Next Article in Journal
Near-Infrared Photoimmunotherapy Combined with CTLA4 Checkpoint Blockade in Syngeneic Mouse Cancer Models
Next Article in Special Issue
Relationship between Influenza Vaccination Coverage Rate and COVID-19 Outbreak: An Italian Ecological Study
Previous Article in Journal
Exciting Times for Cytomegalovirus (CMV) Vaccine Development: Navigating the Pathways toward the Goal of Protecting Infants against Congenital CMV Infection
Previous Article in Special Issue
Trends, Spatial Disparities, and Social Determinants of DTP3 Immunization Status in Indonesia 2004–2016
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Behavioral Differences in the Preference for Hepatitis B Virus Vaccination: A Discrete Choice Experiment

1
China Population and Development Research Center, Beijing 100081, China
2
Dong Fureng Institute of Economic and Social Development, Wuhan University, Wuhan 430072, China
3
Center for Health Economics and Management in School of Economics and Management, Wuhan University, Wuhan 430072, China
4
Australian National Institute of Management and Commerce, Sydney, NSW 2015, Australia
5
Research Institute for International Strategies, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, Guangzhou 510420, China
6
School of Economics and School of Management, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300074, China
7
Newcastle Business School, University of Newcastle, Newcastle, NSW 2015, Australia
8
Management School, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L697ZH, UK
9
China Center for Health Development Studies, Peking University, Beijing 100191, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 8 August 2020 / Revised: 10 September 2020 / Accepted: 11 September 2020 / Published: 14 September 2020

Abstract

:
Understanding behavioral factors differences in the preferences for vaccinations can improve predictions of vaccine uptake rates and identify effective policy interventions to increase the demand for vaccinations. In this study, 353 adults in Shandong province in China were interviewed about their preferences for hepatitis B virus (HBV) vaccination. A discrete choice experiment (DCE) was employed to analyze the preference for HBV vaccinations, and a mixed logit model was used to estimate respondent preferences for vaccination attributes included in the DCE. While the protection rate against hepatitis B (HB), duration of protection, risk of side-effects, and vaccination cost were shown to influence adults’ preferences for HBV vaccination, adults valued “99% hepatitis B protection” above other attributes, followed by “20 years’ protection duration” and “1 in 150,000 risk of side-effects”. Individuals with lower time discount rates, non-overconfidence, or higher risk aversion were more likely to choose a vaccine. Lower risk aversion individuals showed a higher preference for lower risk of side-effects. Lower time discount rate individuals showed a higher preference for longer protection duration. Non-overconfidence individuals showed a higher preference for higher hepatitis B protection and cost. Interventions should be targeted to the behavioral determinants impeding vaccination.

1. Introduction

Vaccination is one of the most important public health interventions, saving 2–3 million lives every year, hundreds of millions of lives in the 20th century, and eradicating diseases such as smallpox [1]. Even though the beneficial impacts of vaccination on society and the individual are well proved, almost every country struggles with “vaccine hesitancy”. Some individuals refuse or delay vaccinations despite the availability of vaccines [2]. The consequences of vaccine hesitancy are serious both to the individual, who gets sick, and the society, as the disease spreads widely. Vaccine hesitancy has resulted in a series of repeated outbreaks of vaccine preventable diseases [3,4] such as measles, which made a comeback in the United States after being eradicated for over 19 years, with severe economy-wide consequences [5,6].
Vaccine hesitancy is complex, and standard rational economic models of decision-making display some weaknesses in explaining and predicting the vaccination decision [7,8,9]. The decision to vaccinate is based on its benefits and costs, which involves various uncertainties, such as the infection risk, vaccine effectiveness, and vaccine side-effects. Some of these uncertainties, such as side-effects, have a very low probability, which makes vaccine hesitancy difficult to comprehend [10]. One problem is that the financial costs of vaccinations occur around the time of the vaccination, while the benefits are reaped at some uncertain future point in time. This means that an individual’s attitude to risk and time, as well as confidence around the vaccine, play important roles in assessing the costs and benefits of vaccinations [11,12,13].
Recent research in behavioral economics, which incorporates insights from psychology, neuroscience, cognition and institutional economics, provides new insights into the vaccination decision [10,14,15,16,17,18]. The cost of the vaccine, risk aversion, time discount rate (or time preference), and overconfidence (or complacency) affect the decision to vaccinate. Risk aversion, which means an individual’s fear of being infected and the vaccine’s side-effects, has a positive effect on the decision to vaccinate [15,16]. Individuals’ time discount (time preference) rates suggest that people with smaller time discount rates (future-oriented or higher time preference individuals) display a greater probability to vaccinate [12,16]. The overconfidence effect, which is the individual’s tendency to underestimate “real” objective risks, does not have direct effects on vaccination decision, but has indirect effects by means of perception variables, such as the subjective probability of infection, and assessment of the effectiveness and side-effects of the vaccine [16,19].
Although previous studies have examined the association between behavioral factors, such as risk aversion, time discount rate, and overconfidence, with the vaccination decision, those studies did not account for consumer preferences. Providing an important tool for policymakers, preferences, or an individual’s valuation, can have a major impact on the willingness to use health care services [20]. It is important to understand the differences in vaccination preferences to establish effective interventions that support or complement public health vaccination policies. Drawing on hepatitis B virus (HBV) vaccination data for Shandong province, we evaluate the association between vaccination decisions and behavioral factors, and investigate how preferences vary across individual-specific behavioral factors.
HBV infection is a major public health issue in China, with an estimated 90 million chronic hepatitis B virus carriers, which accounts for almost 7% of the Chinese population [21]. In 2018, an estimated 1.28 million people were infected by viral hepatitis (dominated by type B) in China, with viral hepatitis the top infectious disease and the third killer infection in China in 2018 [22]. Free vaccination for children from 2002, with catch-up vaccinations for those born between 1995 and 2002, in 2009, dramatically reduced the HBV prevalence in children, but left those in the 15–44 age group displaying a high incidence of HBV [23,24]. These high incident groups born before 1995 must pay for the HB vaccine [25]. However, in 2020, hepatitis imposed a heavy economic burden on HBV sufferers, their families and the society [26]. In Shandong province, Lu et al. estimated the inpatient medical costs of hepatitis B-related diseases ranged from $US2954 to $US10,635 per patient and Zhang et al. found that in Jiangsu province the direct economic costs of HBV-related diseases ranged from $US107 for outpatients to $US3193 for inpatients [27,28]. These prevalence rates and public health costs call for expanded vaccinations of susceptible adults and the need for public health officials to intervene to reduce hepatitis B infections in adults.
Given the individual and economic costs of HBV in China, understanding behavioral factors differences in the preferences for vaccinations can throw light on vaccine uptake rates and guide policy makers in setting interventions to increase the demand for hepatitis B vaccinations. Equally important, our results from HBV provide insights into the vaccination decision in China generally.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sampling and Participants

The HBV vaccination program for adults was used to elicit vaccination preferences and to examine how preferences vary across different individual behavioral factor characteristics. Ten communities in Linyi city Shandong province were chosen on the basis of GDP per capita using a stratified random sampling method to conduct the face-to-face survey. In 2018, 50 individuals in each community were surveyed randomly using a questionnaire. Our response rate was 353 or 70.6%. Free HBV vaccinations for individuals born after 1994, limited adults in our study to those 24 or older.
A detailed explanatory statement of the study was provided to all respondents, which defined the study aims and confirmed no identifiable personal data would be collected and participation was voluntary. Verbal consent of respondents was obtained. The project was approved by the Ethics Committee at Shandong University (No. 201001052).

2.2. Study Design

Discrete choice experiment (DCE) methodology is a quantitative research method for eliciting preferences over hypothetical alternative scenarios, including the preferences of adults for different HBV vaccine characteristics that influence their decision to vaccinate [29,30,31]. Displayed in Table 1, the HBV vaccination attributes, comprising protection rates, out-of-pocket (OOP) expenses, protection duration and side-effect risks [32], and their levels were based on the extant literature and a panel of experts [25]. The 108 hypothetical alternatives from combining the four attributes, with each attribute level, meant a single individual was unable to answer all these alternatives, so a sample of alternatives was generated by using an orthogonal experimental design. Thirty-six choice questions addressed all main effects sufficiently, and these questions were divided randomly into 6 different versions of the questionnaire. Each version of the survey included 6 choice questions, with each question describing 2 alternative vaccines in terms of 4 attributes and a “neither vaccine or not vaccinate” option. Participants were asked, “If you were actually offered the 2 vaccines, which would you prefer to choose?” The questionnaire set out a detailed description of the attributes and also collected data on age, sex, level of education, income, health status, perceived risk of HBV infection, perceived severity of HBV, risk aversion, time discount rate, and overconfidence. According to Johnson and Orme [33], the minimum sample size for DCE is 167, which is below our sample of 353 responses.

2.3. Behavioral Factors

Follow previous research, three simple questions were used to measure risk aversion, time discount rate, and overconfidence [12,13,15,16]. Risk aversion was measured by: When you usually go out, how high does the probability between zero and 100 percent of rain have to be before you take an umbrella? The group with lower risk aversion was identified by using a cutoff of 50 percentage to generate a binary lower/high risk aversion variable. Time preference was measured by: “In planning your/your family’s saving and spending, which of the following time periods is more important to you and your husband/wife/partner?: next few weeks; next few months; next year; next few years; next 5–10 years; or longer than 10 years”. Participants who chose next year or longer were classified with a low time discount rate. The variable measuring overconfidence of respondents was based on the responses to the statement “I will never be robbed”, with response options 1, strongly agree to 5, strongly disagree. Participants with a score of above 2 were classified as non-overconfidence.

2.4. Control Variables

Information on participants’ sociodemographic characteristics, perceived risk of HBV infection, and perceived severity of HBV. Sociodemographic variables consisted of sex (female or male), age (under 40 or 40 and above), education attainment (primary school or below and secondary school or higher) and income (≤RMB 2000 or RMB > 2000 per month) were collected. Health status was measured by “Would you say your health is very good, good, fair, poor or very poor?”, which were dichotomized into good (very good or good) and poor (fair, poor, or very poor) health. The perceived risk of HBV infection was assigned values by the respondents depending on the answer to the question of “how likely would you be infected by HBV (Percentages between 0 and 100)”. The group with lower risk were identified by using a cutoff of 30 percentage to generate a binary perceived risk of HBV infection variable (0 = Low, 1 = High). The perceived severity of HBV was assigned values depending on how serious (not serious, moderately serious, or very serious) the respondent believed HBV.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were used to analyze the non-DCE variables. This study used a mixed logit model, with 500 Halton draws with normally distributed parameters, to estimate participants’ preferences for the HBV vaccine [34]. The mixed logit model allows for attribute coefficients to be randomly distributed, and improves the fit of the model significantly over conditional logit modelling. Each estimated coefficient is a preference weight and represents the relative contribution of the attribute level to the utility that respondents assign to an alternative [35]. There are 3 alternatives in each of the stated choice tasks in the DCE survey: vaccine A, vaccine B, or “neither vaccine or not vaccinate”. The association between individuals’ vaccination decisions to choose “neither vaccine or not vaccinate” and behavioral factors were tested using the mixed logit model with individual behavioral characteristics interacted with the ‘‘neither vaccine or not vaccinate” indicator [25]. For the interacted terms, positive (negative) estimators are correlated with decreased (increased) stated uptake. This study employed a set of models which included interaction between individuals’ behavioral characteristics (including risk aversion, time discount rate, and overconfidence) and all attributes of the vaccine to estimate the potential difference in preference between groups of individuals’ behavioral factors [36].
Following previous studies, we also estimated the changes in the probability of individuals choosing a vaccine with specified attributes [32,37]. These results are present in graphs illustrating the marginal effect of varying one attribute level at a time from the base case, holding all other attributes constant. All heterogeneities were considered in the calculation of the mean uptake, which means that the mean uptake is just not the uptake of someone with average coefficient values [25]. The impact of alternative levels of vaccine attributes on individuals’ preferences inform policymakers in determining vaccine strategies.
A p-value of 0.05 was considered statistically significant. All statistical analyses were performed in STATA 15 (Stata Corp, College Station, TX, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Respondent Characteristics

Table 2 displays the sample’s descriptive statistics. A total of 53.5% of the sample was male, 62.9% had a high education (years of schooling higher than nine years), and most reported their monthly income as more than RMB 2000. Our respondents reported that 56.7% had “good” or ‘‘excellent” health, 52.4% had a relatively lower risk of infection, and 38.2% had a perceived high severity of HBV. In terms of behavioral characteristics, 46.18% reported a lower risk aversion, 51.56% had a lower time discount rate, and 72.24% were non-confident.

3.2. General Preference for HBV Vaccine

Table 3 shows the mixed logit model results for preferences. In the base Model 1, all vaccination characteristics except “10 years’ protection duration” proved to influence adults’ preferences for the HBV vaccination (p < 0.05). The variable with the greatest magnitude of association with HBV vaccination preference was “99% hepatitis B protection”, followed by “20 years’ protection duration” and “1/150,000 risk of side-effects”. As expected, respondents demonstrated a negative preference for out-of-pocket costs.
Model 2 shows the mixed logit model with individual characteristics interacting with the “neither vaccine” indicator. For the interacting terms, positive (negative) values are associated with decreased (increased) stated uptake. Those who were older and those who perceived a high risk of infection were more likely to choose an HBV vaccine than to choose ‘‘neither vaccine”. Individuals with a lower time discount rate or non-overconfidence degree were more likely to take the vaccine while those with lower risk aversion were less likely to choose a vaccine and selected “neither vaccine” more often.

3.3. Behavioral Factors’ Differences in Preference for Vaccine

Results from three interaction models are presented in Table 4. In Model 3, the non-significant coefficient of the characteristic level “risk of side-effects” indicates that individuals with higher risk aversion did not significantly prefer to choose one of the side-effect levels over the reference level of side-effects. The positive signs for the interaction terms of ‘side-effects’ and lower risk aversion indicate that adults with a low risk aversion showed a significantly higher preference for “lower risk of side-effects” than adults with a high risk aversion. In Model 4, “20 years’ protection duration” was more important to adults with a lower time discount rate compared to adults with a higher time discount rate. Compared to adults with overconfidence, non-overconfidence respondents showed a higher preference for “99% protection against HBV” and willingness to pay more for vaccination (Model 5).

3.4. Change in the Probability of Participation

Figure 1 shows the univariate marginal change in the probability of individuals taking a vaccination. In our total sample, changes in the protection rate from 99% or 89% to 79%, protection duration from 20 years to five years, and risk of serious side-effects from 1/150,000 to 1/50,000 had a relatively large impact on the predicted probability of individuals taking a vaccination. A decreased risk of serious side-effects from 1/50,000 to 1/150,000 had a relatively large 10.1% (95% CI: 5.3% to 15.0%) impact on the expected uptake among the low risk aversion group, but a relatively small 2.9% (95% CI: 1.4% to 4.4%) impact among the high risk aversion group. Compared to the high time discount rate group, an increased protection duration from five years to 20 years had a relatively large 8.5% (95% CI: 4.0% to 13.0%) impact on the expected uptake in the low time discount rate group. An increased protection rate from 79% to 99% had a 16.2% (95% CI: 8.7% to 23.7%) impact on the predicted probability of uptake among the non-overconfident group, but a relatively small 7.9% (95% CI: 3.8% to 12.0%) impact among the overconfident group.

4. Discussion

This study used discrete choice experiment modelling to elicit the preferences for HBV vaccinations, investigated the association between individual’s behavioral factors and vaccination decisions, and examined how preferences vary across different individual behavioral factor characteristics. Understanding behavioral differences in the vaccination decision and preference for a vaccine is particularly important for designing more effective policy to increase the vaccination rate. The risk of side-effects, duration of protection, and protection rate were shown to influence adults’ vaccination decisions and preferences for HBV vaccination. Individuals with lower time discount rates, non-overconfidence or high risk aversion were more likely to choose a vaccine. Low risk aversion individuals showed a higher preference for lower risk of side-effects; lower time discount rate individuals showed a higher preference for longer protection duration; and non-overconfidence individuals showed a higher preference for higher hepatitis B protection and willingness to pay more.
This study found the protection rate, duration of protection, risk of side-effects, and out-of-pocket cost had an important influence on adults’ vaccination decision which is in line with previous DCE studies on preference for the HBV vaccination [25]. Protection rate (effectiveness), duration of protection, and risk of side-effects were also shown to influence preferences for HPV, influenza and rotavirus vaccinations in previous DCE studies [32,37,38,39]. In addition, DCE studies that investigated preferences for HPV vaccination found that out-of-pocket cost influence mothers’ preferences for vaccinating daughters, which is in line with our results [37].
Our results showed that high risk aversion was positively associated with the decision to be vaccinated, which is consistent with previous studies [15]. In theory, risk aversion can affect the decision of vaccination through two opposing paths. First, the fear of being infected by the disease will lead some to vaccinate and, second, the fear of the vaccine’s side-effects will prevent others from vaccinating [16]. Since the risk of getting infected is much larger than the risk of side-effects, the impact of the perceived risk of getting infected dominates the impact of the risk of side-effects.
This study found that adults with low risk aversion showed a significantly higher preference for “lower risk of side-effects” than adults with high risk aversion, which has not been addressed in previous research. This means that low risk aversion individuals will give greater weight to the risk of side-effects than high risk aversion individuals, and have higher sensitivity to any change in side-effect risks. The dominant effect of the risk of getting infection weakens in individuals with low risk aversion, and even a minor increase (1/150,000 to 1/50,000) in risk of side-effects will have a large decrease (10.1%) in the expected uptake in those people.
This study confirms that individuals’ time preference is associated with the vaccination decision [11,15,16], and further illustrates that preferences for “protection duration” vary across different individual time discount rates. As expected, individuals with a lower time discount rate (more future oriented) were more likely to take the vaccine, and showed a higher preference for “longer protection duration” than individuals with higher time discount rate. Since the benefits of the vaccine are reaped at some uncertain future point in time, more future oriented individuals will give a high weight to the benefits of the vaccine under the certain protection duration than lower time preference individuals [12]. Individuals with a high time discount rate will give high weight to the costs since they occur around the time of vaccination [13]. In addition, individuals with lower time preference will give more weight on longer protection duration.
The current study found that overconfidence directly affects the vaccine decision while previous research only found the indirect influence via perceived probability of infection and the severity of the disease [15,16]. After controlling for perceived risk of infection and the severity of the disease, the overconfident were still less likely to take the vaccine and selected “neither vaccine” more often. A possible explanation is that individuals with high overconfidence will overestimate their own ability, such as resistance to diseases, while simultaneously underestimating the risk of infection and the severity of the disease [40,41]. In addition, compared to overconfidence, non-overconfidence shows higher sensitivity to the vaccine protection rate and lower sensitivity to the cost. Non-overconfident respondents felt more threatened by infectious diseases [3]. Therefore, they valued the effectiveness of vaccine more than others, and cared less about the cost than others.
The results of this paper have important policy implications. Adult decision-making frequently deviates from rationality, with behavioral factors playing an important role in the decision and preference for vaccinations. Interventions by health policymakers should consider irrational aspects of human behavior and should targeted interventions to address behavioral differences. For example, vaccine interventions should target the behavioral factors that imped vaccination. For individuals with low risk aversion, their high sensitivity to adverse side-effects means that public policies should address misperceptions about vaccine side-effects. In terms of time preference, policy measures should emphasize the protection duration, which will improve the uptake among future oriented individuals; deploying incentives might be an effective intervention for individuals with higher time discount rates [3,7]. When people do not vaccinate because of overconfidence, the dissemination of information on disease risks and severity, or lowering the vaccination cost, may help raise the vaccination rate [3,15].
Although this study enhances the understanding of the behavioral factors’ differences in the preference for vaccination, it has several limitations. First, the study is only able to demonstrate adults’ preference for the attributes and levels presented [42]. While this study undertook an extensive literature review and interviews with experts in the field of HBV vaccination to guard against bias of omitted variables, certain meaningful attributes may not have been included. Second, our sample only contains information from one province of China, with a relatively small sample size, so the generalizability to populations outside of this region requires further studies undertaken in other provinces and countries. Finally, although this study analyzed how the demand and preference varies across different levels of risk aversion, time discount rate, and overconfidence, the analysis cannot demonstrate whether the preferences differ between other behavioral factors, such as altruism [43] and status quo bias [16]. These variables should be included in future studies.

5. Conclusions

Individuals with lower time discount rates, non-overconfidence, or high risk aversion were more likely to choose to vaccinate. Low risk aversion individuals showed a higher preference for the lower risk of side-effects, lower time discount rate individuals showed a higher preference for longer protection duration, and non-overconfidence individuals showed a higher preference for higher hepatitis B protection and willingness to pay more. Interventions should be targeted to address the behavioral determinants that imped vaccinations.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.Z. and N.G.; methodology, D.Z.; software, D.Z.; validation, D.Z., and N.G.; formal analysis, D.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, N.G.; writing—review and editing, D.Z., S.N., J.W., and E.M.; funding acquisition, D.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (No.71904005) and China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (No.2018M641126).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. World Health Organization. World Immunization Week 2012. Available online: https://www.who.int/immunization/newsroom/events/immunization_week/2012/further_information/en/ (accessed on 11 October 2019).
  2. MacDonald, N.E. Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine 2015, 33, 4161–4164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Betsch, C.; Böhm, R.; Chapman, G.B. Using behavioral insights to increase vaccination policy effectiveness. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2015, 2, 61–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Phadke, V.K.; Bednarczyk, R.A.; Salmon, D.A.; Omer, S.B. Association between vaccine refusal and vaccine-preventable diseases in the United States: A Review of Measles and Pertussis. JAMA 2016, 315, 1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Lo, N.C.; Hotez, P.J. Public health and economic consequences of vaccine hesitancy for Measles in the United States. JAMA Pediatr. 2017, 171, 887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Measles Cases and Outbreaks. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/measles/cases-outbreaks.html (accessed on 11 October 2019).
  7. Cappelen, A.; Mæstad, O.; Tungodden, B. Demand for Childhood Vaccination—Insights from Behavioral Economics. J. Dev. Stud. 2010, 37, 349–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hornsey, M.J.; Harris, E.A.; Fielding, K.S. The psychological roots of anti-vaccination attitudes: A 24-nation investigation. Health Psychol. 2018, 37, 307–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Hendrix, K.S.; Finnell, S.M.E.; Zimet, G.D.; Sturm, L.A.; Lane, K.A.; Downs, S.M. Vaccine message framing and parents’ intent to immunize their infants for MMR. Pediatrics 2014, 134, e675–e683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Brewer, N.T.; Chapman, G.B.; Rothman, A.J.; Leask, J.; Kempe, A. Increasing vaccination: Putting psychological science into action. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2017, 18, 149–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Shahrabani, S.; Gafni, A.; Ben-Zion, U. Low flu shot rates puzzle-some plausible behavioral explanations. Am. Econ. 2008, 52, 66–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Chapman, G.B.; Coups, E.J. Time preferences and preventive health behavior: Acceptance of the Influenza Vaccine. Med. Decis. Mak. 1999, 19, 307–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Chapman, G.B. Short-term cost for long-term benefit: Time preference and cancer control. Health Psychol. 2005, 24, S41–S48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Matjasko, J.L.; Cawley, J.H.; Baker-Goering, M.M.; Yokum, D.V. Applying behavioral economics to public health policy. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2016, 50, S13–S19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Tsutsui, Y.; Benzion, U.; Shahrabani, S.; Din, G.Y. A policy to promote influenza vaccination: A behavioral economic approach. Health Policy 2010, 97, 238–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Tsutsui, Y.; Benzion, U.; Shahrabani, S. Economic and behavioral factors in an individual’s decision to take the influenza vaccination in Japan. J. Soc. Econ. 2012, 41, 594–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Buttenheim, A.M.; Fiks, A.G.; Burson, R.C., II; Wang, E.; Coffin, S.E.; Metlay, J.P.; Feemster, K.A. A behavioral economics intervention to increase pertussis vaccination among infant caregivers: A randomized feasibility trial. Vaccine 2016, 34, 839–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Böhm, R.; Meier, N.W.; Groß, M.; Korn, L.; Betsch, C. The willingness to vaccinate increases when vaccination protects others who have low responsibility for not being vaccinated. J. Behav. Med. 2019, 42, 381–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Russo, E.J.; Schoemaker, P.J.H. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Manag. Rev. 1992, 33, 7–17. [Google Scholar]
  20. Phillips, K.A.; Van, B.S.; Marshall, D.; Walsh, J.; Thabane, L. A review of studies examining stated preferences for cancer screening. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2006, 3, A75. [Google Scholar]
  21. World Health Organization. WHO Technical Consultation on a Comprehensive National Hepatitis Programme in China with a Focus on Viral Hepatitis B and C Treatment, Beijing, China, 21 February 2014: Meeting Report; WHO Regional Office for the Western Pacific: Manila, Philippines, 2014; p. 31. [Google Scholar]
  22. National Health Commission. China Health Yearbook; China Union Medical University Press: Beijing, China, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hadler, S.C.; Fuqiang, C.; Averhoff, F.; Taylor, T.; Fuzhen, W.; Li, L.; Xiaofeng, L.; Weizhong, Y. The impact of hepatitis B vaccine in China and in the China GAVI Project. Vaccine 2013, 31, J66–J72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Zhu, D.; Wang, J.; Wangen, K.R. Hepatitis B vaccination coverage rates among adults in rural China: Are economic barriers relevant? Vaccine 2014, 32, 6705–6710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Guo, N.; Zhang, G.; Zhu, D.; Wang, J.; Shi, L. The effects of convenience and quality on the demand for vaccination: Results from a discrete choice experiment. Vaccine 2017, 35, 2848–2854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Hu, M.; Chen, W. Assessment of total economic burden of chronic Hepatitis B (CHB)-related diseases in Beijing and Guangzhou, China. Value Health 2009, 12, S89–S92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Lu, J.; Xu, A.; Wang, J.; Zhang, L.; Song, L.; Li, R.; Zhang, S.; Zhuang, G.; Lu, M. Direct economic burden of hepatitis B virus related diseases: Evidence from Shandong, China. BMC Health Serv. Res. 2013, 13, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Zhang, H.; Chao, J.; Zhu, L.; Song, L.; Li, X.; Liu, P. Direct economic burden and influencing factors in patients with Hepatitis B virus related diseases in Jiangsu, China. Asia Pac. J. Public Health 2015, 27, 41S–48S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ryan, M. Discrete choice experiments in health care. BMJ 2004, 328, 360–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ryan, M.; Bate, A.; Eastmond, C.J.; Ludbrook, A. Use of discrete choice experiments to elicit preferences. Qual. Saf. Health Care 2001, 10, i55–i60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Liao, Q.; Lam, W.W.T.; Wong, C.K.H.; Lam, C.; Chen, J.; Fielding, R. The relative effects of determinants on Chinese adults’ decision for influenza vaccination choice: What is the effect of priming? Vaccine 2019, 37, 4124–4132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. De Bekker-Grob, E.W.; Hofman, R.; Donkers, B.; van Ballegooijen, M.; Helmerhorst, T.J.M.; Raat, H.; Korfage, I.J. Girls’ preferences for HPV vaccination: A discrete choice experiment. Vaccine 2010, 28, 6692–6697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. de Bekker-Grob, E.W.; Donkers, B.; Jonker, M.F.; Stolk, E.A. Sample size requirements for discrete-choice experiments in healthcare: A practical guide. Patient Patient Cent. Outcomes Res. 2015, 8, 373–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Hole, A.R.; Kolstad, J.R. Mixed logit estimation of willingness to pay distributions: A comparison of models in preference and WTP space using data from a health-related choice experiment. Empir. Econ. 2012, 42, 445–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Janssen, E.M.; Hauber, A.B.; Bridges, J.F.P. Conducting a discrete-choice experiment study following recommendations for good research practices: An application for eliciting patient preferences for diabetes treatments. Value Health 2018, 21, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Zhu, D.; Shi, X.; Nicholas, S.; Ye, X.; Chen, S.; He, P. Preferences for hearing aid attributes among people with moderate or greater hearing loss in rural China: A discrete choice experiment. Patient Prefer Adher. 2020, 14, 643–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Brown, D.S.; Johnson, F.R.; Poulos, C.; Messonnier, M.L. Mothers’ preferences and willingness to pay for vaccinating daughters against human papillomavirus. Vaccine 2010, 28, 1702–1708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. De Bekker-Grob, E.W.; Veldwijk, J.; Jonker, M.; Donkers, B.; Huisman, J.; Buis, S.; Swait, J.; Lancsar, E.; Witteman, C.L.M.; Bonsel, G.; et al. The impact of vaccination and patient characteristics on influenza vaccination uptake of elderly people: A discrete choice experiment. Vaccine 2018, 36, 1467–1476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Veldwijk, J.; Lambooij, M.S.; Bruijning-Verhagen, P.C.J.; Smit, H.A.; Wit, G.A.D. Parental preferences for rotavirus vaccination in young children: A discrete choice experiment. Vaccine 2014, 32, 6277–6283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Svenson, O. Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychol. 1981, 47, 143–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Dunning, D. Essays in social psychology. In Self-Insight: Roadblocks and Detours on the Path to Knowing Thyself; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; ISBN 978-1-84169-074-2. [Google Scholar]
  42. Larson, E.; Vail, D.; Mbaruku, G.M.; Kimweri, A.; Freedman, L.P.; Kruk, M.E. Moving toward patient-centered care in Africa: A discrete choice experiment of preferences for delivery care among 3,003 Tanzanian women. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0135621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Böhm, R.; Betsch, C.; Korn, L. Selfish-rational non-vaccination: Experimental evidence from an interactive vaccination game. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2016, 131, 183–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Univariate marginal estimates for predicted probability of participation.
Figure 1. Univariate marginal estimates for predicted probability of participation.
Vaccines 08 00527 g001
Table 1. Attributes and levels.
Table 1. Attributes and levels.
AttributesLevels
Risk of serious side-effects1/50,000; 1/100,000; 1/150,000
Protection duration (years)5; 10; 20
Protection rate against HBV (%)79; 89; 99
Out-of-pocket cost (RMB)0; 30; 60; 90
Table 2. Respondent characteristics.
Table 2. Respondent characteristics.
CharacteristicsAll Sample (N = 353)
Socioeconomic factors
Sex
Female164 (46.5)
Male189 (53.5)
Age group
Under 40213 (60.3)
41+140 (39.7)
Education level
Low education131 (37.1)
High education222 (62.9)
RMB income group (monthly)
≤2000 145 (41.1)
2001+ 208 (58.9)
Health status
Poor 153 (43.3)
Good200 (56.7)
Perceived risk of HBV infection
Low185 (52.4)
High168 (47.6)
Perceived severity of HBV
Low126 (35.7)
Moderate92 (26.1)
High135 (38.2)
Behavioral factors
Risk aversion
Higher193 (53.82)
Lower163 (46.18)
Time discount rate
Higher171 (48.44)
Lower182 (51.56)
Overconfidence
Yes98 (27.76)
No255 (72.24)
Table 3. Mixed logit model results for a discrete choice experiment addressing adults’ preferences for HBV vaccination.
Table 3. Mixed logit model results for a discrete choice experiment addressing adults’ preferences for HBV vaccination.
Model 1Model 2
Constant7.558(0.830) ***6.990(1.467) ***
Attribute
Out-of-pocket cost−0.010(0.003) ***−0.010(0.003) ***
Risk of side-effects (ref. = 1/50,000)
1/100,0000.215(0.096) *0.226(0.097) *
1/150,0000.418(0.097) ***0.415(0.092) ***
Protection duration (ref. = 5 years)
10 years0.109(0.096)0.110(0.095)
20 years0.456(0.095) ***0.439(0.092) ***
Protection rate (ref. = 79%)
89% 0.363(0.096) ***0.365(0.094) ***
99%0.993(0.107) ***0.977(0.103) ***
Socioeconomic factors
Neither × Male 0.311(0.668)
Neither × Age 40+ −3.079(0.799) ***
Neither × High education 0.290(0.822)
Neither × Income 2000 RMB+ 0.344(0.892)
Neither × Good health status −0.235(0.511)
Neither × High risk of infection −1.070(0.580)
Neither × Perceived moderate severity −0.229(0.695)
Neither × Perceived high severity −1.738(0.656) **
Behavioral factors
Neither × Lower risk aversion 2.049(0.497) ***
Neither × Lower time discount rate −1.185(0.594) *
Neither × Non-overconfidence degree −1.166(0.558) *
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses. *** p <0.001, ** p <0.01, * p <0.05.
Table 4. Mixed logit model results with interactions for a discrete choice experiment addressing adults’ preferences for HBV vaccination.
Table 4. Mixed logit model results with interactions for a discrete choice experiment addressing adults’ preferences for HBV vaccination.
Model 3
Lower Risk Aversion
Model 4
Lower Time Discount Rate
Model 5
Non-Overconfidence
Constant7.870 (0.940) ***6.976 (0.915) ***6.597 (0.684) ***
Attribute
Out-of-pocket cost−0.011 (0.004) **−0.010 (0.004) **−0.024 (0.006) ***
Risk of side-effects (ref. = 1/50,000)
1/100,0000.030 (0.139)0.114 (0.118)0.098 (0.168)
1/150,0000.202 (0.127)0.341 (0.118) **0.347 (0.172) *
Protection duration (ref. = 5 years)
10 years0.240 (0.129)0.127 (0.120)0.147 (0.174)
20 years0.443 (0.126) ***0.258 (0.116) *0.298 (0.168)
Protection rate (ref. = 79%)
89% 0.267 (0.130) *0.315 (0.121) **0.114 (0.177)
99%0.916 (0.142) ***0.859 (0.133) ***0.547 (0.184) **
Interaction terms
Out-of-pocket cost × covariate−0.001 (0.005)0.000 (0.005)0.018 (0.006) **
Risk of side-effects (ref. = 1/50,000)
1/100,000 × covariate0.433 (0.208) *0.143 (0.182)0.136 (0.209)
1/150,000 × covariate0.598 (0.198) **0.131 (0.192)0.121 (0.211)
Protection duration (ref. = 5 years)
10 years × covariate−0.319 (0.198)−0.009 (0.181)−0.029 (0.211)
20 years × covariate0.054 (0.191)0.413 (0.176) *0.283 (0.204)
Protection rate (ref. = 79%)
89% × covariate0.220 (0.197)0.059 (0.181)0.343 (0.213)
99% × covariate0.218 (0.204)0.177 (0.192)0.669 (0.227) **
Notes: Standard errors in parentheses. *** p <0.001, ** p <0.01, * p <0.05.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Guo, N.; Wang, J.; Nicholas, S.; Maitland, E.; Zhu, D. Behavioral Differences in the Preference for Hepatitis B Virus Vaccination: A Discrete Choice Experiment. Vaccines 2020, 8, 527. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/vaccines8030527

AMA Style

Guo N, Wang J, Nicholas S, Maitland E, Zhu D. Behavioral Differences in the Preference for Hepatitis B Virus Vaccination: A Discrete Choice Experiment. Vaccines. 2020; 8(3):527. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/vaccines8030527

Chicago/Turabian Style

Guo, Na, Jian Wang, Stephen Nicholas, Elizabeth Maitland, and Dawei Zhu. 2020. "Behavioral Differences in the Preference for Hepatitis B Virus Vaccination: A Discrete Choice Experiment" Vaccines 8, no. 3: 527. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/vaccines8030527

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop