Next Article in Journal
Toward a Representation of Semantic Change in Linked Data
Next Article in Special Issue
Buenas no[tʃ]es y mu[ts]isimas gracias: A Sociophonetic Study of the Alveolar Affricate in Peninsular Spanish Political Speech
Previous Article in Journal
Updating Old English Dative–Genitives: A Diachronic Construction Grammar Account
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Feature Alignment Approach to Plural Realization in Eastern Andalusian Spanish
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Phonation Patterns in Spanish Vowels: Spectral and Spectrographic Analysis

Department of Modern Languages and Linguistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 22 February 2024 / Revised: 3 May 2024 / Accepted: 22 May 2024 / Published: 12 June 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Phonetics and Phonology of Ibero-Romance Languages)

Abstract

:
This article provides a detailed examination of voice quality in word-final vowels in Spanish. The experimental task involved the pronunciation of words in two prosodic contexts by native Spanish speakers from diverse dialects. A total of 400 vowels (10 participants × 10 words × 2 contexts × 2 repetitions) were analyzed acoustically in Praat. Waveforms and spectrograms were inspected visually for voice, creak, breathy voice, and devoicing cues. In addition, the relative amplitude difference between the first two harmonics (H1–H2) was obtained via FFT spectra. The findings reveal that while creaky voice is pervasive, breathy voice is also common, and devoicing occurs in 11% of tokens. We identify multiple phonation types (up to three) within the same vowel, of which modal voice followed by breathy voice was the most common combination. While creaky voice was more frequent overall for males, modal voice tended to be more common in females. In addition, creaky voice was significantly more common at the end of higher prosodic constituents. The analysis of spectral tilt shows that H1–H2 clearly distinguishes breathy voice from modal voice in both males and females, while H1–H2 values consistently discriminate creaky and modal voice in male participants only.

1. Introduction

This article investigates the occurrence of non-modal voicing in word-final vowels in Spanish. Modal voicing is characterized by periodic vibration of the vocal folds (Bissiri et al. 2011; Garellek 2014), while non-modal voicing involves non-periodicity and/or a degree of noise (Laver 1980; Esling et al. 2019). Modal voicing is commonly referred to in the literature as “voicing”, while non-modal voicing encompasses phonatory qualities such as voicelessness, creaky voice and breathy voice. Voicelessness is defined by a lack of vocal fold vibration, while creaky voice (or creak) involves a glottal constriction, a low rate of vocal fold vibration (i.e., low pitch), and/or irregular F0 (Ladefoged 1971; Gordon and Ladefoged 2001; Garellek 2019, among others). Breathy voice involves voicing in addition to noise, and concentration of acoustic energy in the F3 region (Laver 1980; Keating et al. 2015; Garellek 2014, 2019; Esling et al. 2019). These phonation qualities relate to the relative degree of the vocal fold aperture, which is most open for voicelessness, then breathy voice, and most constricted for modal voice, then creaky voice (see, for example, Gordon and Ladefoged 2001).
In Spanish, as in many other languages, vowels are typically voiced, but non-modal realizations are attested in specific phonological environments and dialects. For example, vowel sequences across words (as in la uva ‘the grape’) can have creaky voice (Lorenzo Criado 1948; Lope Blanch 1987; González and Weissglass 2017). This is often the case when Spanish is in a contact situation with a language that has phonological glottalization, such as Yucatec Maya, Guaraní, or Arabic (Thon 1989; Valentín-Márquez 2006; Chappell 2013; Michnowicz and Kagan 2016; McKinnon 2018; Mohamed et al. 2019; Gynan and López Almada 2020). Vowels can also have creaky voice word-finally in various dialects, including Peninsular, Chilean, and Mexican Spanish (Morrison and Escudero 2007; Garellek and Keating 2015; Bolyanatz 2023; cf. Kim 2017). Fewer studies focusing on breathy voice are available for Spanish. Mendoza et al. (1996) and Trittin and de Santos y Lleó (1995) find that Spanish-speaking females have breathier voices than males. This is also the case in English and other languages, and it is considered to result from anatomical differences in males and females: the larynx in females tends to have a longer opening, particularly posteriorly, which is conducive to aspiration noise in the F3 region (Klatt and Klatt 1990). Breathy vowels are also reported to occur utterance-finally in Andalusian Spanish (O’Neill 2005). As for voiceless vowels, they are also attested word- and utterance-finally in Spanish, often when preceded (or followed) by voiceless consonants (Delforge 2009, 2012; Torreira and Ernestus 2011; Sessarego 2012; Dabkowski 2018).
In a previous study, González et al. (2022) examined the occurrence of creaky voice in word-final Spanish vowels. Creaky voice was analyzed via visual inspection of waveforms and spectrograms, following Dilley et al. (1996), Redi and Shattuck-Hufnagel (2001) and Ladefoged and Maddieson (1996). Specifically, creaky voice was coded when one or more of the following acoustic cues was present: aperiodicity (non-regular duration of pulses), creak (gradual pulse widening with F0 lowering and damping), diplophonia (variable pulse amplitude or shape), glottal squeak (sudden F0 increase), or the presence of a glottal stop. González et al. (2022) found a prevalence of creaky voice word-finally for vowels across several Spanish dialects, particularly for men, and at the end of higher prosodic constituents. The data were originally examined for creaky voice only, but further inspection showed that at least some of the vowels coded as creaky tended to end in a noisy and/or non-periodic interval which could be consistent with breathy voice and/or devoicing.
In this study, we re-examine our original dataset to provide a more nuanced analysis of non-modal phonation in word-final Spanish vowels, focusing not just on creaky voice, but also on breathy voice and devoicing. We provide a more detailed analysis of non-modal phonation using waveforms and spectrograms, and, unlike in González et al. (2022), we additionally include a measure of spectral tilt, i.e., the degree of energy present in lower vs. higher frequencies. We focus in particular on H1–H2 values, i.e., the relative amplitude of the first harmonic (H1) (corresponding to the fundamental frequency or F0) compared to the second harmonic (H2), since previous studies show that H1–H2 correlates well with differences between modal voice, breathy voice, and creaky voice (see, for example, Klatt and Klatt 1990; Hillenbrand et al. 1994; Trittin and de Santos y Lleó 1995; Gordon and Ladefoged 2001; Kreiman and Gerratt 2012; Keating et al. 2015; Kim 2017; Garellek 2019). Specifically, breathy voice has a higher spectral tilt (and, therefore, a higher H1–H2 amplitude) than modal voice, and modal voice has in turn a higher spectral tilt than creaky voice. These differences spring from open quotient differences related to phonation. Creaky voice, for example, has a low open quotient since it involves glottal constriction and increased medial vocal fold thickness. Open quotient is high for modal voice, and highest for breathy voice, since the vocal folds have a wider opening in the latter.
This study focuses on two research questions: (1) what is the prevalence of non-modal phonation beyond creaky voice in word-final vowels, and (2) does the distribution of phonation type differ in terms of speaker sex (i.e., male vs. female) or prosodic context (i.e., at the end of full or intermediate intonational phrases). Regarding (1), we expect to find examples of both breathy voice and devoicing in addition to creaky voice in our dataset. For (2), we hypothesize that non-modal phonation will be more prevalent at the end of full intonational phrases (IPs) than at the end of intermediate ones (ips), and that creaky voice will be more prevalent for males and breathy voice in females.
The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. Section 2 outlines the methodology of the study; experimental findings are presented in Section 3. Section 4 provides a discussion, and Section 5 closes with concluding remarks.

2. Methodology

This study is part of a larger project and builds on the analysis of creaky voice reported in González et al. (2022). The participants were 10 native Spanish speakers from a range of different dialects (Argentina, Bolivia, Colombia (2), Cuba (2), Peru, Puerto Rico, Spain, Venezuela). The participants from Spain, Argentina, Peru, and one from Colombia were male; the rest were female. All were 20–39 years old at the time of recording and had spent between 0 and 14 years in the US. All were raised in Spanish-speaking countries and spoke Spanish daily in their personal and professional life.
The study was approved by the IRB board of Florida State University. Participant data were collected in the phonetics laboratory after obtaining written informed consent. A digital recorder with a high-quality cardioid condenser microphone and a presence boost adapter was used to record audio data. Recordings were obtained in .wav format, in mono, with a sampling rate of 44,100 Hz.
This phase of the project involved a picture identification task comprising 12 images that participants had to name and frame in a short sentence. For example, when participants were shown a picture of a window, they were asked to say Ventana. Es una Ventana. (‘Window. It’s a window.’). A short training phase preceded the task. The picture identification task involved 10 token words and two distractors, one at the beginning and one at the end, to avoid list intonation effects. The task was conducted twice per participant.
Stimuli had penultimate stress, were two or three syllables long, and ended in /a/ or /o/ (Table 1). The final vowel of each token word occurred at the end of two different prosodic contexts: (i) a lower prosodic constituent, corresponding to an intermediate intonational phrase (ip) in the Spanish Tones and Breaks Indices framework (Sp_ToBI; see Beckman et al. 2002; Sosa 2003; Aguilar et al. 2009; Estebas-Vilaplana and Prieto 2009); and (ii) a higher prosodic constituent, corresponding to a full intonational phrase (IP) (1). The ends of full intonational phrases in our data involve a distinct pause coinciding with the end of the participant’s turn and tend to be realized with a final low boundary tone (L%). In contrast, the ends of intermediate phrases are cued by a slight or no pause and often involve a rise (a high boundary tone H-) to indicate the message is continuing (Frota et al. 2007; Aguilar et al. 2009; Baxter 2017).
(1)a.[Ventana.]ip[Esunaventana.]IP
Window.(It) isawindow.
A total of 400 words were analyzed (10 participants × 10 tokens × 2 prosodic contexts × 2 repetitions) using Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2023). All measurements were taken by hand. Acoustic analysis involved inspection of the waveform and spectrogram for cues of modal and non-modal voicing. As in González et al. (2022), modal voice was characterized by periodicity, while creaky voice was characterized by one or more of the following: (i) irregular F0 (‘aperiodicity’), (ii) F0 lowering, (iii) changes in pulse amplitude or shape (‘diplophonia’), and/or (iv) presence of silence followed by a stop burst (‘glottal stop’) (Dilley et al. 1996; Docherty and Foulkes 2005; Gordon and Ladefoged 2001; Huber 1988; Keating et al. 2015; Ladefoged and Maddieson 1996; Redi and Shattuck-Hufnagel 2001). All token vowels were also examined for intervals involving lack of voicing—which were coded as devoiced—and for intervals involving noise in the F3 region, coded as breathy voice (Laver 1980; Keating et al. 2015; Garellek 2014, 2019; Esling et al. 2019). H2-H1 measurements were also taken via FFT spectra. Afterwards, token vowels were analyzed and divided into phonation intervals of modal voicing, creaky voice, breathy voice, and voicelessness based on visual cues from the spectrogram and waveform. FFT spectra were generated at the middle of each phonation interval to measure the relative amplitude of the first harmonic (H1) compared to the second harmonic (H2). As indicated in Section 1, H1–H2 is highest for breathy voice and lowest for creaky voice (Garellek 2019).
Figure 1, Figure 2 and Figure 3 provide examples of waveforms, spectrograms, and FFT spectra for vowels fully realized as breathy, modal, and creaky (see Figure 4 and Figure 5 below for examples of vowels involving more than one phonation interval). As shown in Figure 1b, breathy voice is characterized by a much higher amplitude of the first harmonic compared to the second. For modal voice, the first harmonic is higher in amplitude than the second (Figure 2b), but not as much as with breathy voice. On the other hand, creaky voice is characterized by a higher amplitude of the second harmonic compared to the first (Figure 3a).
All coding and measurements were checked by at least two of the authors. Note that we chose to conduct the spectral analysis of phonation intervals rather than full vowels or syllables, unlike in previous studies focusing on Spanish (see, for example, Trittin and de Santos y Lleó 1995; Mendoza et al. 1996; Kim 2017). This methodological approach allows for a fine-grained exploration of voice quality, capturing cases where vowels have two or more phonation type sequences or ‘dynamic combinations of non-modal phonations’ in the words of Esposito and Khan (2020, p. 2) (for example, vowels that begin as creaky but end as breathy, as shown in Figure 4 and Figure 5 below; see also Ladefoged 1983; DiCanio 2009).
For the analysis of phonation type distribution in word-final vowels, 18 tokens were discarded since they involved errors (4 tokens) or were produced as non-final in the ip context (14 tokens). Descriptive statistics were produced for 382 vowels. When determining if phonation type was associated with prosodic context and/or speaker sex, the 382 vowels were partitioned into 685 phonation intervals coded as modal, creaky, breathy, or devoiced. The analysis of H1–H2 did not include voiceless intervals (n = 72) since harmonics are only present in periodic sounds. In addition, any phonation intervals occupying less than 30% of the vowel duration (n = 64) were also discarded. The reason for this exclusion is two-fold: we were interested in obtaining clear H1–H2 cut-off points among modal, creaky, and breathy voice, which would be facilitated by longer analysis windows in FFT spectra. In addition, the 30% threshold has been used in other studies focusing on the analysis of creaky voice (Bolyanatz 2023), based on the fact that at least 30% of a vowel needs to have creak to be perceived as such utterance-finally (Crowhurst 2018). This resulted in the H1–H2 analysis of 549 phonation intervals (females, n = 354; males, n = 195).
Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 29) (IBM Corp. 2022) and R (Version 4.3.1) (R Core Team 2023); these included ANOVAs, chi-squared tests, and Bonferroni post hoc tests. Statistical analyses were considered significant if p ≤ 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Non-Modal Phonation in Word-Final Vowels: Overall Findings

As shown in Table 2, the most common type of non-modal phonation attested in word-final vowels in our database was creaky voice (occurring in 31% of vowels), followed closely by breathy voice (29%). Devoicing was found in 11% of cases. Creaky voice, when present, tended to encompass a larger portion of the vowel duration compared to breathy voice and devoicing (66%, 52%, and 36%, respectively).
Table 3 shows the relative occurrence of phonation combinations for all participants pooled. Overall, vowels with double phonation are the most common (58%), followed by vowels pronounced with single phonation (31%). Vowels with triple phonation are attested only in 11% of cases. The two most common phonation realizations are modal vowels ending in breathy voice (23%) and fully creaky vowels (19%). Creaky/breathy voiced vowels and modal/creaky voiced vowels are also relatively common (11% and 9%, respectively). Vowels including three different types of phonation mostly begin with modal voice, although some instances involving initial creak are also attested.
Most cases of vowels pronounced with double phonation end in breathy voice (34%), followed by devoicing (15%). In contrast, only 9% of vowels produced with double phonation end in creaky voice. For vowels involving triple phonation, most end in devoicing (5%) or breathy voice (5%). In contrast, only 1% of vowels end in creaky voice. Examples of vowels with double and triple phonation ending in breathy voice or devoicing are given in Figure 4 and Figure 5.
Some positional generalizations for non-modal phonation were observed. Fully devoiced vowels were not attested, and devoicing did not precede any other phonation type. In vowels pronounced with double phonation, modal voice could only precede another phonation type, not follow it; and if a vowel began as breathy voiced, it could only be followed by devoicing. Finally, all cases of triple phonation involved modal or creaky voice followed by other phonation types.

3.2. Speaker Sex and Prosodic Context

3.2.1. Phonation Type and Speaker Sex

Figure 6 and Table 4 illustrate the frequency of occurrence of phonation type for male and female speakers. Results from a Pearson’s two-sided chi-squared test of independence show a significant association between phonation type and sex (χ2 (3, 685) = 15.559, p = 0.001). Males tend to favor creaky voice, while females show only a slight preference for modal voice over other phonation types. The frequency of occurrence of breathy voice and devoicing are comparable in both groups.

3.2.2. Phonation Type and Prosodic Context

Figure 7 and Table 5 illustrate the frequency of occurrence of phonation type at the end of intermediate and full intonation phrases (ips and IPs, respectively). Results from a Person’s two-sided chi-squared test of independence show that there is a significant association between phonation type and prosodic context (χ2 (3, 685) = 17.342, p < 0.001). Modal voice was more prevalent than other phonation types at the end of intermediate phrases (ips), while creaky voice and breathy voice were the most frequent phonation types attested at the end of full intonational phrases (IPs). The frequency of occurrence of devoicing was comparable in both contexts.
As shown in Table 6, the two most common phonation types attested ip-finally for female participants are modal vowels ending in breathy voice (46%) and modal vowels ending in creaky voice (13%). IP-finally, fully creaky vowels, modal/breathy vowels, and creaky/breathy vowels are the most common phonation realizations (25%, 22%, and 19%, respectively). Other phonation type combinations occur in less than 10% of cases and are for the most part comparable across prosodic contexts.
For male participants, fully creaky vowels were the most common vowel type in both prosodic contexts (Table 7). ip-finally, modal vowels, modal/creaky, and creaky/devoiced vowels were also relatively common (13%, 16% and 13%, respectively). IP-finally, the second most common phonation type was breathy voice (24%).
There was a significant interaction between sex and prosodic context for breathy voice (χ2 (1, 201) = 4.311, p = 0.027) (Figure 8, Table 8). For males, breathy voice was more widespread at the end of full intonational phrases (IPs), unlike for females, where breathy voice occurred as frequently in both prosodic contexts.
There was also a significant association between sex and prosodic context for creaky voice (χ2 (1, 215) = 3.882, p = 0.033) (Figure 9, Table 9). For women, creaky voice was more widespread at the end of full intonational phrases (IPs). For men, creaky voice was only slightly more frequent at the end of intermediate phrases (ips) than full intonational phrases (IPs).
For devoicing, no significant association was found between sex and prosodic context (χ2 (1, 72) = 0.252, p = 0.4 (Figure 10, Table 10). For males, devoicing was equally as prevalent at the end of ips and IPs. For females, there was slighly more devoicing at the end of full intonational phrases.

3.3. Visual Cues and Spectral Tilt

Figure 11 and Table 11 shows average H1–H2 differences in phonation types for both sexes. As expected, H1–H2 values are higher for breathy voice. For males, H1–H2 values are higher for modal voice than creaky voice. However, H1–H2 values are very similar for modal voice and creaky voice intervals for females.
The results from a one-way ANOVA show a significant difference for H1–H2 and phonation type for female participants (df = 2, N = 331; F = 28.776, p < 0.001). The results from a Bonferroni post hoc analysis with multiple comparisons shows a significant difference between breathy voice and modal voice and between breathy voice and creaky voice; the difference between modal voice and creaky voice is not significant (Table 12).
For males, the results from a one-way ANOVA also show a significant difference for H1–H2 values across phonation type (df = 2, N = 188; F = 52.669, p < 0.001). The results from Bonferroni post hoc tests with multiple comparisons show that modal voice, breathy voice, and creaky voice are all significantly different from each other (Table 13).

4. Discussion

Our study re-examines the dataset in González et al. (2022) to investigate the occurrence of non-modal voice quality beyond creaky voice at the end of prosodic constituents in Spanish, an area that is generally understudied for Spanish. Our results show that both creaky voice and breathy voice occur frequently at the end of ips and IPs; devoicing, while attested, is much less common.
Unlike previous studies, we move beyond considering one type of non-modal phonation only (as in Kim 2017 or González et al. 2022 for creaky voice; or Mendoza et al. 1996; and Trittin and de Santos y Lleó 1995, which focus on breathy voice) and provide a fine-grained description and analysis of phonation combinations in word-final vowels in Spanish. We find that vowels with double phonation are the most widespread in our data, particularly those beginning with modal voice and ending in breathy voice, followed by vowels with single phonation, especially fully creaky vowels. In addition, 11% of the vowels in our dataset involved triple phonation. The latter tend to begin with modal voice, although some instances involving initial creaky voice are also attested.
Fully devoiced vowels are unattested in the contexts examined; in addition, devoicing is positionally restricted to the end of the vowel. Other languages with positional restrictions for non-modal phonation include Santa Ana del Valle Zapotec, where non-modal phonation occurs only vowel-finally (Esposito 2005), and White Hmong, where breathy voice can only occur vowel-initially (Keating et al. 2010). In our dataset, vowels with double or triple phonation overwhelmingly tend to end in breathy voice or devoicing. As suggested by one reviewer, it is possible that this relates to vocal fold spreading in anticipation of a pause or breath intake. The fact that dynamic phonation combinations are common in Spanish vowels, and that some are more frequent than others, is not only a novel finding, but might be helpful for the segmentation of Spanish vowels in future studies, particularly those focusing on word-final contexts.
Our findings reveal a significant effect of prosodic context on phonation type. Modal voice is more widespread at the end of intermediate phrases (ips) overall, while creaky and breathy voice are more common at the end of full intonational phrases (IPs). These results are in line with prior studies showing that non-modal phonation voice is a cue to the end of higher prosodic constituents across several languages. This is the case for devoicing and breathy voice in French (Smith 1999) and for creaky voice in English, Spanish, and Italian, among other languages (Keating et al. 2015; Dilley et al. 1996; González et al. 2022; Di Napoli 2015).
In addition, our results show a significant effect of sex on phonation type, with creaky voice being more common in males, and modal and breathy voice occurring more frequently in females. Similar results for creaky and modal voice were reported in González et al. (2022). Biologically, males tend to have longer and thicker vocal folds than females, resulting in a lower pitch/F0, which may be more conducive to creaky voice overall, particularly at the end of prosodic constituents when intonation falls. On the other hand, females’ vocal folds tend to not close completely when vibrating, which can often result in breathy voice (Laver 1980). We find of particular interest the fact that while males favored creaky voice, they tended to use breathy voice more often at the end of IPs. On the other hand, while females tended to prefer modal or breathy voice, they significantly had more creaky voice at the end of IPs. These findings suggest that both creaky voice and breathy voice can cue the end of higher prosodic units in Spanish, depending on the sex of the speaker. It is interesting that, as shown in Table 2, among non-modal phonation creaky voice has the longest duration, followed by breathy voice, while devoicing tends to be very short. This might indicate that creaky voice is a more salient prosodic cue at the end of constituents in Spanish than breathy voice or devoicing.
Our study also investigates the relative alignment between spectral tilt, as measured by H1–H2, and visual phonation cues in waveforms and spectrograms. As expected, spectral tilt is highest for breathy voice, both for males and females (see, for example, Klatt and Klatt 1990; Garellek 2019). For males, spectral tilt is also higher in modal voice than in creaky voice, as expected, although the average H1–H2 value for creak is not necessarily negative, as reported in some of the prior literature. For females, however, H1–H2 values for modal and creaky voice are practically the same. As Esposito and Khan (2020, p. 8) point out, H1–H2 sometimes fails to successfully measure specific voice qualities. For example, in Marathi H1–H2 is a reliable acoustic cue of breathy voice for male speakers but not so consistent for female ones (Berkson 2012).
There are several possible reasons explaining the lack of complete alignment between spectral tilt and visual phonation cues for creaky and modal voice in the female participants in our dataset. These include (i) a wide range of individual variation in spectral tilt for Spanish females; (ii) the occurrence of nasalization in stimuli where the final vowel is preceded by a nasal segment (as in ventana ‘window’); (iii) a possible effect of vowel quality on harmonic amplitude (Klatt and Klatt 1990; Garellek et al. 2016; Garellek 2019, 2022). We consider (iii) to be the most likely explanation: our study included tokens ending in /a/, which has a high F1 that does not largely impact voice quality, but also ending in /o/, usually involving a relatively low first formant that might have influenced the amplitudes of the first and second harmonics. Further studies investigating voice quality in Spanish could include the use of additional spectral tilt measures and/or use formant correction algorithms in the analysis of H1–H2 (Iseli et al. 2007; Esposito et al. 2021). In any case, we conclude that the identification of breathy voice in Spanish vowels benefits from using H1–H2 measurements, for both males and females. For creaky voice, however, visual examination of cues such as aperiodicity and diplophonia in waveforms and spectrograms might be enough to identify it consistently.
The participants in this study were Spanish-dominant bilingual speakers with English as their L2 who used Spanish daily in personal and professional interactions. Some had just arrived in the US, and some had been living in this country for up to 40 years. Cantor-Cutiva et al. (2023) reports that Spanish-dominant bilingual speakers do not produce creaky voice as often as English-dominant bilinguals, while Kim (2017) shows that Spanish–English bilinguals often transfer creaky voice from English into Spanish (Kim 2017). González et al. (2022), who examined creaky voice in the participants of the present study, argue against English transfer of this voice quality based on the fact that (i) creaky voice was as frequent in participants who had just moved to the US as in participants who had been residing in the US for many years, and (ii) creaky voice is more frequent for females than males in American English, unlike in the Spanish dataset, which shows the opposite trend. While the present study still shows this pattern, we find it intriguing that the female participants had significantly more creak at the end of IPs, while male participants preferred breathy voice in the same content. We consider then that transfer of creaky voice from English might be a possibility at the end of higher prosodic units, at least for females, but we leave this point for future investigation.
Finally, the participants of this study were from eight different Spanish-speaking countries. Except for the case of Colombian Spanish, all Spanish dialects represented included male or female speakers, but not both. Some dialectal differences in phonation are reported for Spanish; for example, Kim (2017) did not find creaky voice utterance-finally in native speakers of Mexican Spanish (unlike Garellek and Keating 2015). We leave a more detailed investigation of dialectal differences in the voice quality of Spanish vowels for future investigations.

5. Conclusions

This study contributes to the investigation of phonation in Spanish vowels. It shows that creaky voice and breathy voice are pervasive word-finally, particularly at the end of full intonational phrases (IPs). While creaky voice was favored by males and modal and breathy voice by females, at the end of full intonational phrases males tended to have more breathy voice, and females more creaky voice. Our findings also show that word-final Spanish vowels often involves double or triple phonation types. In vowels realized with multiple phonation types, modal voice tends to precede other voice qualities; devoicing, when present, always occurs at the end of the vowel. These results have implications for future acoustic studies of Spanish vowels, which are often assumed to be modally voiced throughout.
Our study included the examination of phonatory visual cues in waveforms and spectrograms in addition to the measurement of H1–H2 values. The latter was calculated for each phonation interval, rather than as the average for the entire vowel, resulting in a more fine-grained investigation of voice quality. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time this methodology is employed, at least for Spanish. Our results show that H1–H2 differences between modal and breathy voice align well with phonatory visual cues in acoustic displays. However, H1–H2 values consistently distinguish modal and creaky voice for males only. Future studies on Spanish vowels should include formant correction algorithms for H1–H2 and/or the use of additional spectral tilt measures, particularly if focusing on female speakers. It is our hope that other scholars continue to investigate phonation in vowels, since it can inform our understanding of their acoustic and perceptual characteristics both in L1 and L2 Spanish.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, all authors; methodology, all authors; writing—original draft preparation, C.G.; writing—review and editing, all authors; acoustic analysis, all authors; statistics, G.R.I.; visualization, S.L.C. and G.R.I.; supervision, C.G.; funding acquisition, C.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was funded in part by a COFRS grant awarded to the first author by Florida State University in 2014 (award ID: 082003 551 032945 and agency: FSU Council of Research and Creativity).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of Florida State University (protocol code HSC No. 2011.7371) for studies involving humans.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of the conditions specified in the IRB approval for the project.

Acknowledgments

Our sincere thanks to all participants of the study, the linguistics faculty and students at the Department of Modern Languages and Linguistics at Florida State University for their support, and the audience at the 2023 International Conference on Experimental Phonetics/Congreso Internacional de Fonética Experimental (CIFE) held at the University of Vigo in Spain for their feedback and questions. We are also immensely grateful to the three anonymous reviewers of this article for their suggestions, which have greatly improved this article. All errors, of course, remain ours. Finally, we would like to acknowledge the assistance provided by ChatGPT, an AI language model developed by OpenAI, in guiding our searches and providing valuable insights on statistical analyses and R/SPSS procedures related to the research conducted in this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Aguilar, Lourdes, Carme De-la-Mota, and Pilar Prieto. 2009. Sp_ToBI Training Materials. Available online: http://prosodia.upf.edu/sp_tobi/ (accessed on 22 February 2024).
  2. Baxter, Robert Patrick. 2017. Co-Constructing Turns as Interactional Competence: Collaborative Turn Sequences in L1 and L2 Spanish. Ph.D. dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA. [Google Scholar]
  3. Beckman, Mary E., Manuel Díaz-Campos, Julia Tevis McGory, and Terrell A. Morgan. 2002. Intonation across Spanish, in the Tones and Break Indices framework. Probus 14: 9–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Berkson, Kelly H. 2012. Acoustic correlates of breathy sonorants in Marathi. Journal of Phonetics 73: 70–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bissiri, Maria Paola, Maria Luisa Lecumberri, Martin Cooke, and Jan Volín. 2011. The role of word-initial glottal stops in recognizing English words. Paper presented at INTERSPEECH 2011, Florence, Italy, August 27–31; pp. 165–68. [Google Scholar]
  6. Blanch, Lope. 1987. Estudios sobre el español de Yucatán. Mexico City: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. [Google Scholar]
  7. Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2023. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (3.17.24) [Computer Software]. Available online: https://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/ (accessed on 3 May 2023).
  8. Bolyanatz, Mariška. 2023. Creaky Voice in Chilean Spanish: A Tool for Organizing Discourse and Invoking Alignment. Languages 8: 161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cantor-Cutiva, L. C., P. Bottalico, J. Webster, C. Nudelman, and E. Hunter. 2023. The Effect of Bilingualism on Production and Perception of Vocal Fry. Journal of Voice 37: 970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Chappell, Whitney. 2013. Social and Linguistic Factors Conditioning the Glottal Stop in Nicaraguan Spanish. Ph.D. dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA. [Google Scholar]
  11. Criado, Lorenzo. 1948. El habla de Albalá: Contribución al studio de la dialectología extremeña. Revista de Estudios Extremeños 4: 398–407. [Google Scholar]
  12. Crowhurst, Megan J. 2018. The Influence of Varying Vowel Phonation and Duration on Rhythmic Grouping Biases among Spanish and English Speakers. Journal of Phonetics 66: 82–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Dabkowski, Meghan F. 2018. Variable Vowel Reduction in Mexico City Spanish. Ph.D. dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA. [Google Scholar]
  14. Delforge, Ann M. 2009. The Rise and Fall of Unstressed Vowel Reduction in the Spanish of Cusco, Peru: A Sociophonetic Study. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Davis, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
  15. Delforge, Ann M. 2012. ‘Nobody wants to sound like a provinciano’: The recession of unstressed vowel devoicing in the Spanish of Cusco, Perú. Journal of Sociolinguistics 16: 311–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Di Napoli, Jessica. 2015. Glottalization at phrase boundaries in Tuscan and Roman Italian. In The Phonetics-Phonology Interface: Representations and Methodology. Edited by Joaquín Romero and María Riera. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 125–48. [Google Scholar]
  17. DiCanio, Christian. 2009. The phonetics of register in Takhian thong Chong. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 39: 162–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Dilley, Laura, Stephanie Shattuck-Hufnagel, and Mari Ostendorf. 1996. Glottalization of word-initial vowels as a function of prosodic structure. Journal of Phonetics 24: 423–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Docherty, Gerard, and Paul Foulkes. 2005. Glottal varitants of (t) in the Tyneside variety of English: An acoustic profiling study. In A Figure of Speech—A Festschrift for John Laver. Edited by William Hardcastle and Janet Beck. London: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 173–99. [Google Scholar]
  20. Esling, John H., Scott R. Moisik, Allison Benner, and Lise Crevier-Buchman. 2019. Voice Quality: The Laryngeal Articulator Model. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Esposito, Christina M. 2005. An acoustic and electroglottographic study of phonation in Santa Ana del Valle Zapotec. Paper presented at the 79th Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, San Francisco, CA, USA, November 18–21. [Google Scholar]
  22. Esposito, Christina M., and Sameer ud Dowla Khan. 2020. The cross-linguistic patterns of phonation types. Language and Linguistics Compass 14: e12392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Esposito, Christina M., Morgan Sleeper, and Kevin Schäfer. 2021. Examining the relationship between vowel quality and voice quality. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 51: 361–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Estebas-Vilaplana, Eva, and Pilar Prieto. 2009. La notación prosódica del español: Una revisión del Sp_ToBI [Prosodic notation in Spanish: A review of Sp_ToBI]. Estudios de Fonética Experimental 17: 265–83. [Google Scholar]
  25. Frota, Sonia, Mariapaola d’Imperio, Gorka Elordieta, Pilar Prieto, and Marina Vigário. 2007. The phonetics and phonology of intonational phrasing in Romance. In Segmental and Prosodic Issues in Romance Phonology. Edited by Pilar Prieto, Joan Mascaró and Maria-Josep Solé. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 131–53. [Google Scholar]
  26. Garellek, Marc. 2014. Voice quality strengthening and glottalization. Journal of Phonetics 45: 106–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Garellek, Marc. 2019. The phonetics of voice. In The Routledge Handbook of Phonetics. Edited by William F. Katz and Peter F. Assmann. London: Routledge, pp. 75–106. [Google Scholar]
  28. Garellek, Marc. 2022. Theoretical achievements of phonetics in the 21st century: Phonetics of voice quality. Journal of Phonetics 94: 101155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Garellek, Marc, and Patricia Keating. 2015. Phrase-final creak: Articulation, acoustics, and distribution. Paper presented at the 89th Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Portland, OR, USA, January 8. [Google Scholar]
  30. Garellek, Marc, Robin Samian, Bruce R. Gerratt, and Jody Kreiman. 2016. Modeling the voice source in terms of spectral slopes. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 139: 1404–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. González, Carolina, and Christine Weissglass. 2017. Hiatus resolution in L1 and L2 Spanish: An optimality account. In Selected Papers from the 45th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages. Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory (LSRL) 12. Edited by Ruth E. V. Lopes, J. Juanito Ornelas de Avelar and Sonia M. L. Cyrino. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 79–96. [Google Scholar]
  32. González, Carolina, Christine Weissglass, and Daniel Bates. 2022. Creaky voice and prosodic boundaries in Spanish: An acoustic study. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 15: 33–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Gordon, Matthew, and Peter Ladefoged. 2001. Phonation types: A cross-linguistic overview. Journal of Phonetics 29: 383–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gynan, Shaw Nicholas N., and Ernesto Luís López Almada. 2020. The Glottal Stop in Guaraní and Paraguayan Spanish. Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics. Edited by Rajiv Rao. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 28, pp. 227–62. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hillenbrand, James, Ronald A. Cleveland, and Robert L. Erickson. 1994. Acoustic correlates of breathy vocal quality. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37: 769–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Huber, Dieter. 1988. Aspects of the Communicative Fuction of Voice in text Intonation: Constancy and Variability in Swedish Fundamental Frequency Contours. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Göteborg, Göteborg, Sweden. [Google Scholar]
  37. IBM Corp. 2022. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 29.0. Armonk: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
  38. Iseli, Markus, Yen-Liang Shue, and Abeer Alwan. 2007. Age, sex, and vowel dependencies of acoustic measures related to the voice source. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 121: 2283–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Keating, Patricia, Christina Esposito, Marc Garellek, Sameer ud Dowla Khan, and Jianjing Kuang. 2010. Phonation contrasts across languages. UCLA Working Paper in Phonetics 108: 188–202. [Google Scholar]
  40. Keating, Patricia, Marc Garellek, and Jody Kreiman. 2015. Acoustic properties of different kinds of creaky voice. Paper presented at 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS), Glasgow, UK, August 10–14. [Google Scholar]
  41. Kim, Ji Young. 2017. Voice quality transfer in the production of Spanish heritage speakers and English L2 learners of Spanish. In Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory. Edited by Perpiñán Silvia, David Heap, Itziri Moreno-Villamar and Adriana Soto-Corominas. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 11, pp. 191–207. [Google Scholar]
  42. Klatt, Dennis H., and Laura C. Klatt. 1990. Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 87: 820–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Kreiman, Jody, and Bruce R. Gerratt. 2012. Perceptual interactions of the harmonic source and noise in voice. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 131: 492–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Ladefoged, Peter. 1971. Preliminaries to Linguistic Phonetics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. Ladefoged, Peter. 1983. The linguistic use of different phonation types. In Vocal Fold Physiology: Contemporary Research and Clinical Issues. Edited by Diane M. Bless and James Abbs. San Diego: College-Hill Press. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ladefoged, Peter, and Ian Maddieson. 1996. The Sounds of the World’s Languages. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  47. Laver, John. 1980. The Phonetic Description of Voice Quality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  48. McKinnon, Sean. 2018. A sociophonetic analysis of word-initial vowel glottalization in monolingual and bilingual Guatemalan Spanish. Paper presented at Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA, October 25. [Google Scholar]
  49. Mendoza, Elvira, Nieves Valencia, Juana Muñoz, and Humberto Trujillo. 1996. Differences in voice quality between men and women: Use of the long-term average spectrum (LTAS). Journal of Voice 10: 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Michnowicz, Jim, and Laura Kagan. 2016. On Glottal Stops in Yucatan Spanish: Language Contact and Dialect Standardization. Edited by Sandro Sessarego and Fernando Tejedo-Herrero. Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, vol. 8, pp. 217–40. [Google Scholar]
  51. Mohamed, Sherez, Carolina González, and Anje Muntendam. 2019. Arabic-Spanish Language Contact in Puerto Rico: A Case of Glottal Stop Epenthesis. Languages 4: 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Morrison, Geoffrey, and Paula Escudero. 2007. A cross-dialect comparison of Peninsular-and Peruvian-Spanish vowels. Paper presented at 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrücken, Germany, August 6–10; pp. 1505–8. [Google Scholar]
  53. O’Neill, Paul. 2005. Utterance final /s/ in Andalusian Spanish. The phonetic neutralization of a phonological contrast. Language Design 7: 151–66. [Google Scholar]
  54. R Core Team. 2023. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, Version 4.3.1; Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 May 2023).
  55. Redi, Laura, and Stephanie Shattuck-Hufnagel. 2001. Variation in the realization of glottalization in normal speakers. Journal of Phonetics 29: 407–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Sessarego, Sandro. 2012. Unstressed vowel reduction in Cochabamba, Bolivia. Revista Internacional de Lingüística Iberoamericana 10: 213–27. [Google Scholar]
  57. Smith, Caroline. 1999. Marking the boundary: Utterance-final prosody in French questions and statements. Paper presented at 14th International Congress of Phonetic Science, San Francisco, CA, USA, August 1–7; pp. 1181–84. [Google Scholar]
  58. Sosa, Juan Manuel. 2003. La notación tonal del español en el modelo Sp_ToBI. In Teorías de la entonación [Theories of Intonation]. Edited by Pilar Prieto. Barcelona: Ariel, pp. 155–84. [Google Scholar]
  59. Thon, Sonia. 1989. The glottal stop in the Spanish spoken in Corrientes, Argentina. Hispanic Linguistics 3: 199–218. [Google Scholar]
  60. Torreira, Francisco, and Mirjam Ernestus. 2011. Realization of voiceless stops and vowels in conversational French and Spanish. Laboratory Phonology 2: 331–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Trittin, Pamela J., and Andrés de Santos y Lleó. 1995. Voice quality analysis of male and female Spanish speakers. Speech Communication 16: 359–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Valentín-Márquez, Wilfredo. 2006. La Oclusión Glotal y la Construcción Lingüística de Identidades Sociales en Puerto Rico. In Selected Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Edited by Nuria Sagarra and Almeida J. Toribio. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 326–41. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Breathy voice in word-final /o/ in redondo ‘round’, male speaker (P2).
Figure 1. Breathy voice in word-final /o/ in redondo ‘round’, male speaker (P2).
Languages 09 00214 g001
Figure 2. Modal voice in word-final /o/ in redondo ‘round’, male speaker (P2).
Figure 2. Modal voice in word-final /o/ in redondo ‘round’, male speaker (P2).
Languages 09 00214 g002
Figure 3. Vowel with full creak: ventana ‘window’, male speaker (P7).
Figure 3. Vowel with full creak: ventana ‘window’, male speaker (P7).
Languages 09 00214 g003aLanguages 09 00214 g003b
Figure 4. Example of double phonation. Casa ‘house’. P5 (female), IP context. 5000 Hz, 40 dB.
Figure 4. Example of double phonation. Casa ‘house’. P5 (female), IP context. 5000 Hz, 40 dB.
Languages 09 00214 g004
Figure 5. Example of triple phonation. Toro ‘bull’. P3 (male), ip context. 5000 Hz, 40 dB.
Figure 5. Example of triple phonation. Toro ‘bull’. P3 (male), ip context. 5000 Hz, 40 dB.
Languages 09 00214 g005
Figure 6. Phonation type frequency by speaker sex.
Figure 6. Phonation type frequency by speaker sex.
Languages 09 00214 g006
Figure 7. Percentages of frequency of phonation type by prosodic context.
Figure 7. Percentages of frequency of phonation type by prosodic context.
Languages 09 00214 g007
Figure 8. Breathy voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Figure 8. Breathy voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Languages 09 00214 g008
Figure 9. Creaky voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Figure 9. Creaky voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Languages 09 00214 g009
Figure 10. Devoicing across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Figure 10. Devoicing across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Languages 09 00214 g010
Figure 11. Means plots for H1–H2 in creaky, modal, and breathy voice intervals in males and females.
Figure 11. Means plots for H1–H2 in creaky, modal, and breathy voice intervals in males and females.
Languages 09 00214 g011
Table 1. Stimuli.
Table 1. Stimuli.
Ending in /a/Ending in /o/
Luna‘moon’Toro‘bull’
Mesa‘table’Libro‘book’
Casa‘house’Dinero‘money’
Ventana‘window’Camino‘road’
Pregunta‘question’Círculo‘circle’
Table 2. Percentage of occurrence and average relative duration by phonation type.
Table 2. Percentage of occurrence and average relative duration by phonation type.
Phonation Typen%Vowel Duration Ratio (%)
MeanSD
Non-Modal48971%51%21%
Creaky21531%66%28%
Breathy20129%52%23%
Devoiced7211%36%13%
Modal19729%56%21%
Total685100%53%22%
Table 3. Distribution of single, double, and triple phonation types.
Table 3. Distribution of single, double, and triple phonation types.
Single n%Double n%Triplen%
Creaky 7519%Modal/Breathy8723%Modal/Creaky/Breathy164%
Breathy277%Creaky/Breathy4211%Modal/Breathy/Devoiced103%
Modal195%Modal/Creaky359%Modal/Creaky/Devoiced20.5%
Devoiced00%Creaky/Devoiced298%Modal/Breathy/Creaky20.5%
Modal/Devoiced195%Creaky/Breathy/Devoiced51%
Breathy/Devoiced72%Creaky/Modal/Creaky20.5%
Creaky/Modal/Breathy41%
Creaky/Modal/Devoiced10.5%
Total12131%Total219 58%Total4211%
Table 4. Descriptive statistics for phonation type by speaker sex.
Table 4. Descriptive statistics for phonation type by speaker sex.
FemalesMalesTotal
n%n%N%
Breathy13529%6628%20129%
Creaky12027%9541%21531%
Devoiced4811%2410%7211%
Modal14833%4921%19729%
Total451100%234100%685100%
Table 5. Descriptive statistics for phonation type by prosodic context.
Table 5. Descriptive statistics for phonation type by prosodic context.
ipIPTotal
n%n%N%
Breathy9127%11032%20129%
Creaky9728%11835%21531%
Devoiced3310%3911%7211%
Modal12235%7522%19729%
Total343100%342100%685100%
Table 6. Distribution of phonation types for females across prosodic contexts.
Table 6. Distribution of phonation types for females across prosodic contexts.
Phonation TypeipIPTotal
n%n%N%
Modal voice44%33%73%
Creaky voice65%3025%3616%
Breathy voice00%11%10%
Modal/Creak1513%33%188%
Modal/Breathy5146%2622%7733%
Modal/Devoiced98%98%188%
Creaky/Breathy65%2219%2812%
Creaky/Devoiced76%54%125%
Modal/Creaky/Devoiced11%11%21%
Modal/Breathy/Devoiced44%65%104%
Modal/Creaky/Breathy54%22%73%
Modal/Breathy/Creaky22%00%21%
Creaky/Breathy/Devoiced00%54%52%
Creaky/Modal/Creaky22%00%21%
Creaky/Modal/Devoicing00%11%10%
Creaky/Modal/Breathy00%43%42%
Total112100%118100%230100%
Table 7. Distribution of phonation types for males across prosodic contexts.
Table 7. Distribution of phonation types for males across prosodic contexts.
Phonation TypeipIPTotal
n%n%N%
Modal voice1013%23%128%
Creaky voice1925%2026%3926%
Breathy voice811%1824%2617%
Modal/Creak1216%57%1711%
Modal/Breathy57%57%107%
Modal/Devoiced11%00%11%
Creaky/Breathy811%68%149%
Creaky/Devoiced1013%68%1611%
Breathy/Devoiced11%68%75%
Modal/Creaky/Breathy11%811%96%
Total75100%76100%151100%
Table 8. Descriptive statistics: breathy voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Table 8. Descriptive statistics: breathy voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
FemalesMalesTotal
n%n%N%
ip6851%2335%9145%
IP6749%4365%11055%
Total135100%66100%201100%
Table 9. Descriptive statistics for creaky voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Table 9. Descriptive statistics for creaky voice across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
FemalesMalesTotal
n%n%N%
ip4739%5053%9745%
IP7361%4547%11855%
Total120100%95100%215100%
Table 10. Descriptive statistics for devoicing across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
Table 10. Descriptive statistics for devoicing across prosodic contexts for both sexes.
FemalesMalesTotal
n%n%N%
ip2144%1250%3346%
IP2756%1250%3954%
Total48100%24100%72100%
Table 11. Descriptive statistics: H1–H2 values for creaky, modal, and breathy voice.
Table 11. Descriptive statistics: H1–H2 values for creaky, modal, and breathy voice.
Females (N = 354)Males (N = 195)Total
n%MeanSDn%MeanSDN%
Creaky10329%0.076.238745%0.075.6719035%
Modal13538%−0.848.064523%4.564.1618033%
Breathy11633%5.89.176332%7.676.9717932%
Total354100% 195100% 549100%
Table 12. Multiple comparisons, Bonferroni post hoc test of phonation and H1–H2 for females.
Table 12. Multiple comparisons, Bonferroni post hoc test of phonation and H1–H2 for females.
Modal VoiceCreaky VoiceBreathy Voice
p-Value Mean Diff.SEp-ValueMean Diff.SEp-ValueMean Diff.SE
Modal voice 1−0.350.93<0.001−6.2−0.90
Creaky voice10.350.93 <0.001−5.840.95
Breathy voice<0.0016.20.90<0.0015.840.95
Table 13. Multiple comparisons, Bonferroni post hoc test of phonation and H1–H2 for males.
Table 13. Multiple comparisons, Bonferroni post hoc test of phonation and H1–H2 for males.
Modal VoiceCreaky VoiceBreathy Voice
p-Value Mean Diff.SEp-ValueMean Diff.SEp-ValueMean Diff.SE
Modal voice <0.001-5.10.930.0023.50.99
Creaky voice<0.0015.10.92 <0.0018.60.85
Breathy voice0.002−3.50.99<0.001−8.60.85
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

González, C.; Cox, S.L.; Isgar, G.R. Phonation Patterns in Spanish Vowels: Spectral and Spectrographic Analysis. Languages 2024, 9, 214. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/languages9060214

AMA Style

González C, Cox SL, Isgar GR. Phonation Patterns in Spanish Vowels: Spectral and Spectrographic Analysis. Languages. 2024; 9(6):214. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/languages9060214

Chicago/Turabian Style

González, Carolina, Susan L. Cox, and Gabrielle R. Isgar. 2024. "Phonation Patterns in Spanish Vowels: Spectral and Spectrographic Analysis" Languages 9, no. 6: 214. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/languages9060214

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop