Next Article in Journal
The Functional Network of the Visual Cortex Is Altered in Migraine
Previous Article in Journal
Chronic Macular Oedema as a Late MIRAgel-Related Complication
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Application of Eye Tracking Technology in Medicine: A Bibliometric Analysis

by
Gianpaolo Zammarchi
* and
Claudio Conversano
Department of Economics and Business Sciences, University of Cagliari, 09123 Cagliari, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 6 October 2021 / Revised: 4 November 2021 / Accepted: 8 November 2021 / Published: 11 November 2021

Abstract

:
Eye tracking provides a quantitative measure of eye movements during different activities. We report the results from a bibliometric analysis to investigate trends in eye tracking research applied to the study of different medical conditions. We conducted a search on the Web of Science Core Collection (WoS) database and analyzed the dataset of 2456 retrieved articles using VOSviewer and the Bibliometrix R package. The most represented area was psychiatry (503, 20.5%) followed by neuroscience (465, 18.9%) and psychology developmental (337, 13.7%). The annual scientific production growth was 11.14% and showed exponential growth with three main peaks in 2011, 2015 and 2017. Extensive collaboration networks were identified between the three countries with the highest scientific production, the USA (35.3%), the UK (9.5%) and Germany (7.3%). Based on term co-occurrence maps and analyses of sources of articles, we identified autism spectrum disorders as the most investigated condition and conducted specific analyses on 638 articles related to this topic which showed an annual scientific production growth of 16.52%. The majority of studies focused on autism used eye tracking to investigate gaze patterns with regards to stimuli related to social interaction. Our analysis highlights the widespread and increasing use of eye tracking in the study of different neurological and psychiatric conditions.

1. Introduction

Eye tracking is a technique used to measure and study the range of eye movements of participants while they are engaged in different activities (e.g., during reading, assessment of a visual stimulus and so on), in a non-invasive way and with a high degree of precision. The assessment of eye movements is made through the eye tracker, a device that sends out a beam of invisible near-infrared light that is reflected in the cornea. After the reflection is collected by the eye tracker’s sensors, it is possible to apply algorithms to calculate where a person is looking. An eye tracker can capture the position of the eyes several times per second. It is, therefore, possible to produce a visual map to measure how and for how long the person looked at different visual stimuli. In addition to being useful for several commercial applications (e.g., to assess the impact of different aspects of packaging and/or to improve their visual presentation [1,2], evaluate the usability of websites [3] and so on), eye tracking has been increasingly applied to the study of different medical conditions, such as neurological and psychiatric disorders, based on the observation that eye movement can provide insights into cognitive processing [4].
Considerable evidence suggests that patients with different psychiatric and neurological conditions show abnormalities in eye movement. For instance, autism spectrum disorders (a group of neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by repetitive behaviors and alterations in social interaction/communication, language and nonverbal communication [5]) are associated with alterations in gaze patterns when exposed to different types of stimuli [6]. Moreover, patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder show distinct patterns of eye movement during smooth pursuit and visual search [7]. While studies on eye movement characteristics in these disorders have been conducted since the early 1900s, the increasing accessibility of eye tracking technology has made it possible to provide precise and quantitative measurements of these impairments. In the last few years, an increasing number of studies have investigated gaze behavior in individuals with different psychiatric disorders besides those already mentioned (for instance, eating disorders [8]) or with neurological disorders. For instance, eye tracking has been applied to the study of stroke [9], brain injury [10] or neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disorder or Parkinson’s disorder [11,12].
The eye tracking technique produces objective data, which are not influenced by the opinions of the subjects carrying out the study or by those who analyze the results. Moreover, the eye tracker can also be used in combination with other neurophysiological or brain imaging techniques, such as electroencephalography (EEG) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), to increase the understanding of the underlying neurobiological processes. Overall, eye tracking represents a non-invasive technique to collect objective and precise data allowing for the study of complex phenotypes such as cognition, emotion and social interaction.
In this article, we provide a comprehensive and up-to-date bibliometric analysis of studies that applied eye tracking technology to different medical fields, aiming to describe trends regarding the most investigated conditions and to identify countries and institutions with the highest scientific production and map collaboration networks. Our analysis fills a gap in the literature by providing novel insights on the current trends as well as emerging and declining themes in the application of eye tracking to the study of different medical conditions.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Literature Search

We conducted a literature search on the Web of Science Core Collection (WoS) online database updated to the 29 August 2021 (the day on which the study records were downloaded from WoS) to identify scientific articles or proceeding papers that used eye tracking to study different disorders. The search strategy was the following: (“eye track*” OR “eye-track*” OR “gaze track*” OR “gaze-track*”) AND (disorder* OR disease*). We searched for studies mentioning these terms in any searchable field (default option in WoS). No language or date restrictions were applied. As suggested in the Bibliometrix documentation, we used WoS instead of Scopus, as the former is preferable in terms of data quality for a bibliometric search due to the standardization of cited reference items, for example. In addition, WoS and not Scopus allowed us to split the collection of records retrieved with the search into multiple downloads.

2.2. Bibliometric Analysis

From each identified article, the following characteristics were extracted and used for the bibliometric analysis: title, abstract, keywords, authors’ affiliations, year of publication, journal title and the number of citations. We used the freely available VOSviewer software to conduct a term co-occurrence analysis based on the network of scientific publications retrieved with the literature search and to plot the network map [13]. Terms were extracted from the titles and abstracts of the retrieved articles using the default options in VOSviewer (binary counting method and minimum of ten occurrences for included terms). In the co-occurrence term map based on network data, circles represented terms, the size of the circle was proportional to the number of articles in which the term was found and the curved lines between circles represented the strength of the relatedness of two terms (i.e., terms with a high number of co-occurrences were linked by a thicker line). Terms strongly related to each other formed clusters of nodes that were identified based on the modularity-based clustering algorithm implemented in VOSviewer [13] and represented using different colors. The VOSviewer software allowed us to highlight the relationship between two or more nodes, creating a distance-based map of terms. For each node, the VOS clustering techniques first compute a normalized association strength, then place nodes on a bi-dimensional plane and lastly assign them to a cluster (i.e., a group of closely related nodes). When a network is represented, it is normal for some nodes to have much more connections than others. If normalization is not applied, the disparity between nodes with different numbers of edges, i.e., nodes with a high number of edges and a node with a small number of edges, would make it difficult to represent the network effectively. VOSviewer applies the normalization criterion proposed by Van Eck and Waltman [14]. Let aij be the weight of the edge between node i and node j, which might be equal to zero in the case of no connection. The normalization association strength allows us to create a network where the weight of the edge between nodes i and j is given by:
s i j = 2 m a i j k i k j
where ki and kj are the sum of the weights of all the edges of node i and node j, and m is the sum of the weights for all edges in the network.
After computing the weights for all nodes, a variant of the Multidimensional Scaling Using Majorization (SMACOF) algorithm [15] is applied to minimize
V ( x 1 , , x n ) = s i j | x i x j | 2
subject to the constraint
2 n ( n 1 ) | x i x j | = 1
where n is the number of the network’s nodes and | x i x j | is the Euclidean distance between nodes i and node j. In this way, we could compute the distances between each node and represent the network in a bi-dimensional space where, for each pair of nodes, their distance is proportional to the strength of their relationship [16]. Finally, each node is assigned to one cluster [17] using a variant of the modularity function introduced by Newman and Girvan [18] and Newman [19]:
V ( c 1 , , c n ) = δ ( c i , c j ) ( s i j γ )
where c i is the cluster to which node I is assigned, δ ( c i , c j ) is an indicator function that equals 1 only when c i = c j , and γ is a parameter that determines the number of clusters (larger values of γ means more clusters).
Next, we used the Bibliometrix package [20] version 3.1 in R version 4.1.1. [21] and the Biblioshiny shiny app [20] to conduct bibliometric analyses on our dataset. We computed the annual growth rate and identified peaks based on the year of publication of the retrieved articles. We identified countries with the highest number of published articles and/or citations (based on the affiliation of corresponding authors as well as of other authors) and plotted the country collaboration network. The country collaboration network was generated using the Louvain clustering algorithm implemented in Bibliometrix, setting the number of nodes to 20 for better clarity of representation. We also identified the most influential institutions based on the number of retrieved articles. A word cloud of the most frequent keywords was plotted using the wordcloud2 package in R [22]. Scripts developed to conduct the described analysis are available at [23]. Finally, we used Bibliometrix to analyze the evolution of themes investigated in the retrieved articles based on a clustering analysis of keywords. To this aim, we divided the timespan into three periods identified based on the computed annual growth rate, and for each period we constructed a thematic map. In this map, each cluster was represented with a bubble. The horizontal axis represented Callon’s centrality (i.e., the importance of the theme in the research field) while the vertical axis represented Callon’s density (a measure of the theme’s development) [24].
Based on the results of the previously described analyses (frequency of keywords related to this disorder, sources of retrieved articles as well as the thematic evolution analysis) we identified autism spectrum disorders as the most investigated theme. Therefore, to conduct an in-depth analysis on this topic, we repeated the analyses on a subgroup of 638 articles focused on autism spectrum disorders.

3. Results

3.1. Trends Regarding Most Investigated Disorders

A total of 2456 documents (2141 journal articles and 215 proceeding papers), published from 1991 up to 29 August 2021 were retrieved. The analysis of research fields conducted using the WOS analytical instrument showed that the most represented area was psychiatry (503, 20.5%) followed by neuroscience (465, 18.9%) and psychology development (337, 13.7%). The annual scientific production growth was 11.14% and showed exponential growth with three main peaks in 2011, 2015 and 2017 (Figure S1). Figure 1 shows a term co-occurrence network in which circles represent terms, the size of the circle is proportional to the number of articles in which the term was retrieved and the thickness of the lines between circles represents the strength of the relatedness of two terms. Four main clusters of terms strongly related to each other were identified by VOSviewer: (1) red, focused on autism spectrum disorders, (2) blue, including studies investigating attentional bias, emotion recognition, major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders; (3) yellow, including studies on schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder, and (4) green, including more generic terms (e.g., tracking, disease) as well as terms related to disorders such as cerebral palsy or amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS).
A word cloud plotted using the most frequent keywords (after the exclusion of words included in the search strategy), is shown in Figure 2. As in the co-occurrence term map, autism spectrum disorders were the most represented of the conditions, followed by schizophrenia, depression, anxiety, Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease.

3.2. Countries and Institutions

Studies were conducted by investigators from 2204 institutions located in 63 countries. The three countries with the highest scientific production in terms of the retrieved articles, based on the affiliation of the corresponding author, were the USA (867, 35.3%), the UK (234, 9.5%) and Germany (178, 7.3%) (Table 1).
A map of international collaboration based on the affiliations of authors is shown in Figure 3. The width of the lines is proportional to the number of studies in which a collaboration between two countries was observed. The USA showed the highest number of collaborations (42 countries) followed by the UK (33 countries) and Germany (20 countries). Collaborations present in the highest number of articles were those between the USA and Canada (47 articles), the UK (47 articles), China (31 articles) and Germany (29 articles), highlighting strong collaborations between the countries with the highest scientific production identified in our search (Table S1). However, the USA showed a modest collaboration percentage (calculated as the ratio between articles with international collaborations and articles with no international collaborations, multiplied by 100), as only 15% of articles attributed to the USA based on the affiliation of the corresponding author included international collaborations (Table 1). Among countries with at least ten published articles, Finland showed the highest collaboration percentage, while Japan showed the lowest (Table 1). For most countries, the international collaboration percentage was approximately 30% (one-third of studies included international collaborations).
A country collaboration network was generated based on the approach proposed by previous articles [20,25]. We identified three main clusters, two of which were centered on the two countries with the highest scientific production (USA and UK). A third smaller cluster included France, Belgium and Switzerland (Figure 4).
The USA also received the highest number of citations (23,434), while the highest average number of citations per article was received by Singapore (38.50), Canada (35.90) and Norway (35.71) (Table S2). Among institutions, the University of Toronto ranked first for the number of articles (88), followed by Vanderbilt University in Nashville (83) and the University of California in Davis (81) (Table S3).
Of the three most relevant sources for the retrieved articles, two were related to autism spectrum disorders: the Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (104 articles), PLOS ONE (65 articles) and Autism Research (51 articles) (see the list of journals in which at least ten articles were published in Table S4).

3.3. Trends in Topics

To identify trends in topics, such as emerging or declining themes, we generated a thematic map, which is a Cartesian representation of the thematic clusters identified by performing a cluster analysis on a co-occurrence network of keywords. In order to study the evolution of themes, we divided the timespan into three periods based on two of the peaks previously identified through the analysis of the scientific growth of eye tracking (Figure S1) and plotted a thematic map for each period (Figure 5). In the thematic map, each bubble represents a cluster of keywords, the horizontal axis represents Callon’s centrality (i.e., the importance of the theme in the research field) while the vertical axis represents Callon’s density (a measure of the theme’s development) [24]. The left upper quadrant (high density and low centrality) shows highly developed and isolated themes (niches); the right upper quadrant (high density and high centrality) shows motor themes; the left lower quadrant (low density and low centrality) shows emerging or declining themes, and the right lower quadrant (low density and high centrality) shows themes transversal to different research areas in the field. The topics that during the time periods moved from the left lower quadrant to the right quadrants, represent topics that are moving towards mainstream theme areas, while topics moving from the right to left quadrants (i.e., topics with decreasing centrality) represent declining themes. In the first period (Figure 5A), we identified four niche themes, two of which represented the medical conditions of aphasia and vertigo. Transversal themes included pursuit ocular movements and the psychiatric disorder schizophrenia. In the second period (Figure 5B), schizophrenia moved to the left lower quadrant, suggesting a loss of centrality for this theme. Niche themes for this period included age-related macular degeneration and intellectual disability. Learning/education was identified as a motor theme and the neurodevelopmental disorder autism was found to represent a basic and transversal theme. Finally, in the third period (Figure 5C), autism was included among transversal and motor themes, while deep learning was identified as a niche theme. This finding suggests that recent studies are increasingly applying machine learning methods to eye tracking data, exploring the possibility of using eye movements as potential diagnostic markers to discriminate between patients and controls or as biomarkers of response to different interventions.

3.4. Case Study: Autism Spectrum Disorders

Based on results from the analyses regarding the co-term occurrence and the most frequent keywords and sources of retrieved articles, we identified autism spectrum disorders as the most investigated condition for which eye tracking was applied. We, therefore, conducted a bibliometric analysis limited to articles focused on this topic. To this aim, among the 2456 articles retrieved with our search, we extracted the 638 articles that mentioned the words “autism” or “autism spectrum disorders” or “ASD” in the title or abstract fields. These documents included 584 articles and 54 proceedings papers published from 2002 to 2021.
The annual scientific production growth was 16.52% and showed exponential growth with three main peaks in 2011, 2014 and 2018 (Figure S2). Studies were conducted by researchers affiliated with 781 institutions located in 37 countries. The three countries with the highest scientific production in terms of retrieved articles, based on the affiliation of the corresponding author, were the USA (234, 36.7%), the UK (88, 13.8%) and Germany (52, 8.2%) (Table 2).
The USA showed the highest number of collaborations (22) followed by the UK (20), France, Italy and Switzerland (10). Collaborations present in the highest number of articles were between the USA and China (17 articles), the UK (15 articles) and Canada (15 articles) (Table S5). Nonetheless, most articles attributed to the USA (based on the affiliation of the corresponding author) did not include authors from other countries. Among countries with at least ten published articles, Switzerland showed the highest collaboration percentage, while Japan showed the lowest as in the original search (Table 2). Most of the countries showed a percentage of international collaboration of approximately 30% (approximately one-third of studies included international collaborations).
The country collaboration network shows four clusters (Figure S3). In addition to two main clusters centered on the USA and UK (as in the original search), we identified two small clusters including France–Switzerland and Sweden–Australia (Figure S3). The USA also received the highest number of citations (7671), while the highest average number of citations per article was received by Singapore (71.50), the Netherlands (34.91) and Denmark (33.33) (Table S6). The most relevant affiliations for the number of articles were Vanderbilt University in Nashville (54), La Trobe University in Melbourne (45) and Duke University in Durham (44) (Table S7).
Table 3 shows that the articles with the highest number of citations are mainly focused on the analysis of face processing and social interactions. The majority of these articles investigated differences in eye movements during the observation of visual stimuli related to social cues or social interactions, such as images or videos depicting faces [26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38] or social scenes/interactions [39,40,41,42,43,44].

4. Discussion

In this article, we conducted a bibliometric analysis of studies investigating eye tracking in different medical conditions. We observed a substantial growth of the use of this technology in the last few years and identified strong collaboration networks among the countries with the highest scientific production (USA, UK and Germany). This substantial growth might be explained by the increased availability of eye tracking systems at different price ranges as well as by open-source data acquisition software. Based on the most frequent keywords, autism spectrum disorders represented the most investigated condition, followed by other psychiatric disorders (schizophrenia and depression) and neurodegenerative disorders (Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease). Different causes might explain the observed relevance of eye tracking technology for research on autism spectrum disorders. First, autism represents a neurodevelopmental disorder and eye tracking is a non-invasive technique that can be applied to studies including infants and children. Second, different studies support the hypothesis that patients with this condition focus their attention on different types of stimuli compared to typically developing individuals, especially in the case of stimuli related to social interactions. Indeed, an analysis of the most cited articles retrieved in our search and related to autism identified social interactions and face processing as the most investigated field. The majority of these articles used eye tracking to investigate differences in gaze patterns during vision exposure to social stimuli, such as faces or social events, either in patients with autism [27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,37,38,40,41,42,43], in their siblings [26,36] or participants at risk of autism [39] compared with typically developing controls. Some of these studies combined eye tracking with additional techniques such as MRI in order to investigate whether the identified differences in gaze patterns were associated with neurobiological abnormalities [26,27,45]. In the most cited article, Dalton and colleagues showed that patients with autism spent less time looking at the eye region when presented with photographs of human faces compared to typically developing controls [27]. The authors showed that the amount of time spent observing the eye region positively correlated with the activation of a specific brain area (fusiform gyrus), measured with MRI. In a subsequent study, the authors also showed similar findings in unaffected siblings of patients with autism spectrum disorders compared with controls [26]. Abnormalities identified using eye tracking might also be associated with the increased severity of illness as shown by Jones and colleagues in a study including 2-year-old children with autism [28]. In this study, children with autism spent less time looking at the eyes and more time looking at the mouths of people pictured in videos compared with typically developing children or children with a developmental delay different from autism. Importantly, children with autism showing a low number of fixations located on eyes also showed higher levels of social disability [28].
Our analysis of trend topics supported the relevance of autism spectrum disorders as a central theme and identified “deep learning” as the niche theme that is mostly reported in recent articles. This finding might be explained by the fact that, in the last few years, a number of studies have started to explore the utility of eye tracking markers to improve the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. While different pipelines to analyze eye tracking data are available, increasing attention is now focused on the development of visual attention models using machine learning methods [46,47,48,49,50]. Since the diagnosis of autism is challenging and no biomarker is available [51], the development of computational models based on early abnormalities such as the differences in gaze processing might be of substantial help to improve and anticipate the diagnosis, thus, making it possible to initiate treatment at an earlier stage, when it is most effective [52]. Eye tracking measurements that might prove to be useful as early biomarkers include dysregulations in pupil dilation [53,54,55], changes in saccadic behavior, differences in gaze patterns during vision exposure to social stimuli [56,57,58] and analysis of scan paths or gaze patterns [59,60,61,62,63,64]. Some studies combined eye tracking data with other measurements such as resting-state EEG data [65,66,67]. Using this approach, Kang and colleagues showed a classification accuracy of up to 85.44% using a support vector machine (SVM) classifier to discriminate children aged from 3 to 6 with autism from controls [65]. Even higher performances were shown by another study conducted by Li and colleagues, using the three-layer Long Short-Term Memory (LSTM) network to discriminate 136 children with autism compared with 136 typically developing children based on gaze patterns during observations of 272 videos [68]. In this study, accuracy improved from 86.4% to 92.6% using LSTM compared to SVM [68]. Overall, results from these studies appear to be promising, although further research will be needed to evaluate their potential utility in the clinical setting and better assess the role of potential confounding factors that might affect gaze patterns (such as age, disease severity and duration, cognitive functioning and so on). Similarly, eye tracking measurements might be useful as prognostic markers [69] or to evaluate the efficacy of different types of interventions [70,71,72,73].
While we conducted a comprehensive analysis with no date and language restrictions, findings from our article must be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, our search might have missed some articles in which the terms included in our search strategy were mentioned in the main text but not in the title, abstract or keywords. In addition, our search might include some articles that mentioned the terms included in our search strategy but in which eye tracking experiments were not conducted (for instance reviews incorrectly classified by WOS as research articles, or articles in which new analytical methods were proposed). Despite these limitations, our analysis of large numbers of documents provides an updated and comprehensive picture of the use of eye tracking to study different medical conditions and highlights relevant trends with regards to investigated disorders and specific applications.
In conclusion, the eye tracking technique is increasingly being used in the study of different medical conditions. In autism spectrum disorders, eye tracking is widely used to evaluate eye movement abnormalities during face processing or looking at other social stimuli. Studies developing machine learning models provided promising evidence to support the utility of eye tracking measurements as biomarkers to discriminate patients with autism spectrum disorders from typically developing controls.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/article/10.3390/vision5040056/s1, Table S1: World collaboration network for articles investigating the use of eye tracking in different medical fields; Table S2: Most cited countries based on number of total citations; Table S3: Institutions with highest numbers of retrieved articles based on authors affiliations; Table S4: List of sources in which at least ten articles retrieved in the search were published; Table S5: World collaboration network for articles investigating the use of eye tracking in autism spectrum disorders; Table S6: Most cited countries based on number of total citations for articles focused on autism spectrum disorders; Table S7: Institutions with highest numbers of retrieved articles focused on autism spectrum disorders based on authors affiliations. Figure S1: Annual scientific production growth of all articles retrieved in our search; Figure S2: Annual scientific production growth of articles that applied eye tracking to the study of autism spectrum disorders; Figure S3: Country collaboration network of articles that applied eye tracking to the study of autism spectrum disorders.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, G.Z.; Data curation, G.Z.; Formal analysis, G.Z.; Supervision, C.C.; Writing—original draft, G.Z.; Writing—review and editing, C.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bix, L.; Sundar, R.P.; Bello, N.M.; Peltier, C.; Weatherspoon, L.J.; Becker, M.W. To See or Not to See: Do Front of Pack Nutrition Labels Affect Attention to Overall Nutrition Information? PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0139732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. McNeill, A.; Gravely, S.; Hitchman, S.C.; Bauld, L.; Hammond, D.; Hartmann-Boyce, J. Tobacco packaging design for reducing tobacco use. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2017, 4, CD011244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Zammarchi, G.; Frigau, L.; Mola, F. Markov chain to analyze web usability of a university website using eye tracking data. Stat. Anal. Data Min. 2021, 14, 331–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Liversedge, S.P.; Findlay, J.M. Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 6–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Persico, A.M.; Ricciardello, A.; Cucinotta, F. The psychopharmacology of autism spectrum disorder and Rett syndrome. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2019, 165, 391–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Howard, P.L.; Zhang, L.; Benson, V. What Can Eye Movements Tell Us about Subtle Cognitive Processing Differences in Autism? Vision 2019, 3, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Morita, K.; Miura, K.; Kasai, K.; Hashimoto, R. Eye movement characteristics in schizophrenia: A recent update with clinical implications. Neuropsychopharmacol. Rep. 2020, 40, 2–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Giel, K.E.; Friederich, H.C.; Teufel, M.; Hautzinger, M.; Enck, P.; Zipfel, S. Attentional processing of food pictures in individuals with anorexia nervosa--an eye-tracking study. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, 661–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Walle, K.M.; Nordvik, J.E.; Becker, F.; Espeseth, T.; Sneve, M.H.; Laeng, B. Unilateral neglect post stroke: Eye movement freqauencies indicate directional hypokinesia while fixation distributions suggest compensational mechanism. Brain Behav. 2019, 9, e01170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Hunfalvay, M.; Murray, N.P.; Roberts, C.M.; Tyagi, A.; Barclay, K.W.; Carrick, F.R. Oculomotor Behavior as a Biomarker for Differentiating Pediatric Patients with Mild Traumatic Brain Injury and Age Matched Controls. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 581819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Archibald, N.K.; Hutton, S.B.; Clarke, M.P.; Mosimann, U.P.; Burn, D.J. Visual exploration in Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s disease dementia. Brain 2013, 136, 739–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Boucart, M.; Bubbico, G.; Szaffarczyk, S.; Pasquier, F. Animal spotting in Alzheimer’s disease: An eye tracking study of object categorization. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2014, 39, 181–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Waltman, L.; van Eck, N.J.; Noyons, E.C.M. A unified approach to mapping and clustering of bibliometric networks. J. Informetr. 2010, 4, 629–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L. How to normalize cooccurrence data? An analysis of some well-known similarity measures. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 2009, 60, 1635–1651. [Google Scholar]
  15. Borg, I.; Groenen, P.J.F. Modern Multidimensional Scaling: Theory and Applications, 2nd ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  16. Van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L.; Dekker, R.; Van den Berg, J. A comparison of two techniques for bibliometric mapping: Multidimensional scaling and VOS. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 2010, 61, 2405–2416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Waltman, L.; Van Eck, N.J. A smart local moving algorithm for large-scale modularity-based community detection. Eur. Phys. J. B. 2013, 86, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Newman, M.E.; Girvan, M. Finding and evaluating community structure in networks. Phys. Rev. E 2004, 69, 026113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Newman, M.E.J. Detecting community structure in networks. Eur. Phys. 2004, 38, 321–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Aria, M.; Cuccurullo, C. Bibliometrix: An R-tool for comprehensive science mapping analysis. J. Informetr. 2017, 11, 959–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  22. Wordcloud2 R Package. Available online: https://github.com/lchiffon/wordcloud2 (accessed on 1 November 2021).
  23. OSF. Available online: https://osf.io/chtjs/?view_only=3ef84f55d5264fd1900cfdfb2c29da8d (accessed on 1 November 2021).
  24. Callon, M.; Courtial, J.P.; Laville, F. Co-word analysis as a tool for describing the network of interactions between basic and technological research: The case of polymer chemsitry. Scientometrics 1991, 22, 155–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Batagelj, V.; Cerinsek, M. On bibliographic networks. Scientometrics 2013, 96, 845–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Dalton, K.M.; Nacewicz, B.M.; Alexander, A.L.; Davidson, R.J. Gaze-fixation, brain activation, and amygdala volume in unaffected siblings of individuals with autism. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 61, 512–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Dalton, K.M.; Nacewicz, B.M.; Johnstone, T.; Schaefer, H.S.; Gernsbacher, M.A.; Goldsmith, H.H.; Alexander, A.L.; Davidson, R.J. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat. Neurosci. 2005, 8, 519–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Jones, W.; Carr, K.; Klin, A. Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008, 65, 946–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Riby, D.M.; Hancock, P.J. Do faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 421–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Speer, L.L.; Cook, A.E.; McMahon, W.M.; Clark, E. Face processing in children with autism: Effects of stimulus contents and type. Autism 2007, 11, 265–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Young, G.S.; Merin, N.; Rogers, S.J.; Ozonoff, S. Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 798–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Nacewicz, B.M.; Dalton, K.M.; Johnstone, T.; Long, M.T.; McAuliff, E.M.; Oakes, T.R.; Alexander, A.L.; Davidson, R.J. Amygdala volume and nonverbal social impairment in adolescent and adult males with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2006, 63, 1417–1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. van der Geest, J.N.; Kemner, C.; Verbaten, M.N.; van Engeland, H. Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: A fixation time study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2002, 43, 669–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Neumann, D.; Spezio, M.L.; Piven, J.; Adolphs, R. Looking you in the mouth: Abnormal gaze in autism resulting from impaired top-down modulation of visual attention. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2006, 1, 194–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Nakano, T.; Tanaka, K.; Endo, Y.; Yamane, Y.; Yamamoto, T.; Nakano, Y.; Ohta, H.; Kato, N.; Kitazawa, S. Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2010, 277, 2935–2943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Merin, N.; Young, G.S.; Ozonoff, S.; Rogers, S.J. Visual Fixation Patterns during Reciprocal Social Interaction Distinguish a Subgroup of 6-Month-Old Infants At-Risk for Autism from Comparison Infants. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2007, 37, 108–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Chawarska, K.; Shic, F. Looking but not seeing: Atypical visual scanning and recognition of faces in 2 and 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 1663–1672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Wang, S.; Jiang, M.; Duchesne, X.M.; Laugeson, E.A.; Kennedy, D.P.; Adolphs, R.; Zhao, Q. Atypical Visual Saliency in Autism Spectrum Disorder Quantified through Model-Based Eye Tracking. Neuron 2015, 88, 604–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Chawarska, K.; Macari, S.; Shic, F. Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 74, 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Riby, D.M.; Hancock, P.J. Viewing it differently: Social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 2855–2860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Fletcher-Watson, S.; Leekam, S.R.; Benson, V.; Frank, M.C.; Findlay, J.M. Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 248–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Sasson, N.J.; Turner-Brown, L.M.; Holtzclaw, T.N.; Lam, K.S.; Bodfish, J.W. Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Res. 2008, 1, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Chawarska, K.; Macari, S.; Shic, F. Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 903–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Pierce, K.; Conant, D.; Hazin, R.; Stoner, R.; Desmond, J. Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2011, 68, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Elison, J.T.; Paterson, S.J.; Wolff, J.J.; Reznick, J.S.; Sasson, N.J.; Gu, H.; Botteron, K.N.; Dager, S.R.; Estes, A.M.; Evans, A.C.; et al. White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. Am. J. Psychiatry 2013, 170, 899–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Chang, Z.; Di Martino, J.M.; Aiello, R.; Baker, J.; Carpenter, K.; Compton, S.; Davis, N.; Eichner, B.; Espinosa, S.; Flowers, J.; et al. Computational Methods to Measure Patterns of Gaze in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder. JAMA Pediatr. 2021, 175, 827–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Oliveira, J.S.; Franco, F.O.; Revers, M.C.; Silva, A.F.; Portolese, J.; Brentani, H.; Machado-Lima, A.; Nunes, F.L.S. Computer-aided autism diagnosis based on visual attention models using eye tracking. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 10131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. He, Q.; Wang, Q.; Wu, Y.; Yi, L.; Wei, K. Automatic classification of children with autism spectrum disorder by using a computerized visual-orienting task. Psych J. 2021, 10, 550–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Carette, R.; Cilia, F.; Dequen, G.; Bosche, J.; Guerin, J.L.; Vandromme, L.; Carette, R.; Cilia, F.; Dequen, G.; Bosche, J.; et al. Automatic Autism Spectrum Disorder Detection Thanks to Eye-Tracking and Neural Network-Based Approach. Internet Things (Iot) Technol. Healthc. 2017, 225, 75–81. [Google Scholar]
  50. Vabalas, A.; Gowen, E.; Poliakoff, E.; Casson, A.J. Applying Machine Learning to Kinematic and Eye Movement Features of a Movement Imitation Task to Predict Autism Diagnosis. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 8346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hayes, J.; Ford, T.; Rafeeque, H.; Russell, G. Clinical practice guidelines for diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults and children in the UK: A narrative review. BMC Psychiatry 2018, 18, 222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Frazier, T.W.; Coury, D.L.; Sohl, K.; Wagner, K.E.; Uhlig, R.; Hicks, S.D.; Middleton, F.A. Evidence-based use of scalable biomarkers to increase diagnostic efficiency and decrease the lifetime costs of autism. Autism Res. 2021, 14, 1271–1283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Anderson, C.J.; Colombo, J. Larger tonic pupil size in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Psychobiol. 2009, 51, 207–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Anderson, C.J.; Colombo, J.; Unruh, K.E. Pupil and salivary indicators of autonomic dysfunction in autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Psychobiol. 2013, 55, 465–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Camero, R.; Martinez, V.; Gallego, C. Gaze Following and Pupil Dilation as Early Diagnostic Markers of Autism in Toddlers. Children (Basel) 2021, 8, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Buffle, P.; Cavadini, T.; Posada, A.; Gentaz, E. A study on visual preference for social stimuli in typical Ecuadorian preschoolers as a contribution to the identification of autism risk factors. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 8461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Jiang, M.; Francis, S.M.; Srishyla, D.; Conelea, C.; Zhao, Q.; Jacob, S.; Jiang, M.; Francis, S.M.; Srishyla, D.; Conelea, C.; et al. Classifying Individuals with ASD through Facial Emotion Recognition and Eye-Tracking. In Proceedings of the 41st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Berlin, Germany, 23–27 July 2019; pp. 6063–6068. [Google Scholar]
  58. Vargas-Cuentas, N.I.; Roman-Gonzalez, A.; Gilman, R.H.; Barrientos, F.; Ting, J.; Hidalgo, D.; Jensen, K.; Zimic, M. Developing an eye-tracking algorithm as a potential tool for early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in children. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0188826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Chatterjee, M.; Manyakov, N.V.; Bangerter, A.; Kaliukhovich, D.A.; Jagannatha, S.; Ness, S.; Pandina, G. Learning Scan Paths of Eye Movement in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 2020, 270, 287–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Cho, K.W.; Lin, F.; Song, C.; Xu, X.W.; Hartley-McAndrew, M.; Doody, K.R.; Xu, W.Y.; Cho, K.W.; Lin, F.; Song, C.; et al. Gaze-Wasserstein: A Quantitative Screening Approach to Autism Spectrum Disorders. In Proceedings of the 2016 IEEE Wireless Health (WH), Bethesda, MD, USA, 25–27 October 2016; pp. 14–21. [Google Scholar]
  61. Falck-Ytter, T.; Nystrom, P.; Gredeback, G.; Gliga, T.; Bolte, S.; The EASE Team. Reduced orienting to audiovisual synchrony in infancy predicts autism diagnosis at 3 years of age. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2018, 59, 872–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Fujioka, T.; Inohara, K.; Okamoto, Y.; Masuya, Y.; Ishitobi, M.; Saito, D.N.; Jung, M.; Arai, S.; Matsumura, Y.; Fujisawa, T.X.; et al. Gazefinder as a clinical supplementary tool for discriminating between autism spectrum disorder and typical development in male adolescents and adults. Mol. Autism 2016, 7, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Fukushima, S.; Takahashi, T.; Tsukamoto, K.; Matsumura, M.; Takigawa, R.; Sakai, Y.; Maniwa, S.; Murphy, L.; Taketani, T. The feasibility of Gazefinder under 12 months of age infants. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 10009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Tsuchiya, K.J.; Hakoshima, S.; Hara, T.; Ninomiya, M.; Saito, M.; Fujioka, T.; Kosaka, H.; Hirano, Y.; Matsuo, M.; Kikuchi, M.; et al. Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorder without Expertise: A Pilot Study of 5- to 17-Year-Old Individuals Using Gazefinder. Front. Neurol. 2020, 11, 603085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kang, J.; Han, X.; Song, J.; Niu, Z.; Li, X. The identification of children with autism spectrum disorder by SVM approach on EEG and eye-tracking data. Comput. Biol. Med. 2020, 120, 103722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Thapaliya, S.; Jayarathna, S.; Jaime, M.; Thapaliya, S.; Jayarathna, S.; Jaime, M. Evaluating the EEG and Eye Movements for Autism Spectrum Disorder. In Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Big Data (BIG DATA), Seattle, WA, USA, 10–13 December 2018; pp. 2328–2336. [Google Scholar]
  67. Zhang, S.; Chen, D.; Tang, Y.; Zhang, L. Children ASD Evaluation through Joint Analysis of EEG and Eye-Tracking Recordings with Graph Convolution Network. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 651349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Li, J.; Zhong, Y.H.; Han, J.X.; Ouyang, G.X.; Li, X.L.; Liu, H.H.; Li, J.; Zhong, Y.; Han, J.; Ouyang, G.; et al. Classifying ASD children with LSTM based on raw videos. Neurocomputing 2020, 390, 226–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Bacon, E.C.; Moore, A.; Lee, Q.; Carter Barnes, C.; Courchesne, E.; Pierce, K. Identifying prognostic markers in autism spectrum disorder using eye tracking. Autism 2020, 24, 658–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Aresti-Bartolome, N.; Garcia-Zapirain, B. Cognitive rehabilitation system for children with autism spectrum disorder using serious games: A pilot study. Biomed. Mater. Eng. 2015, 26 (Suppl. 1), S811–S824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Bradshaw, J.; Shic, F.; Holden, A.N.; Horowitz, E.J.; Barrett, A.C.; German, T.C.; Vernon, T.W. The Use of Eye Tracking as a Biomarker of Treatment Outcome in a Pilot Randomized Clinical Trial for Young Children with Autism. Autism Res. 2019, 12, 779–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Gepner, B.; Charrier, A.; Arciszewski, T.; Tardif, C. Slowness Therapy for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Blind Longitudinal Randomized Controlled Study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2021. online ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Strathearn, L.; Kim, S.; Bastian, D.A.; Jung, J.; Iyengar, U.; Martinez, S.; Goin-Kochel, R.P.; Fonagy, P. Visual systemizing preference in children with autism: A randomized controlled trial of intranasal oxytocin. Dev. Psychopathol. 2018, 30, 511–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Co-occurrence term map of studies investigating eye tracking in different disorders.
Figure 1. Co-occurrence term map of studies investigating eye tracking in different disorders.
Vision 05 00056 g001
Figure 2. Word cloud based on the most frequent keywords of articles retrieved with the search. The size of the words is proportional to their frequency.
Figure 2. Word cloud based on the most frequent keywords of articles retrieved with the search. The size of the words is proportional to their frequency.
Vision 05 00056 g002
Figure 3. Map of international scientific collaborations for studies investigating eye tracking in different disorders. The color intensity is proportional to the number of international collaborations (gray represents the lack of collaborations retrieved in our search).
Figure 3. Map of international scientific collaborations for studies investigating eye tracking in different disorders. The color intensity is proportional to the number of international collaborations (gray represents the lack of collaborations retrieved in our search).
Vision 05 00056 g003
Figure 4. Country collaboration network of articles retrieved in the search. The size of the country’s name is proportional to the number of articles retrieved for that country.
Figure 4. Country collaboration network of articles retrieved in the search. The size of the country’s name is proportional to the number of articles retrieved for that country.
Vision 05 00056 g004
Figure 5. Thematic evolution of eye tracking studies. A thematic map is constructed via the application of a clustering algorithm on the network of keywords of the retrieved articles. Thematic networks are plotted in two dimensions, where axes are the function of centrality (the importance of the theme in the research field, horizontal axis) and density (a measure of the theme’s development, vertical axis) of the thematic network. Three time slices are represented: (A) Period 1: before 2011; (B) Period 2: 2011–2015; and (C) Period 3: after 2015. For each period, the left upper quadrant (high density and low centrality) shows highly developed and isolated themes (niches), the right upper quadrant (high density and high centrality) shows motor themes, the left lower quadrant (low density and low centrality) shows emerging or declining themes and the right lower quadrant (low density and high centrality) shows basic and transversal themes. Each bubble represents a cluster of topics (colors indicate different clusters). The name of the two topics with higher occurrence is shown in the bubble. The size of the bubble is proportional to the cluster word occurrences, while the bubble position is set according to the cluster centrality and density.
Figure 5. Thematic evolution of eye tracking studies. A thematic map is constructed via the application of a clustering algorithm on the network of keywords of the retrieved articles. Thematic networks are plotted in two dimensions, where axes are the function of centrality (the importance of the theme in the research field, horizontal axis) and density (a measure of the theme’s development, vertical axis) of the thematic network. Three time slices are represented: (A) Period 1: before 2011; (B) Period 2: 2011–2015; and (C) Period 3: after 2015. For each period, the left upper quadrant (high density and low centrality) shows highly developed and isolated themes (niches), the right upper quadrant (high density and high centrality) shows motor themes, the left lower quadrant (low density and low centrality) shows emerging or declining themes and the right lower quadrant (low density and high centrality) shows basic and transversal themes. Each bubble represents a cluster of topics (colors indicate different clusters). The name of the two topics with higher occurrence is shown in the bubble. The size of the bubble is proportional to the cluster word occurrences, while the bubble position is set according to the cluster centrality and density.
Vision 05 00056 g005
Table 1. Country-specific scientific production.
Table 1. Country-specific scientific production.
CountryArticlesSingle Country PublicationsMultiple Country Publications% of International
Collaborations
USA86773912815%
UK2341538135%
Germany1781225631%
China132933930%
Canada108842422%
Australia89593034%
France85691619%
Italy81572430%
Netherlands73532027%
Japan625935%
Sweden57431425%
Spain48321633%
Poland4334921%
Korea383538%
Switzerland38142463%
Belgium29151448%
India2923621%
Brazil2315835%
Austria2214836%
Israel22111150%
Denmark1510533%
Finland134969%
Ireland129325%
Portugal116545%
The table reports, for each country, the total number of articles retrieved in the search, the articles with no international collaborations (single country publications), with international collaborations (multiple country publications) and the percentage of articles with international collaborations (international collaboration %). Only countries with at least ten articles retrieved with our search are included.
Table 2. Country-specific scientific production for articles focused on autism spectrum disorders.
Table 2. Country-specific scientific production for articles focused on autism spectrum disorders.
CountryArticlesSingle Country PublicationsMultiple Country Publications% of International
Collaborations
USA2341983615%
UK88583034%
China52361631%
Australia31191239%
France3124723%
Sweden2316730%
Germany1912737%
Japan1816211%
Italy1611531%
Switzerland1521387%
Belgium137646%
India1310323%
Canada127542%
Netherlands119218%
The table reports, for each country, the total number of articles retrieved in the search and related to autism, the articles with no international collaborations (single country publications), with international collaborations (multiple country publications) and the percentage of articles with international collaborations (% of international collaboration). Only countries with at least ten articles retrieved with our search are included in the table.
Table 3. Twenty most cited articles focused on autism spectrum disorders.
Table 3. Twenty most cited articles focused on autism spectrum disorders.
ArticleCitCit/YearMain TopicRef.
Dalton et al., 200595456.12Face processing[27]
Jones et al., 200832423.14Face processing[28]
Chawarska et al., 2013 26529.44Pictures with social scenes[39]
Riby and Hacock, 200823516.79Pictures with social scenes[40]
Pierce et al., 201120919.00Vision of images with geometric Patterns compared to social images[44]
Speer et al., 200720013.33Face processing[30]
Fletcher-Watson et al., 200919615.08Pictures with social scenes[41]
Young et al., 200919515.00Face processing[31]
Nacewicz et al., 200618411.50Discriminate facial expressions[32]
Sasson et al., 200818112.93Social or nonsocial images[42]
Van Der Geest et al., 20021668.30Face processing[33]
Dalton et al., 200716310.87Face processing[26]
Riby et al., 200916212.46Face processing[29]
Neumann et al., 20061569.75Face processing[34]
Elison et al., 201314716.33Oculomotor functioning and visual orienting[45]
Nakano et al., 201014712.25Gaze patterns on social and nonsocial stimuli[35]
Chawarska et al., 201214614.60Pictures with social scenes[43]
Merlin et al., 20071469.73Videos with social scenes[36]
Chawarska and Shic, 200914511.15Face processing[37]
Wang et al., 201514020.00Face processing[38]
The table shows, for the twenty most cited articles, the total number of citations (Cit), the number of citations per year (Cit/Year), the main topic of the article and the corresponding reference.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zammarchi, G.; Conversano, C. Application of Eye Tracking Technology in Medicine: A Bibliometric Analysis. Vision 2021, 5, 56. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/vision5040056

AMA Style

Zammarchi G, Conversano C. Application of Eye Tracking Technology in Medicine: A Bibliometric Analysis. Vision. 2021; 5(4):56. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/vision5040056

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zammarchi, Gianpaolo, and Claudio Conversano. 2021. "Application of Eye Tracking Technology in Medicine: A Bibliometric Analysis" Vision 5, no. 4: 56. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/vision5040056

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop