0.7)17 (3.0)
How often have you hugged your children?Almost everyday203 (81.9)185 (74.6)<0.001
1–3 times a week29 (11.7)31 (12.5)
Once or a few times14 (5.6)31 (12.5)
Not at all2 (0.8)1 (0.4)
How often have you kissed your children?Almost everyday192 (77.4)179 (72.2)0.002
1–3 times a week 33 (13.3)32 (12.9)
Once or a few times 19 (7.7)33 (13.3)
Not at all 4 (1.6)4 (1.6)
How often have you caressed your children?Almost everyday198 (79.8)180 (72.6)0.001
1–3 times a week27 (10.9)33 (13.3)
Once or a few times20 (8.1)32 (12.9)
Not at all 3 (1.2)3 (1.2)
How often have you held your children’s hands?Almost everyday 193 (77.8)177 (71.4)0.001
1–3 times a week33 (13.8)34 (13.7)
Once or a few times19 (7.7)34 (13.7)
Not at all 3 (1.2)3 (1.2)
How often have you hugged your relatives?Almost everyday217 (47.9)145 (32.0)<0.001
1–3 times a week95 (21.0)107 (23.6)
Once or a few times123 (27.2)158 (34.9)
Not at all 18 (4.0)43 (9.5)
How often have you kissed your relatives?Almost everyday 198 (43.7)127 (28.0)<0.001
1–3 times a week88 (19.4)87 (19.2)
Once or a few times138 (30.5)177 (39.1)
Not at all 29 (6.4)62 (13.7)
How often have you caressed your relatives?Almost everyday196 (43.3)132 (29.1)<0.001
1–3 times a week90 (19.9)88 (19.4)
Once or a few times128 (28.3)171 (37.7)
Not at all 39 (8.6)62 (13.7)
How often have you held your relatives’ hands?Almost everyday203 (44.8)143 (31.6)<0.001
1–3 times a week87 (19.2)81 (17.9)
Once or a few times 128 (28.3)168 (37.1)
Not at all35 (7.7)61 (13.5)
How often have you hugged your friends?Almost everyday19 (31.1)11 (18.0)<0.001
1–3 times a week18 (29.5)15 (24.6)
Once or a few times17 (27.9)21 (34.4)
Not at all 7 (11.5)14 (23.0)
How often have you kissed your friends?Almost everyday 18 (29.5)9 (14.8)<0.001
1–3 times a week 10 (16.4)12 (19.7)
Once or a few times21 (34.4)22 (36.1)
Not at all 12 (19.7)18 (29.5)
How often have you caressed your friends?Almost everyday19 (31.1)10 (16.4)<0.001
1–3 times a week 9 (14.8)12 (19.7)
Once or a few times 19 (31.1)20 (32.8)
Not at all 14 (23.0)19 (31.1)
How often have you held your friends’ hands?Almost everyday 20 (32.8)11 (18.0)<0.001
1–3 times a week 14 (23.0)11 (18.0)
Once or a few times19 (31.1)23 (37.7)
Not at all 8 (13.1)16 (26.2)
Notes. N = frequency, % = valid percentage, p-value = Wilcoxon test for correlated samples.
Table 2. Frequency of reporting physical positive touch before the COVID-19 Pandemic and in the last month with non-cohabiting people.
Table 2. Frequency of reporting physical positive touch before the COVID-19 Pandemic and in the last month with non-cohabiting people.
Before Pandemic
N (%)
In the Last Month
N (%)
p-Value
How often have you hugged your children?Almost everyday13 (14.9)6 (6.9)<0.001
1–3 times a week 38 (43.7)25 (28.7)
Once or a few times36 (41.4)47 (54.0)
Not at all0 (0.0)9 (10.3)
How often have you kissed your children?Almost everyday 12 (13.8)6 (6.9)<0.001
1–3 times a week 36 (41.4)23 (26.4)
Once or a few times 37 (42.5)47 (54.0)
Not at all 2 (2.3)11 (12.6)
How often have you caressed your children?Almost everyday12 (13.8)6 (6.9)<0.001
1–3 times a week37 (42.5)23 (26.4)
Once or a few times37 (42.5)47 (54.0)
Not at all 1 (1.1)11 (12.6)
How often have you held your children’s hands?Almost everyday 14 (16.1)6 (6.9)<0.001
1–3 times a week36 (41.4)24 (27.6)
Once or a few times36 (41.4)46 (52.9)
Not at all 1 (1.1)11 (12.6)
How often have you hugged your relatives?Almost everyday102 (10.3)40 (4.0)<0.001
1–3 times a week374 (37.7)131 (13.2)
Once or a few times446 (45.0)553 (55.8)
Not at all 69 (7.0)267 (26.9)
How often have you kissed your relatives?Almost everyday 95 (9.6)41 (4.1)<0.001
1–3 times a week331 (33.4)116 (11.7)
Once or a few times474 (47.8)530 (53.5)
Not at all 91 (9.2)304 (30.7)
How often have you caressed your relatives?Almost everyday93 (9.4)40 (4.0)<0.001
1–3 times a week294 (29.7)115 (11.6)
Once or a few times479 (48.3)524 (52.9)
Not at all 125 (12.6)312 (31.5)
How often have you held your relatives’ hands?Almost everyday104 (10.5)41 (4.1)<0.001
1–3 times a week314 (31.7)129 (13.0)
Once or a few times 479 (48.3)527 (53.2)
Not at all94 (9.5)294 (29.7)
How often have you hugged your friends?Almost everyday122 (12.3)24 (2.4)<0.001
1–3 times a week500 (50.5)117 (11.8)
Once or a few times319 (32.2)555 (56.0)
Not at all 50 (5.0)295 (29.8)
How often have you kissed your friends?Almost everyday 103 (10.4)18 (1.8)<0.001
1–3 times a week 407 (41.1)92 (9.3)
Once or a few times370 (37.3)495 (49.9)
Not at all 111 (11.2)386 (39.0)
How often have you caressed your friends?Almost everyday96 (9.7)20 (2.0)<0.001
1–3 times a week 336 (33.9)85 (8.6)
Once or a few times 396 (40.0)506 (51.1)
Not at all 163 (16.4)380 (38.3)
How often have you held your friends’ hands?Almost everyday 132 (13.3)22 (2.2)<0.001
1–3 times a week 398 (40.2)104 (10.5)
Once or a few times359 (36.2)522 (52.7)
Not at all 102 (10.3)343 (34.6)
Notes. N = frequency, % = valid percentage, p-value = Wilcoxon test for correlated samples.
Table 3. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch types and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress).
Table 3. Log binomial regression between the prevalence of the four physical positive touch types and each of the three mental health outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress).
DEPRESSION
Cohabiting PartnerβSE βWald’s χ2dfpOdds Ratio
(eβ)
95%CI
(eβ)
How often have you hugged your partner?−1.1870.505.5710.0180.300.11–0.82
How often have you kissed your partner?0.4810.411.3610.2431.620.72–3.63
How often have you caressed your partner?0.6100.491.5710.2101.840.71–4.78
How often have you held your partner’s hands?−0.3320.340.9610.3270.720.37–1.39
Overall model evaluation: Goodness-of-fit test: Hosmer and Lemeshow: χ2 = 3.13, df = 4, p = 0.54.
Nagelkerke R2 = 0.05.
ANXIETY
Cohabiting RelativesβSE βWald’s χ2dfpOdds Ratio
(eβ)
95%CI
(eβ)
How often have you hugged your relatives?−0.3040.251.4910.2230.740.45–1.20
How often have you kissed your relatives?−0.310.231.7710.1840.730.46–1.16
How often have you caressed your relatives?0.5750.274.4810.0341.781.04–3.03
How often have you held your relatives’ hands?−0.0430.220.0410.8450.960.62–1.47
Overall model evaluation: Goodness-of-fit test: Hosmer and Lemeshow: χ2 = 1.67, df = 6, p = 0.95.
Nagelkerke R2 = 0.02.
ANXIETY
Non-Cohabiting RelativesβSE βWald’s χ2dfpOdds Ratio
(eβ)
95%CI
(eβ)
How often have you hugged your relatives?−0.6090.313.8610.0490.540.30–1.00
How often have you kissed your relatives?0.6630.334.0110.0451.941.01–3.71
How often have you caressed your children?−0.330.340.9110.3390.720.37–1.41
How often have you held your relatives’ hands?0.1760.270.4210.5191.190.70–2.03
Overall model evaluation: Goodness-of-fit test: Hosmer and Lemeshow: χ2 = 1.35, df = 5, p = 0.93.
Nagelkerke R2 = 0.01.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bruno, F.; Tagliaferro, C.; Canterini, S.; Laganà, V.; Contrada, M.; Fioravanti, C.; Altomari, N.; Pistininzi, R.; Tarantino, F.; Placanica, A.; et al. Positive Touch Deprivation during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Effects on Anxiety, Stress, and Depression among Italian General Population. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 540. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci13040540

AMA Style

Bruno F, Tagliaferro C, Canterini S, Laganà V, Contrada M, Fioravanti C, Altomari N, Pistininzi R, Tarantino F, Placanica A, et al. Positive Touch Deprivation during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Effects on Anxiety, Stress, and Depression among Italian General Population. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(4):540. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci13040540

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bruno, Francesco, Carlotta Tagliaferro, Sonia Canterini, Valentina Laganà, Marianna Contrada, Chiara Fioravanti, Natalia Altomari, Rebecca Pistininzi, Francesca Tarantino, Alessia Placanica, and et al. 2023. "Positive Touch Deprivation during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Effects on Anxiety, Stress, and Depression among Italian General Population" Brain Sciences 13, no. 4: 540. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci13040540

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop