Next Article in Journal
Control–Value Appraisals and Achievement Emotions: A Moderation Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Unveiling the Dot-Perspective Task: Integrating Implicit-Mentalistic with Sub-Mentalistic Processes
 
 
Please note that, as of 22 March 2024, Psych has been renamed to Psychology International and is now published here.
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Induction of Religious Experiences and Temporal Lobe Activation: Neuronal Source Localization Using EEG Inverse Solutions

1
Laboratory for Cognitive Neurosciences, University of Fribourg, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland
2
Translational Research Center, University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy UPD, University of Bern, 3000 Bern, Switzerland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 31 August 2023 / Revised: 21 October 2023 / Accepted: 1 November 2023 / Published: 13 November 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Cognitive Psychology)

Abstract

:
Knowledge about brain source localizations for religious states of mind is still limited. Previous studies have usually not set a direct emphasis on experience. The present study investigated the phenomenon of religious experience using inverse solution calculations, and it is one of the first to measure the dimension of experience directly. A total of 60 evangelical Christians participated in an experiment where they were asked to engage in worship and try to connect with God. Using a bar slider, the participants continuously rated how strongly they sensed God’s presence at any given moment. A selection of songs helped to induce the desired experience. Measurements were made using EEG with 64 electrodes and inverse solutions were calculated with sLORETA. We appropriated two mutually compatible hypotheses from the literature pertaining to religious experiences: the executive inhibition hypothesis (reformulated as the frontal relaxation hypothesis) and the temporal involvement hypothesis. Our results did not yield any information about the frontal areas; however, they indicated that the right temporal cortex appeared to be involved during the experience.

1. Introduction

1.1. Religious Experience

In the psychological study of religion, various approaches are employed, depending largely on the specific facet of religion under examination (cf. [1,2,3]). While there is no universally accepted definition of religion and its related phenomena [4], there has been a significant push towards pragmatic definitions that make the terms more accessible for research within specific fields [5,6,7,8]. Within the realm of cognitive science, the focus has primarily been on the mechanisms that underpin beliefs in supernatural powers, which are frequently viewed as sacred and inviolable [9].
In our understanding of religious experience, we largely follow a Tavesian approach, which is often used in empirical research and has been devised specifically to this end [10,11]. This is based on foundational work in the field of religious studies by Ann Taves [12,13,14,15]. It is basically a building-block approach to the phenomenon of religious experiences, which states that through a cognitive mechanism referred to as “singularization”, special experiences are mentally set apart from more ordinary ones and connected to theological constructs. At the core of the theory lies the idea that an occurrence is deemed religious by the experiencer. The implication is that there is a subjective valuation of the experience, and we incorporated this in our study by asking the participants to subjectively rate their own experiences and indicate them on a bar slider.
Previous studies have usually taken religious practice (e.g., praying, singing, meditating) as a proxy for the religious experience itself (cf. [16,17,18,19]). Investigations on the construct system of religion, however, showed that there are five dimensions of religiosity—intellect, ideology, private practice, public practice, and experience—showing that religious practice and religious experience are distinct dimensions [20]. As such, the present study sets an emphasis on experience, and we provide one of the first attempts to measure it directly while studying its neural correlates.

1.2. Religious Experience and the Brain

The neural underpinnings of religious cognition and emotion are an emerging field of research [21]. Some evidence suggests religious experiences may have a positive impact on mental health and wellbeing [22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31], so better understanding the psychobiological mechanisms may be useful for practitioners. Additionally, discussions on the state of the literature demonstrate that research on experiential aspects of religiosity needs to be further strengthened (for current in-depth reviews, see [32,33]).
The investigation of religious experiences is no longer restricted to the humanities and the social sciences but has become a topic of interest within the empirical and experimental sciences as well. Andrew Newberg [34] famously coined the term Neurotheology and applied it to the neuroscientific study of religious occurrences. Shortly after, Cunningham [35] issued a publication with the titled question “Are Religious Experiences Really Localized Within the Brain?”. The article highlighted some methodological challenges but also acknowledged the progress that has been made in trying to find the neural correlates of such phenomena.
EEG and brain imaging techniques have since been applied to study a broad range of religious rituals, such as contemplative prayer [36], praying in tongues [19,37], meditation with concentration tasks [38,39,40], reading scriptures [16,17], religious recollections [41,42], and meditations using mantras [43].
A review by Grafman et al. [32] highlighted several key brain regions associated with religious cognitive processes:
  • Cognitive control: the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is related to the downregulation of supernatural interpretations with unusual experiences;
  • Theory of mind: the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), and the precuneus are involved with rationalizing God’s intent and emotions;
  • Semantic processing: the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC), the superior temporal gyrus (STG), and the temporopolar region are associated with retrieving religious beliefs stored in semantic and episodic memory;
  • Reward and evaluation: the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) are connected to evaluating religious beliefs;
  • Conflict detection: the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is involved in detecting conflicts between religious beliefs and task stimuli or demands.
Drawing from neurological research on religious states of mind, two hypotheses have become particularly famous: (i) the temporal involvement hypothesis [42,44,45,46], and (ii) the executive inhibition hypothesis [47,48,49,50,51,52]. The former claims that religious experiences are triggered by an activation of the temporal lobes and the latter holds that an inhibition of the frontal executive regions is key.
The temporal involvement hypothesis is based on the idea that temporal lobe excitations may be at least partly responsible for religious experiences [46]. It was supplemented by studies showing that spiritual states of mind may sometimes be induced through phenomena such as temporal lobe epilepsy [53,54,55,56]. The temporal lobes are believed to be associated with auditory processing, memory, and emotion. There are also some visual processes involved, such as object recognition. Their connection to the limbic system further implies some motivational functions including automatic states of the vegetative nervous system [57,58].
The executive inhibition hypothesis draws from the fact that certain religious prayer forms and associated experiences exhibit a decrease in function of frontal regions, most notably the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [19,40]. It was said that the DLPFC and the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG), which are both believed to play a role in executive control, are activated more strongly in skeptics when compared to believers while watching emotional pictures [50]. Such an executive inhibition was also discussed in the context of cognitive disinhibition, thereby fostering religious creative thought and emotion [49,52,59]. These studies appear to suggest that there may be a frontal relaxation characterized through a downregulation of frontal areas at play during religious states of mind, which prompted us to reformulate the idea into the frontal relaxation hypothesis.
Using EEG data from the scalp, it would be possible to test these two influential hypotheses through source localization analyses with inverse solution methods in combination with spectral analysis (cf. [60,61,62,63]). Source localization through Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA) has already been applied to meditative states [39] and hence was deemed appropriate for the present purposes.

1.3. Research Goal and Hypotheses

To our knowledge, there has never been an EEG study measuring the dimension of religious experience directly with the attempt to determine the possible source localizations of these experiences using inverse solutions. With the present research, we aimed to address this absence in the literature.
Based on the above-mentioned studies, we used the following two hypotheses:
Frontal relaxation hypothesisReligious worship experiences are associated with a partial downregulation of the frontal lobes;
Temporal involvement hypothesisReligious worship experiences are associated with an activation of the temporal lobes;
On an exploratory noteWe also wanted to figure out whether these regions behaved similarly on the left and right hemispheres.
A previous publication on the same population used microstate analyses to disentangle the involved neural networks in these experiences [64]. It was shown that microstate (MS) class 1 (the auditory network) showed a significant association with the average rating values of the religious experience based on its duration, contribution, and mean global field power (GFP) of the microstate. An association was also found with MS 3 (the default mode network) based on the mean GFP, and with MS 5 (the salience network) based on its duration, occurrence, contribution, and mean GFP. Due to the prevalence of the associations with the feature extractions, the latter was the strongest predictor for the religious experience. This provides important information about the involvement of neural networks in the domain of subjectively experienced sensations of the divine. However, it leaves open which frequency bands would be involved when having a closer look at topographic locations—information that would contribute to the idea that frontal and/or temporal regions might be involved in such experiences. As such, we believe that the present follow-up study adds a valuable perspective on top of the discovered microstates to the brain topography in light of the above formulated hypotheses based on the literature.
If the research sample is selected accordingly, religious experiences can be induced via worship practices with the help of music [65]. To that end, it was shown that a mix of self-selected songs and pre-selected songs may help to both instantiate subjective states of mind as well as standardize them for further analysis [66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74]. Our study design therefore included worship for every condition, consisting of self-selected as well as pre-selected songs by the researchers, both from the secular and the religious musical domains (for more on this type of design, see [75]). It was shown that evangelical Christians appear to be particularly adept in the induction process and that experiences under worship with music can be frequently observed [65,76,77,78]. As such, all our participants were evangelicals and had to state that they were able to easily sense the presence of God when engaging in worship through music (cf. [65]). A previous study [65] discussed the neurological correlates of a special state of mind where people believed to be sensing the presence of God (please note: As the present investigation makes no claim about the ontological reality of any deity, all references to God correspond to the emic perspectives of the believers that are participating in this research; this holds for all terms such as “God”, “religion”, and the “divine”, since there is social evidence suggesting the fluidity of such constructs). Overall, there were four reasons why we used an evangelical sample: (i) they have a strong focus on religious experience, (ii) they often use worship as an induction for the experience, (iii) they have a shared theological background for the mental concepts that are associated with it, and (iv) we already had access to this cohort, which made the recruitment process more effective (for more on evangelicals and worship experiences, see [77,78,79,80]).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 60 evangelical participants agreed to take part in the study, although data from three subjects had to be discarded due to recording errors during experiments. The participants’ age ranged from 19 to 40 years (mean: 27 y; SD: 4.2 y); the gender ratio was roughly equal (male: 45%; female 55%); 87% of them were right-handed; and 70% stated that they played an instrument once or more per week. The highest education was spread out in the following fashion: 22% had a master’s; 23% a bachelor’s; 22% a high school diploma; and 33% had finished an apprenticeship. Auditory tests confirmed adequate hearing, written informed consent was provided, and the study was approved by the local ethics committee (approval was provided by the Swiss ethics committee of the Canon of Bern [KEK Bern] under the ID number 2021-00022).
When asked how they usually experienced God during worship, 23% of the participants held that they experienced something emotional (whereas 22% said that they sensed a divine presence and 21% felt close to God), 12% believed that they sensed something physical, and 9% professed to receive a message from God. A total of 11% claimed that they became happy during the experience and three respondents (1.5%) reported that they became melancholic or sad during the experience.

2.2. Assessment and Experimental Design

At the beginning, informed consent was declared, and a questionnaire was completed. It provided an adequate understanding of the participants’ experiential dimensions, faith and prayer lives, as well as demographic variables.
Then, the EEG was applied and before the experiment started a pre-experimental EEG recording was conducted. This was made with open eyes, closed eyes, during blinking, as well as horizontal eye movement, which was later used for the pre-processing of the EEG data.
Each experiment lasted for approximately one hour with six experimental conditions plus two resting state conditions at the very beginning and the end. Every condition’s duration was around 4.5 min, separated by a time-free distraction task where the subjects had to concentrate on a series of flashing letters and answer questions about them. The goal of the distraction task was to clearly separate the mental states of the conditions so that the spillover effects would be minimized and hence the conditions could be viewed as independent observations. Both the letters in the distraction task, as well as the turn of the experimental conditions (except for the resting states), were randomized as to avoid any systematic halo effect. The Feedback Loop Model of Religious Worship Experiences reported that environmental factors, including music, can help govern and induce the religious experience by helping or distracting a person to focus on God [65]. Hence, the different conditions were carefully selected based on how strongly they are thought to help or distract their focus. The six experimental conditions are portrayed in Table 1.
The task instruction was the same for every condition and was read before each condition started anew. It requested the participants to engage in worship and to try to connect with God to sense his presence, regardless of whether there was a religious song, a secular one, or no music played at all. The participants were not aware of the purpose of the individual conditions and were not informed that one condition was deliberately selected to distract them with dissonant melodies. The conditions all started and ended with a beep tone.
Using sound engineering software (Audacity 2.4.2.), all songs were cut at natural breaks to last no longer than 4.5 min. Songs that were shorter were made longer (e.g., a verse or a chorus was duplicated) but only in a way that sounded natural to the song, so that eventually all conditions lasted approximately 4.5 min.
Subjects had to close their eyes during the songs, the empty condition, and the resting states. Hence, after the beep tone the eyes were closed and after the second beep tone, 4.5 min later, the eyes were reopened. The reasons for closing the eyes were twofold: on the one hand, closing one’s eyes reduces the input stimuli to maximize the guidance of the experimental conditions, and on the other hand, recording the EEG with eyes closed made it more comparable to similar research in the literature. To feel comfortable and to navigate the bar slider with closed eyes at ease, every participant had some time to familiarize themself with the instruments, the surroundings, the screen, the speakers, and the bar slider before the experiment started.
During the experiment, the EEG signal was continuously recorded and eventually the data from the distraction tasks were discarded. The religious experience was measured through a right-hand bar slider that participants were asked to move up and down depending on how strongly they sensed God’s presence at any given moment during the experiment (except for in the resting state and during the distraction tasks).

2.3. EEG Recording and Processing

The electrophysiological potential on the scalp was measured using the Brain Products actiCapTM system with 64 active electrodes and recorded with Brain Vision Recorder 2.2TM. The sampling rate occurred at 500 Hz and an elastic cap was applied to place the active electrodes according to the international 10–20 montage system with Ag/AgCl gel. The impedance level was fixed at 20 kOhm and the EEG was amplified and digitized using two Brain Amps. The beep tones providing the timestamps for the onset and offset of the conditions were recorded with a marker channel together with the EEG data.
The EEG raw data were exported to Brain Vision Analyzer 2.2TM for pre-processing, which occurred in two steps (cf. [81,82]): (i) we set out to create clean data, meaning that they were corrected for artifacts created by eye movement and ECG remnants; (ii) and we created segmented data, which were saved as separate files for the different experimental conditions per person. Both steps are further elucidated below.
First, the pre-experimental EEG recordings of each person were inspected to see if there were malfunctioning channels. Deficient channels were topographically interpolated. The channels were then used to create an individual spatial filter for eye movement artifact correction, and in rare cases it also included heartbeat artifact correction. To build these individual spatial filters, the data were filtered using an infinite impulse response (IIR) band-pass filter between 1.5 Hz and 20 Hz, and an independent component analysis (ICA) was applied to the pre-experimental data. The resulting factors were visually inspected and, depending on their explained variance and contribution to the perceived artifacts, the respective factors were excluded. Through an ICA inverse method, a corrected EEG was recalculated. The reconstructed data were visually inspected one more time to make sure that the artifact correction had worked. Then, based on the exclusion of the respective ICA factors, the individual spatial correction filter was created.
Next, the filters from the pre-experimental recordings were applied to the data from the experimental recordings, after deficient channels were topographically interpolated. On each participant, another visual inspection was performed to mark places for exclusion where the muscle and movement artifacts distorted the experimental signals. Re-referencing was performed by recalculating the data to an average reference.
Second, in a further segmentation process, each session was split into pieces of 2.048 s with no overlaps. From these epochs, cross-spectral matrices were computed and averaged within each condition and subject. Based on these averaged cross-spectral matrices sLORETA inverse solutions were computed. Inverse solutions were calculated with sLORETA [62], which has been validated independently [83,84]. The method implements the lead field described by Fuchs et al. [85] and the electrode coordinates described by Jurcak et al. [86]. Through allocating the respective Brodmann areas, four regions of interest (ROIs) were produced, corresponding to the frontal and temporal regions bilaterally.

2.4. Inverse Solutions Analysis

Our hypotheses motivated us to expect a significant association of the frontal and temporal lobes in the statistical models, either on one or both hemispheres, with a negative correlation of the frontal cortex (frontal relaxation hypothesis) and a positive correlation of the temporal cortex (temporal involvement hypothesis).
The inverse solutions generated using sLORETA were exported and analyzed with SPSS 27. Our statistical outline followed a four-step program: (i) First, a mixed model with the overall effects was calculated to see if there were any interaction effects between the experience, the frequency bands, and the regions of interest (ROIs) with the LORETA values (refer to Table 2 to see how the ROIs were generated). If this was significant, we (ii) computed a mixed model for each of the ROIs to see in which ROI there was a significant interaction effect of the frequency bands and the experience with the LORETA values. (iii) Then, for the significant ROIs, there were individual mixed models for each of the frequency bands to determine whether there was a significant effect of the experience of the LORETA values per frequency band. (iv) Last, the directionality was tested for each significant frequency band by estimating fixed effects. The next sections delineate each of these steps.
(i) The first model was a hierarchical multi-level mixed model (all mixed models were operating on a type 3 sum of squares). The model accounted for the condition, which was only used to induce an experiential variance, and it included the frequency band, the ROI, and the within-condition and subject average rating of the religious experience. Based on our hypotheses, the term of primary interest consisted of a band × ROI × rating interaction. The model controlled for handedness and gender and included subject as a random factor. All frequency bands and ROIs were present in this model.
(ii) If, in the first step, the target interaction was significant, the second step consisted of four linear mixed models, one for each of the four ROIs (left frontal, left temporal, right frontal, right temporal). The data were hence split per ROI and the models were run with the same parameters (excluding ROIs). They still included condition, frequency band, experiential rating, and the interaction term band × rating. The focus lied on this interaction effect. The models controlled for handedness and gender and included subject as a random factor.
(iii) Third, only the significant ROIs were selected, since these were considered to be the ones with a significant association of the experience with the inverse solutions calculations (LORETA values, which in all mixed-models were the dependent variable). For the significant ROIs, separate mixed models were created for each frequency band. The models included the rating, handedness, and gender, with the first of those being of primary interest. The significant frequencies in the ROIs were considered to be the relevant bands for the experience in the specific region.
(iv) Eventually, for each of the significant frequency bands, we performed separate regression models as post-hoc tests to the mixed models because we wanted to know the directionality of the associations. This occurred through the SPSS built-in estimates of fixed effects function for linear mixed models. The models included the rating, handedness, and gender, with the first being our primary interest. The dependent variable was the LORETA values. Here, we were interested in the estimates of the fixed effects (beta values) for the religious experience ratings.
As such, we organized our statistical analyses in a funnel, starting with whether there were interesting associations between the experience with the inverse calculations overall, zooming in on individual ROIs, and then on the respective frequency bands, eventually looking at the directionality of the associations that were left.

3. Results

As seen in Section 2, first, we wanted to see if there was a significant association between the LORETA value and the interaction effect of Band × ROI × Experience (which we call the overarching model). Table 3 shows that this was the case. Hence, the analysis was run for each ROI separately. Table 4 shows that the effect was only significant for the right temporal cortex and Table 5 specifies the details of the results. In Table 6, therefore, the right temporal lobe was singled out and the analysis was run on each frequency band. This showed us that only higher frequencies (1.-3. Beta and Gamma) were significant. The parameter estimates for all of these frequency bands were positive. This demonstrated that the religious experience was associated with an increased presence of higher frequencies in the right temporal cortex.
The specific results of the overarching mixed regression model based on the regional sLORETA source density estimates are depicted in Table 3.
The overarching model was split and recalculated for each of the four ROIs, of which only the model for the right temporal lobe was significant. This can be seen in Table 4 (only the interaction effects are displayed since they are relevant for determining whether there is a significant association of the LORETA values with the frequency bands and the experience).
For this ROI, split analyses were performed where a separate model for each frequency band was calculated to discover which bands were associated with the religious experience in the right temporal cortex. The linear mixed models were significant only for upper frequencies, namely for 1st beta, 2nd beta, 3rd beta, and gamma frequencies. Post-hoc parameter estimates for the fixed effects were calculated to determine the directionality of the association between the religious experience and the LORETA values, which indicated the source localization probability. For this, only the directionality (positive or negative values) of the coefficients was of interest. The results are summarized in Table 6.

4. Discussion

4.1. The Present Findings

The current study investigated the neural activation patterns with potential source localizations of subjectively rated religious experiences in worship, operationalized as sensing the presence of the divine. We performed a spectral analysis with inverse solutions and worked with two hypotheses: the frontal relaxation and the temporal involvement hypothesis. Our results did not show any significant evidence for a frontal relaxation; however, they indicated that the right temporal lobe was significantly associated with the religious experience. Further post-hoc analyses showed that only higher frequencies—namely 1st beta, 2nd beta, 3rd beta, and gamma—were significantly correlated with the experience in this region of interest. Even though the parameter estimates were small, they all displayed a positive value, indicating that the frequencies in the right temporal cortex were positively associated with the experience. Higher frequencies are typically perceived as activation patterns and positive associations may be interpreted as an activation in the respective region under the influence of the religious experience. This means that our data showed a lack of evidence concerning the frontal relaxation hypothesis, but it provided evidence for the temporal involvement hypothesis, specifying that the effects may lateralize strongest in the right hemisphere.
These findings add interesting information to the discovery that, in part, the auditory as well as the default mode network, but most poignantly the salience network, appear to be implicated in such experiences [64]. The next section briefly discusses the relevance of the temporal cortex in regard to the present findings concerning religious experiences.

4.2. Temporal Involvement

An early study linking temporal regions to religious experiences was found with Persinger and colleagues [87,88,89,90], as well as with Ramachandran and Blakeslee [91]. Although there have been methodological criticisms thereof, especially in Persinger’s “God helmet” to induce a sensed presence deemed divine [41,45], further studies have solidified the notion that the temporal cortex is involved in such phenomenological states [46,54,56,92]. As such, Britton and Bootzin [92] stated that “Many studies in humans suggest that altered temporal lobe functioning, especially functioning in the right temporal lobe, is involved in mystical and religious experiences.” Our present findings therefore align well with previous studies and may significantly strengthen them, since, unlike in previous research designs, we set an emphasis on measuring religious experience directly and not just via religious practice as a proxy.
According to Grafman et al. [32], there are three key regions that have been found in this area relevant for religious cognition and emotion: the temporopolar region (TPR), the superior temporal gyrus (STG), and the temporoparietal junction (TPJ).
The TPR and the STG are held to be involved in accessing religious beliefs (for a review, see [32]), which appears to be a necessary act in deeming an experience religious (cf. [14]). Among other things, the TPR is implicated in social cognition and is known to play a role in prayer manifested as an interpersonal phenomenon (see [51] for a further review). This indicates that prayer and associated experiences imply the retrieval of specific religious ideas, such as conceptions of God, and reconstruct them in a social context. These patterns are not surprising since believing to be having a direct experience with God must naturally entail the belief that one has a concept of God in mind and that there is some sort of interpersonal interaction between a divine agent and the self.
The same may be true for the STG, which is said to be involved with processing abstract linguistic content [93]. In their fMRI study, Kapogiannis and colleagues [49] discovered that the STG is explicitly activated in tasks testing religious knowledge. This goes hand in hand with an activation of the TPR, which is believed to be equally responsible for retrieving religious concepts in an interpersonal situation.
Kappogiannis and his team [49] reported an activation of the inferior temporal gyrus for the perception of God’s level of involvement and the middle temporal gyrus for God’s perceived emotion. It makes sense to assume that a subjectively believed encounter with God would implicate God’s involvement as well as his perceived emotions towards the experiencer. However, some religious experiences have been reported to recruit the temporal cortex in a broader sense. For example, a global discussion on a case report about an epilepsy patient reported “right-sided frontotemporal sharp [EEG] waves” in relation to seizure-related hyper religiosity while further discussing evidence on religious sensations during temporal lobe epilepsy [54]. An in-depth literature review by McCrae and Elliott [56] further strengthened the notion that spiritual and religious experiences appear to be more frequently associated with temporal lobe epilepsy.
Perhaps one of the most interesting regions in this area, reviewed by Grafman and colleagues [32], is the temporoparietal junction (TPJ). This is because the TPJ has been mentioned frequently in spiritual and religious experiences, out-of-body occurrences, and mysticism. For example, a literature discussion connected mystical experiences on mountains with the TPJ [94]. The same team also provided electrical stimulation to the left TPJ under surgical conditions, which induced the sensation of an illusory shadow person [95]. A structural MRI study investigated the belief in the miracles of Lourdes via Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM), restricted only to grey matter analysis [96]. The results showed that the belief corresponded positively with TPJ volume and negatively with MPFC volume. An fMRI study about true and false belief reasoning showed that both forms of beliefs were, among others, positively associated with the TPJ [97]. Blanke and his lab became known for researching Out-of-Body experiences (OBE) and they have often, but not always, found an association with the TPJ. In one instance during an epilepsy evaluation, focal electrical stimulation of the right angular gyrus elicited the illusory transformation of the person’s arms and legs, as well as whole-body displacements [98]. In a further study, five out of the six patients in the sample who had reported an OBE also had a diagnosed dysfunction in the TPJ [99]. Stringent reviews have highlighted the relevance of the TPJ for OBEs [100] and respective frameworks have been proposed [101]. A lesion analysis showed that sensations of external vision-like apparitions were positively correlated with lesions in the temporoparietal, insular, and frontoparietal regions [102]. In one interesting case report, a 50-year-old woman who had never before or after reported an OBE, had three such experiences when, during her craniotomy, the TPJ was electrically stimulated [103].
The TPJ is said to be associated with auditory signals, which are often described by mystics [104], and with integrating vision, touch, and hearing in a coordinated reference frame [105,106,107,108], as well as with language and understanding (see Wernicke [109,110]). A TPJ damage on the left hemisphere is reported with a feeling or hearing of a presence in one’s proximate space [102,111] and such experiences have been induced experimentally using manipulation of the congruence between felt and observed sensory stimuli [112,113]. In particular, the right TPJ is said to be involved in multisensory integration for religious experiences (for a further review, see [33]).
In short, the temporal lobe appears to be involved in extraordinary sensations, including the weighing and integration of these potentially differing signals by connecting them to religious beliefs about God’s intents and emotions. In certain instances, this seems to be localized stronger in the right hemisphere. Our study appears to coincide well with such previous findings, since our participants’ reports of sensing the presence of God during worship came along with a significant activation of the right temporal cortex. A religious worship experience might therefore be a state of mind where the believer is confronted with extraordinary sensations in such a way as to invoke the attribution of divine concepts to the occurrence.

5. Conclusions, Limitations, and Future Research

The present study investigated religious experiences in terms of sensing the presence of God with a sample of evangelical Christians. It was hypothesized that such an experience would be characterized by a relaxation of the frontal cortex as well as an activation of the temporal cortex. Our results showed that upon a religious experience under the influence of worship, the right temporal cortex appeared to be activated. As such, our study provides further evidence for the temporal involvement hypothesis, whereas no information can be added to the frontal relaxation hypothesis, which we reformulated from the executive inhibition hypothesis.
One major limitation we faced was a byproduct of the complex nature of these special religious states of mind, which can be summarized in four points. First, we studied one specific phenomenon that belongs to a wider class of “religious experience”. There are many more experiences that may be deemed religious but are not necessarily characterized as “sensing the presence of God” [12,14,114]. Second, there are likely different psychological mechanisms that may lead to the various instances of these states of mind (as already highlighted long ago in [115]). Third, we deliberately selected a narrow population of evangelical Christians with shared theological presuppositions concerning such experiences. Other denominations and religions may have different dogmatic concepts and hence the cognitive constructs associated with such experiences might also differ. Fourth, the class of “evangelicals” is itself a heterogenous one and the participants were not all recruited from a single church, which means that there may be the risk of instantiating somewhat different phenomenal states, even though we tried our best to minimize this possibility. It is therefore possible that the neurophysiological mechanisms we have discovered are merely a small fraction of the potential findings concerning religious experiences. Future studies can remedy this in three ways: (i) by focusing on other types of religious experiences, (ii) by widening the theoretical scope and including more psychological avenues for the characterization of such experiences, and (iii) by recruiting believers from other faith traditions and denominations. It should be noted that religious experiences are likely not immune to social and cultural influences, and studying these dependencies would further benefit the field.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.W. and T.K.; methodology, Y.W. and T.K.; software, Y.W. and T.K.; validation, T.K.; formal analysis, Y.W.; investigation, Y.W.; resources, T.K.; data curation, Y.W.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.W.; writing—review and editing, T.K.; visualization, Y.W.; supervision, T.K.; project administration, Y.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The Swiss ethics committee of the Canon of Bern (KEK Bern) has approved the project under the ID number 2021-00022.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

At reasonable request, data availability can be inquired about with the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We were able to recruit 60 participants for a study that touches on a very intimate subject to all of them. They deserve our appreciation since it cannot be taken for granted that so many people would be willing to offer their most precious experiences under sterile lab conditions where they arguably do not feel as comfortable as in their regular environment.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Clarke, P.B. Introduction: Towards a More Organic Unerstanding of Religion within a Global Framework. In The Oxford Handbook of the Sociology of Religion; Clarke, P.B., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 1–30. [Google Scholar]
  2. Oman, D. Defining Religion and Spirituality. In Handbook of the Psychology of Religion and Spirituality; Paloutzian, R.F., Ed.; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 23–47. ISBN 978-1-4625-2053-4. [Google Scholar]
  3. Oman, D. Spiritual Modeling and the Social Learning of Spirituality and Religion. In APA Handbook of Psychology, Religion, and Spirituality: Context, Theory, and Research; Pergament, K.I., Exline, J., Jones, J., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; pp. 187–204. [Google Scholar]
  4. Droogers, A. Defining Religion. In The Oxford Handbook of the Sociology of Religion; Clarke, P.B., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 263–279. [Google Scholar]
  5. Byrne, P. The Definition of Religion: Squaring the Circle. In The Pragmatics of Defining Religion: Contexts, Concepts, and Contests; Platvoet, J., Molendijk, A.L., Eds.; Brill: Boston, MA, USA, 1999; pp. 379–396. [Google Scholar]
  6. Molendijk, A.L. In Defense of Pragmatism. In The Pragmatics of Defining Religion: Contexts, Concepts, and Contests; Platvoet, J., Molendijk, A.L., Eds.; Brill: Boston, MA, USA, 1999; pp. 3–19. [Google Scholar]
  7. Platvoet, J. Contexts, Concepts and Contests: Towards a Pragmatics of Defining “Religion”. In The Pragmatics of Defining Religion: Contexts, Concepts, and Contests; Platvoet, J., Molendijk, A.L., Eds.; Brill: Boston, MA, USA, 1999; pp. 463–516. [Google Scholar]
  8. Saler, B. Conceptualizing Religion: Immanent Anthropologists, Transcendent Natives, and Unbounded Categories; Berghahn: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bulbulia, J.; Sosis, R. Signalling Theory and the Evolution of Religious Cooperation. Religion 2011, 41, 363–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Paloutzian, R.F.; Park, C.L. Handbook of the Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, Second Edition; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA; London, UK, 2013; ISBN 978-1-4625-1101-3. [Google Scholar]
  11. Paloutzian, R.F.; Park, C.L. The Psychology of Religion and Spirituality: How Big the Tent? Psychol. Relig. Spiritual. 2021, 13, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Taves, A. Religious Experience; Encyclopedia of religion; Thompson-Gale: Detroit, MI, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  13. Taves, A. Rereading the Varieties of Religious Experience in Transatlantic Perspective. Zygon(r) 2009, 44, 415–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Taves, A. Religious Experience Reconsidered: A Building-Block Approach to the Study of Religion and Other Special Things; Second Printing, and First Paperback Printing; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2011; ISBN 978-0-691-14088-9. [Google Scholar]
  15. Taves, A. Mystical and Other Alterations in Sense of Self: An Expanded Framework for Studying Nonordinary Experiences. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 15, 669–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Azari, N.P.; Nickel, J.; Wunderlich, G.; Niedeggen, M.; Hefter, H.; Tellmann, L.; Herzog, H.; Stoerig, P.; Birnbacher, D.; Seitz, R.J. Neural Correlates of Religious Experience. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001, 13, 1649–1652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Azari, N.P.; Missimer, J.; Seitz, R.J. Religious Experience and Emotion: Evidence for Distinctive Cognitive Neural Patterns. Int. J. Psychol. Relig. 2005, 15, 263–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Demmrich, S. Music as a Trigger of Religious Experience: What Role Does Culture Play? Psychol. Music 2018, 48, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Newberg, A.; Wintering, N.A.; Morgan, D.; Waldman, M.R. The Measurement of Regional Cerebral Blood Flow during Glossolalia: A Preliminary SPECT Study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2006, 148, 67–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Huber, S.; Huber, O.W. The Centrality of Religiosity Scale (CRS). Religions 2012, 3, 710–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rim, J.I.; Ojeda, J.C.; Svob, C.; Kayser, J.; Drews, E.; Kim, Y.; Tenke, C.E.; Skipper, J.; Weissman, M.M. Current Understanding of Religion, Spirituality, and Their Neurobiological Correlates. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 2019, 27, 303–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fabricatore, A.N.; Handal, P.J.; Fenzel, L.M. Personal Spirituality as a Moderator of the Relationship between Stressors and Subjective Well-Being. J. Psychol. Theol. 2000, 28, 221–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Fry, P.S. Religious Involvement, Spirituality and Personal Meaning for Life: Existential Predictors of Psychological Wellbeing in Community-Residing and Institutional Care Elders. Aging Ment. Health 2000, 4, 375–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Koenig, G.H.; Larson, D.B. Religion and Mental Health: Evidence for an Association. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2001, 13, 67–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kok, B.E.; Coffey, K.A.; Cohn, M.A.; Catalino, L.I.; Vacharkulksemsuk, T.; Algoe, S.B.; Brantley, M.; Fredrickson, B.L. How Positive Emotions Build Physical Health: Perceived Positive Social Connections Account for the Upward Spiral Between Positive Emotions and Vagal Tone. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1123–1132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Krause, N. Religion and Health: Making Sense of a Disheveled Literature. J. Relig. Health 2011, 50, 20–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Krause, N. Religious Doubt, Helping Others, and Psychological Well-Being. J. Relig. Health 2015, 54, 745–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Lambert, N.M.; Fincham, F.D.; Braithwaite, S.R.; Graham, S.M.; Beach, S.R.H. Can Prayer Increase Gratitude? Psychol. Relig. Spiritual. 2009, 1, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Mueller, P.S.; Plevak, D.J.; Rummans, T.A. Religious Involvement, Spirituality, and Medicine: Implications for Clinical Practice. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2001, 76, 1225–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Park, C.L. Religion as a Meaning-Making Framework in Coping with Life Stress. J. Soc. Issues 2005, 61, 707–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Rizvi, M.A.K.; Hossain, M.Z. Relationship Between Religious Belief and Happiness: A Systematic Literature Review. J. Relig. Health 2017, 56, 1561–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Grafman, J.; Cristofori, I.; Zhong, W.; Bulbulia, J. The Neural Basis of Religious Cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 29, 126–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. van Elk, M.; Aleman, A. Brain Mechanisms in Religion and Spirituality: An Integrative Predictive Processing Framework. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 73, 359–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Newberg, A. Principles of Neurotheology; Ashgate science and religion series; Ashgate Publication: Farnham, UK, 2010; ISBN 978-1-4094-0810-9. [Google Scholar]
  35. Cunningham, P.F. Are Religious Experiences Really Localized Within the Brain? The Promise, Challenges, and Prospects of Neurotheology. J. Mind Behav. 2011, 32, 223–249. [Google Scholar]
  36. Newberg, A.; Pourdehnad, M.; Alavi, A.; d’Aquili, E.G. Cerebral Blood Flow during Meditative Prayer: Preliminary Findings and Methodological Issues. Percept. Mot. Skills 2003, 97, 625–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Walter, Y.; Dieguez, S.; Mouthon, M.; Spierer, L. Brain Structural Evidence for a Frontal Pole Specialization in Glossolalia. IBRO Rep. 2020, 9, 32–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Austin, J.H. Zen and the Brain: Toward an Understanding of Meditation and Consciousness; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1998; ISBN 978-0-262-01164-8. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lehmann, D.; Faber, P.L.; Achermann, P.; Jeanmonod, D.; Gianotti, L.R.R.; Pizzagalli, D. Brain Sources of EEG Gamma Frequency during Volitionally Meditation-Induced, Altered States of Consciousness, and Experience of the Self. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2001, 108, 111–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Newberg, A.; D’Aquili, E.G.; Rause, V. Why God Won’t Go Away: Brain Science and the Biology of Belief; 1. trade edition; Ballantine Books: New York, NY, USA, 2002; ISBN 978-0-345-44034-1. [Google Scholar]
  41. Beauregard, M.; O’Leary, D. The Spiritual Brain: A Neuroscientist’s Case for the Existence of the Soul; HarperOne; 1. Auflage, Nachdruck 2008; HarperCollins: New York, NY, USA, 2008; ISBN 978-0-06-162598-5. [Google Scholar]
  42. Beauregard, M.; Paquette, V. EEG Activity in Carmelite Nuns during a Mystical Experience. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 444, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Stigsby, B.; Rodenberg, J.C.; Moth, H.B. Electroencephalographic Findings during Mantra Meditation (Transcendental Meditation). A Controlled, Quantitative Study of Experienced Meditators. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1981, 51, 434–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Beauregard, M. Neuroscience and Spirituality—Findings and Consequences. In Neuroscience, Consciousness and Spirituality; Studies in Neuroscience, Consciousness and Spirituality; Walach, H., Schmidt, S., Jonas, W.B., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2011; Volume 1, pp. 57–73. ISBN 978-94-007-2078-7. [Google Scholar]
  45. Granqvist, P.; Fredrikson, M.; Unge, P.; Hagenfeldt, A.; Valind, S.; Larhammar, D.; Larsson, M. Sensed Presence and Mystical Experiences Are Predicted by Suggestibility, Not by the Application of Transcranial Weak Complex Magnetic Fields. Neurosci. Lett. 2005, 379, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Tinoca, C.A.; Ortiz, J.L. Magnetic Stimulation of the Temporal Cortex: A Partial “God Helmet” Replication Study. J. Conscious. Explor. Res. 2014, 5, 234–257. [Google Scholar]
  47. Andersen, M.; Schjoedt, U.; Nielbo, K.; Sørensen, J. Mystical Experience in the Lab. Method Theory Study Relig. 2014, 26, 217–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Deeley, P.Q.; Oakley, D.A.; Walsh, E.; Bell, V.; Mehta, M.A.; Halligan, P.W. Modelling Psychiatric and Cultural Possession Phenomena with Suggestion and fMRI. Cortex 2014, 53, 107–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kapogiannis, D.; Barbey, A.K.; Su, M.; Zamboni, G.; Krueger, F.; Grafman, J. Cognitive and Neural Foundations of Religious Belief. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 4876–4881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Lindeman, M.; Svedholm, A.M.; Riekki, T.; Raij, T.; Hari, R. Is It Just a Brick Wall or a Sign from the Universe? An fMRI Study of Supernatural Believers and Skeptics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013, 8, 943–949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Schjoedt, U. The Religious Brain: A General Introduction to the Experimental Neuroscience of Religion. Method Theory Study Relig. 2009, 21, 310–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Schjoedt, U.; Sørensen, J.; Nielbo, K.L.; Xygalatas, D.; Mitkidis, P.; Bulbulia, J. Cognitive Resource Depletion in Religious Interactions. Relig. Brain Behav. 2013, 3, 39–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Devinsky, O.; Lai, G. Spirituality and Religion in Epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2008, 12, 636–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Garcia-Santibanez, R.; Sarva, H. Isolated Hyperreligiosity in a Patient with Temporal Lobe Epilepsy. Case Rep. Neurol. Med. 2015, 2015, 235856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Johnstone, B.; Holliday, G.; Cohen, D. Heightened Religiosity and Epilepsy: Evidence for Religious-Specific Neuropsychological Processes. Ment. Health Relig. Cult. 2016, 19, 704–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. McCrae, N.; Elliott, S. Spiritual Experiences in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy: A Literature Review. Br. J. Neurosci. Nurs. 2012, 8, 346–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Gloor, P. The Temporal Lobe and Limbic System; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; ISBN 978-0-19-509272-1. [Google Scholar]
  58. Joseph, R. Temporal Lobes: Occipital Lobes, Memory, Language, Vision, Emotion, Epilepsy, Psychosis; University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  59. Deeley, P.Q. Social, Cognitive, and Neural Constraints on Subjectivity and Agency: Implications for Dissociative Identity Disorder. Philos. Psychiatry Psychol. 2003, 10, 161–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Hämäläinen, M.S.; Ilmoniemi, R.J. Interpreting Magnetic Fields of the Brain: Minimum Norm Estimates. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 1994, 32, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Pascual-Marqui, R.D. Review of Methods for Solving the EEG Inverse Problem. Int. J. Bioelectromagn. 1999, 1, 75–86. [Google Scholar]
  62. Pascual-Marqui, R.D. Standardized Low-Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography (sLORETA): Technical Details. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 2002, 24 (Suppl. SD), 5–12. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  63. Pascual-Marqui, R.D.; Lehmann, D.; Koukkou, M.; Kochi, K.; Anderer, P.; Saletu, B.; Tanaka, H.; Hirata, K.; John, E.R.; Prichep, L.; et al. Assessing Interactions in the Brain with Exact Low-Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography. Philos. Transact. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2011, 369, 3768–3784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Walter, Y.; Koenig, T. Neural Network Involvement for Religious Experiences in Worship Measured by EEG Microstate Analysis. Soc. Neurosci. 2022, 17, 258–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Walter, Y. Towards a Qualitative Model of Religious Worship Experiences: Perceived Encounters with the Divine in the Ritual Context of Musical Devotion Practices. Am. J. Qual. Res. 2021, 5, 94–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Cheung, V.K.M.; Meyer, L.; Friederici, A.D.; Koelsch, S. The Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus Processes Nested Non-Local Dependencies in Music. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 3822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Cheung, V.K.M.; Harrison, P.M.C.; Meyer, L.; Pearce, M.T.; Haynes, J.-D.; Koelsch, S. Uncertainty and Surprise Jointly Predict Musical Pleasure and Amygdala, Hippocampus, and Auditory Cortex Activity. Curr. Biol. 2019, 29, 4084–4092.e4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Koelsch, S. Investigating Emotion with Music: Neuroscientific Approaches. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2005, 1060, 412–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Koelsch, S. Investigating the Neural Encoding of Emotion with Music. Neuron 2018, 98, 1075–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Koelsch, S.; Bashevkin, T.; Kristensen, J.; Tvedt, J.; Jentschke, S. Heroic Music Stimulates Empowering Thoughts during Mind-Wandering. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 10317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Koelsch, S.; Skouras, S.; Lohmann, G. The Auditory Cortex Hosts Network Nodes Influential for Emotion Processing: An fMRI Study on Music-Evoked Fear and Joy. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0190057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Koelsch, S.; Vuust, P.; Friston, K. Predictive Processes and the Peculiar Case of Music. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 63–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Koelsch, S.; Siebel, W.A. Towards a Neural Basis of Music Perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 578–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Martin, S.; Mikutta, C.; Leonard, M.K.; Hungate, D.; Koelsch, S.; Shamma, S.; Chang, E.F.; Millán, J.R.; Knight, R.T.; Pasley, B.N. Neural Encoding of Auditory Features during Music Perception and Imagery. Cereb. Cortex 2018, 28, 4222–4233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Walter, Y.; Altorfer, A. The Psychological Role of Music and Attentional Control for Religious Experiences in Worship. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2022, 75, 2272–2286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Bielo, J.S. Emerging Evangelicals: Faith, Modernity, and the Desire for Authenticity; New York University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; ISBN 978-0-8147-8955-1. [Google Scholar]
  77. Ingalls, M. Singing the Congregation: How Contemporary Worship Music Forms Evangelical Community; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; ISBN 978-0-19-049966-2. [Google Scholar]
  78. Luhrmann, T.M. When God Talks Back: Understanding the American Evangelical Relationship with God, 1st ed.; Alfred A. Knopf: New York, NY, USA, 2012; ISBN 978-0-307-26479-4. [Google Scholar]
  79. Boyce-Tillman, J. Spirituality in the Musical Experience. In International Handbook of Research in Arts Education; Springer International Handbook of Research in Arts Education; Bresler, L., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 1405–1424. ISBN 978-1-4020-3052-9. [Google Scholar]
  80. Cassaniti, J.L.; Luhrmann, T.M. The Cultural Kindling of Spiritual Experiences. Curr. Anthropol. 2014, 55, S333–S343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Mikutta, C.; Altorfer, A.; Strik, W.; Koenig, T. Emotions, Arousal, and Frontal Alpha Rhythm Asymmetry During Beethoven’s 5th Symphony. Brain Topogr. 2012, 25, 423–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Mikutta, C.A.; Maissen, G.; Altorfer, A.; Strik, W.; Koenig, T. Professional Musicians Listen Differently to Music. Neuroscience 2014, 268, 102–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Greenblatt, R.E.; Ossadtchi, A.; Pflieger, M.E. Local Linear Estimators for the Bioelectromagnetic Inverse Problem. IEEE Trans. Signal Process. 2005, 53, 3403–3412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Sekihara, K.; Sahani, M.; Nagarajan, S.S. Localization Bias and Spatial Resolution of Adaptive and Non-Adaptive Spatial Filters for MEG Source Reconstruction. NeuroImage 2005, 25, 1056–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Fuchs, M.; Kastner, J.; Wagner, M.; Hawes, S.; Ebersole, J.S. A Standardized Boundary Element Method Volume Conductor Model. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2002, 113, 702–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Jurcak, V.; Tsuzuki, D.; Dan, I. 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 Systems Revisited: Their Validity as Relative Head-Surface-Based Positioning Systems. NeuroImage 2007, 34, 1600–1611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Persinger, M.A. Religious and Mystical Experiences as Artifacts of Temporal Lobe Function: A General Hypothesis. Percept. Mot. Skills 1983, 57, 1255–1262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Persinger, M.A. Neuropsychological Bases of God Beliefs, 1st ed.; Praeger: New York, NY, USA, 1987; ISBN 978-0-275-92648-9. [Google Scholar]
  89. Persinger, M.A.; Healey, F. Experimental Facilitation of the Sensed Presence: Possible Intercalation between the Hemispheres Induced by Complex Magnetic Fields. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2002, 190, 533–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Cook, C.M.; Persinger, M.A. Experimental Induction of the “Sensed Presence” in Normal Subjects and an Exceptional Subject. Percept. Mot. Skills 1997, 85, 683–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Ramachandran, V.S.; Blakeslee, S. Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind; 1st Quill edition published 1999; William Morrow, An Imprint of Harper Collins Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 1999; ISBN 978-0-688-17217-6. [Google Scholar]
  92. Britton, W.B.; Bootzin, R.R. Near-Death Experiences and the Temporal Lobe. Psychol. Sci. 2004, 15, 254–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Just, M.A.; Newman, S.D.; Keller, T.A.; McEleney, A.; Carpenter, P.A. Imagery in Sentence Comprehension: An fMRI Study. NeuroImage 2004, 21, 112–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Arzy, S.; Idel, M.; Landis, T.; Blanke, O. Why Revelations Have Occurred on Mountains?: Linking Mystical Experiences and Cognitive Neuroscience. Med. Hypotheses 2005, 65, 841–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Arzy, S.; Seeck, M.; Ortigue, S.; Spinelli, L.; Blanke, O. Induction of an Illusory Shadow Person. Nature 2006, 443, 287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Schienle, A.; Höfler, C.; Wabnegger, A. Belief in the Miracles of Lourdes: A Voxel-based Morphometry Study. Brain Behav. 2019, 10, e01481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Sommer, M.; Döhnel, K.; Sodian, B.; Meinhardt, J.; Thoermer, C.; Hajak, G. Neural Correlates of True and False Belief Reasoning. NeuroImage 2007, 35, 1378–1384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Blanke, O.; Ortigue, S.; Landis, T.; Seeck, M. Stimulating Illusory Own-Body Perceptions. Nature 2002, 419, 269–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Blanke, O.; Landis, T.; Spinelli, L.; Seeck, M. Out-of-Body Experience and Autoscopy of Neurological Origin. Brain J. Neurol. 2004, 127, 243–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Blanke, O. Multisensory Brain Mechanisms of Bodily Self-Consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 13, 556–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Blanke, O.; Slater, M.; Serino, A. Behavioral, Neural, and Computational Principles of Bodily Self-Consciousness. Neuron 2015, 88, 145–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Blanke, O.; Pozeg, P.; Hara, M.; Heydrich, L.; Serino, A.; Yamamoto, A.; Higuchi, T.; Salomon, R.; Seeck, M.; Landis, T.; et al. Neurological and Robot-Controlled Induction of an Apparition. Curr. Biol. 2014, 24, 2681–2686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Bos, E.M.; Spoor, J.K.H.; Smits, M.; Schouten, J.W.; Vincent, A.J.P.E. Out-of-Body Experience During Awake Craniotomy. World Neurosurg. 2016, 92, 586.e9–586.e13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Firth, P.G.; Bolay, H. Transient High Altitude Neurological Dysfunction: An Origin in the Temporoparietal Cortex. High Alt. Med. Biol. 2004, 5, 71–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Bremmer, F.; Schlack, A.; Duhamel, J.R.; Graf, W.; Fink, G.R. Space Coding in Primate Posterior Parietal Cortex. NeuroImage 2001, 14, S46–S51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Duhamel, J.-R.; Colby, C.L.; Goldberg, M.E. Ventral Intraparietal Area of the Macaque: Congruent Visual and Somatic Response Properties. J. Neurophysiol. 1998, 79, 126–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Guldin, W.O.; Grüsser, O.J. Is There a Vestibular Cortex? Trends Neurosci. 1998, 21, 254–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. La’davas, E. Functional and Dynamic Properties of Visual Peripersonal Space. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002, 6, 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Cohen, L.; Dehaene, S. Specialization within the Ventral Stream: The Case for the Visual Word Form Area. NeuroImage 2004, 22, 466–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Hutsler, J.; Galuske, R.A.W. Hemispheric Asymmetries in Cerebral Cortical Networks. Trends Neurosci. 2003, 26, 429–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Blanke, O.; Ortigue, S.; Coeytaux, A.; Martory, M.-D.; Landis, T. Hearing of a Presence. Neurocase 2003, 9, 329–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Ehrsson, H.H. The Experimental Induction of Out-of-Body Experiences. Science 2007, 317, 1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Lenggenhager, B.; Halje, P.; Blanke, O. Alpha Band Oscillations Correlate with Illusory Self-Location Induced by Virtual Reality. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011, 33, 1935–1943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Taves, A.; Wolf, M.G.; Ihm, E.D.; Barlev, M.; Kinsella, M.; Vyas, M. What Counts as Religious Experience? The Inventory of Nonordinary Experiences as a Tool for Analysis across Cultures. PsyArXiv. 2019. Available online: https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/ux28d/ (accessed on 28 December 2019).
  115. James, W. The Varieties of Religious Experience; Centenary Edition; Taylor & Francis: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1902. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Experimental conditions used to induce and guide the religious experience.
Table 1. Experimental conditions used to induce and guide the religious experience.
NameAcronymDescription
Religious subjectiveRsParticipants brought a religious worship song they liked, which had a personal track-record of helping them to sense God’s presence in worship. This song was different for all individuals.
Religious givenRgBased on interviews, the researchers selected a religious worship song that appeared to work well for the induction of the experience for the denominations of the present sample. This song was the same for all individuals. (For the Rg condition, the song Reckless Love by Cory Asbury (2017, Bethel Music) was selected.)
Secular subjectiveSsParticipants brought a secular song they liked, which was similar in style and feel to the Rs song they selected. This song was different for all individuals.
Secular givenSgBased on interviews, the researchers selected a secular song that appeared to work well for the induction of the experience in the denominations of the present sample. This song was the same for all individuals and it was selected to evoke similar feelings to the Rg condition. (For the Sg condition, the song Lose You To Love Me by Selena Gomez (2019, Interscope Records) was selected.)
Empty (blank)BThis was a 4.5 min session where no music was played so that the participants had the opportunity to engage in worship and the experience with no musical guidance or distraction.
Twelve-tone songS12Since the religious experience was our phenomenological variable of interest, we wanted to increase the variability by introducing a dissonant song that made it hard for participants to focus on God, therefore dampening the experience. The S12 song was a disharmonic twelve-tone piece selected to make it difficult for the people to focus. This one was the same for all individuals. (For the S12 condition, the song Pierrot Lunaire by Arnold Schönberg (1874–1951, Op.21: No. 1–4, Mondestrunken, Columbine, Der Dandy, eine blasse Wäscherin) was selected.)
Table 2. Allocation of Brodmann areas to regions of interest (ROIs) for the source localization analysis.
Table 2. Allocation of Brodmann areas to regions of interest (ROIs) for the source localization analysis.
Region of Interest (ROI)Brodmann Areas
Left frontal cortexLeft: 4, 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 24, 25, 32, 33, 44, 45, 46, 47
Left temporal cortexLeft: 20, 21, 22, 27, 28, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 41, 42, 43, 52
Right frontal cortexRight: 4, 6, 8, 9, 10, 11,24, 25, 32, 33, 44, 45, 46, 47
Right temporal cortexRight: 20, 21, 22, 27, 28, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 41, 42, 43, 52
Table 3. Depiction of all the associations from the overarching inverse solution model calculated through hierarchical multi-level linear mixed models. The major focus lies on the interaction term (Band × ROI × Experience). Condition = experimental condition; Band = frequency band; ROI = region of interest; Experience = averaged subjective rating of the religious experience per condition; Handedness = left- or right-handed; Gender = male or female; Df = degrees of freedom.
Table 3. Depiction of all the associations from the overarching inverse solution model calculated through hierarchical multi-level linear mixed models. The major focus lies on the interaction term (Band × ROI × Experience). Condition = experimental condition; Band = frequency band; ROI = region of interest; Experience = averaged subjective rating of the religious experience per condition; Handedness = left- or right-handed; Gender = male or female; Df = degrees of freedom.
VariableDfF-ValueSignificance
Condition6, 127171.10.375
Band7, 127172124.5<0.001
ROI3, 127174217.1<0.001
Experience1, 1271711.30.001
Band × ROI × Experience31, 127176.9<0.001
Handedness1, 1271739.8<0.001
Gender1, 12717182.6<0.001
Dependent variable: LORETA values.
Table 4. Depiction of all the associations from the inverse solution model between the LORETA values and the interaction effect between the frequency and the experience. Only the interaction effects are displayed. Condition = experimental condition; Band = frequency band; ROI = region of interest; Experience = averaged subjective rating of the religious experience per condition; Handedness = left- or right-handed; Gender = male or female; Df = degrees of freedom.
Table 4. Depiction of all the associations from the inverse solution model between the LORETA values and the interaction effect between the frequency and the experience. Only the interaction effects are displayed. Condition = experimental condition; Band = frequency band; ROI = region of interest; Experience = averaged subjective rating of the religious experience per condition; Handedness = left- or right-handed; Gender = male or female; Df = degrees of freedom.
InteractionROIDfF-ValueSignificance
Band × ExperienceLeft frontal7, 31681.010.422
Left temporal7, 31681.670.113
Right frontal7, 31681.500.163
Right temporal7, 31684.52<0.001
Dependent variable: LORETA values.
Table 5. Depiction of all the associations from the inverse solution model of the right temporal lobe. The major focus lies on the interaction term (Band × Experience). Condition = experimental condition; Band = frequency band; Experience = averaged subjective rating of the religious experience per condition; Handedness = left- or right-handed; Gender = male or female; Df = degrees of freedom.
Table 5. Depiction of all the associations from the inverse solution model of the right temporal lobe. The major focus lies on the interaction term (Band × Experience). Condition = experimental condition; Band = frequency band; Experience = averaged subjective rating of the religious experience per condition; Handedness = left- or right-handed; Gender = male or female; Df = degrees of freedom.
VariableDfF-ValueSignificance
Condition6, 31680.10.437
Band7, 3168548.9<0.001
Rating1, 316815.8<0.001
Band × Experience7, 31684.5<0.001
Handedness1, 31684.40.037
Gender1, 3168305.6<0.001
Dependent variable: LORETA values.
Table 6. Summary of the four significant frequency band results including their parameter estimates. Only the frequency bands for significant religious experiences are depicted. The parameter estimates refer to the non-standardized regression coefficients of the religious experience. The F-value refers to the linear mixed model and the T-value to the associated regression coefficient.
Table 6. Summary of the four significant frequency band results including their parameter estimates. Only the frequency bands for significant religious experiences are depicted. The parameter estimates refer to the non-standardized regression coefficients of the religious experience. The F-value refers to the linear mixed model and the T-value to the associated regression coefficient.
Frequency Band DfF-ValueT-ValueSignificanceParameter Estimates
1st Beta1, 3954.72.20.0310.026
2nd Beta1, 39511.93.50.0010.052
3rd Beta1, 39511.33.40.0010.069
Gamma1, 3955.12.30.0250.018
Dependent variable: LORETA values.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Walter, Y.; Koenig, T. The Induction of Religious Experiences and Temporal Lobe Activation: Neuronal Source Localization Using EEG Inverse Solutions. Psych 2023, 5, 1191-1206. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/psych5040079

AMA Style

Walter Y, Koenig T. The Induction of Religious Experiences and Temporal Lobe Activation: Neuronal Source Localization Using EEG Inverse Solutions. Psych. 2023; 5(4):1191-1206. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/psych5040079

Chicago/Turabian Style

Walter, Yoshija, and Thomas Koenig. 2023. "The Induction of Religious Experiences and Temporal Lobe Activation: Neuronal Source Localization Using EEG Inverse Solutions" Psych 5, no. 4: 1191-1206. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/psych5040079

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop