Next Article in Journal
The Association between Korean Clinical Nurses’ Workplace Bullying, Positive Psychological Capital, and Social Support on Burnout
Next Article in Special Issue
Factor Structure and Psychometric Properties for the PTSD Checklist of Chinese Adolescents in the Closed Period after the COVID-19 Outbreak
Previous Article in Journal
Burnout of Healthcare Workers Amid the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Follow-Up Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Changes in Alienation in Physical Education Classes, School Happiness, and Expectations of a Future Healthy Life after the COVID-19 Pandemic in Korean Adolescents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Descriptive and Quasi-Experimental Studies about Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, Anger Management, and Their Relations with Key Competencies in Primary Education

by
Antonio L. González-Gómez
1,
David P. Farrington
2 and
Vicente J. Llorent
1,*
1
Department of Education, University of Cordoba, C/San Alberto Magno, s/n., 14004 Córdoba, Spain
2
Institute of Criminology, Cambridge University, Sidgwick Avenue, Cambridge CB3 9DA, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(21), 11584; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111584
Submission received: 26 September 2021 / Revised: 29 October 2021 / Accepted: 1 November 2021 / Published: 4 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Promoting Health for Adolescents)

Abstract

:
Background: Antisocial behaviours make social interactions difficult among students. Moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management are social and emotional variables related to prosocial and antisocial behaviours and health problems. This research aims to assess the impact of Cooperative Project-Based Learning intervention on these three variables for Primary Education students. Additionally, the relations of these variables with key competencies, such as social and emotional competencies and literacy competence, were studied. Method: This research is made up of two studies, descriptive and quasi-experimental, during regular school hours. The descriptive study was carried out with a sample of 516 primary school students and aimed to assess the development of the three variables, taking into account personal and ethnic-cultural factors. The quasi-experimental study, with pre-test and post-test data, had the participation of 145 students to study the incidence of these variables after Cooperative Project-Based Learning intervention in Primary Education. Results: The results show the relation among the cited variables and the positive impact of the intervention on moral emotions and anger management in the experimental group compared to the control group. Experimental group girls presented higher scores in moral emotions than control group girls. Conclusion: These results open new research lines in relation to the intervention as a programme to prevent the appearance of antisocial behaviours and health problems at school.

1. Introduction

Positive social relationships can be altered by the appearance of antisocial behaviours, such as aggression, violence, or truancy, among others, which transgress moral and social norms [1]. Antisocial behaviours have been related to moral emotions [2]. Moral emotions are the emotional responses to behaviours related to moral values that promote moral actions [3]. Moral emotions are learned during the relationships that people have in moral events [4]. Some moral emotions are guilt, pride, shame, or satisfaction [5]. These moral emotions increase desirable behaviour [6] and promote moral actions [7]. Guilt feelings have been negatively associated with aggression [8], and higher levels of moral reasoning were positively related to prosocial behaviours in children aged 4 to 5 years [9]. A study [10] identified that six-year-old students, who were classified as prosocial by their teachers, presented moral attributions more frequently. Other research [11] revealed that children’s age was a significant predictor of their aggression, and that the moral issues of the children (4–6 years old) predicted an additional significant portion of the variation in aggressive behaviour. However, more studies are required to analyse the development of moral emotions in Primary Education students.
The appearance of antisocial behaviours is also associated with a low level of empathy [12]. Empathy is a key interpersonal variable present in interpersonal interactions [13] and is defined as “understanding and sharing another’s emotional state or context” [14] (p. 988). This variable is made up of two dimensions [15]; affective empathy (the ability to experience and share the emotional states and emotional contexts of other people) and cognitive empathy (the ability to understand the emotions that other people feel in their emotional context). Empathy is positively related to moral reasoning [16], social awareness, and prosocial behaviour, which are dimensions of social and emotional competencies [17]. Empathy has been studied in Primary Education students [18], where results are contradictory by sex, and in Secondary Education [19], where girls show a higher score than boys.
Prosocial behaviours happen face-to-face, but they also happen when students interact online through social networking sites, online video games, e-mail, chats, or text messages. The adolescent population could misuse both the Internet and online social networks [20,21], which can lead to inappropriate problem behaviours online, such as cyberbullying [22]. Some studies have indicated that low empathy is one of the predictors of cyberbullying [23], finding associations between low levels of empathy and cyberperpetration [24]. Virtual or online empathy [25] is shown to be consistent with interpersonal interactions, and positive and significant correlations have been found between face-to-face and virtual empathy, with lower levels of online empathy than levels of empathy offline. Online interaction may reduce an individual’s empathic capacity because the online connection reduces the amount of time spent interacting face-to-face with others [26]. This interaction can result in a person missing non-verbal communication elements that are essential for reading emotions, such as facial expressions, body posture, eye contact, or gestures. However, online behaviours could support or even increase empathy [27]. Other studies highlight that online empathy does not seem to be clearly related to cyberbullying [28]. These studies provide a controversial view of online empathy, indicating the need for new studies that provide a more complete view of this subject, especially in Primary Education.
Attention control, such as anger management, is a regulating mechanism that is also important to ensure adequate social interaction and avoid antisocial behaviours [29]. Prosocial behaviour is negatively related to anger in students of five, seven, and nine years old [2]. Anger is a primary emotion that is considered harmful and that manifests itself when an individual cannot obtain a goal or satisfy a need [30]. Anger at school is considered an emotional reaction experienced by students [31] that is related positively to social pressure exerted by others [32] and related negatively to self-esteem and optimism [33]. The regulation of emotional behaviours makes it possible to predict socially appropriate behaviours [34]. Controlling attentional processes is important for managing negative emotions, such as anger, and this overt behaviour seems to predict problems of showing these emotions [35]. Consequently, it is necessary to carry out studies in Primary Education that broaden knowledge on anger management, due to its importance to develop adequate social relationships among students from the beginning of the school system.
Schools must promote different dimensions of learning, including cognitive, social, emotional, and moral competencies, in their goal of promoting a comprehensive education [36]. A European Union recommendation [37] advises the development of key competencies in the school curricula. As one of these key competencies, social, emotional, and moral competencies could protect children from bullying [38], future antisocial behaviours, and delinquency among students [39]. Findings in social and emotional learning programs [40] have shown their positive impact on students’ social and emotional skills, academic performance, behaviours, and attitudes. A meta-analysis with 29 (quasi-)experimental studies [41] also shows the positive effects of introducing social and emotional competencies in early-age students’ curricula. However, despite all these positive advantages of incorporating key competencies into the curriculum, they are not worked on in a systematic way.
An intervention based on Project-Based Learning, as a didactic method, could allow students to enhance key competencies, especially social, emotional, and moral competencies. This method promotes active participation in the learning process through social interactions and knowledge exchange [42]. This type of inquiry-based learning promotes solving authentic problems within real-world practices [43]. Project-Based Learning leads to meaningful learning experiences that involve students in their own learning [44]. Recent research [45] showed that Project-Based Learning about values improved many skills, such as recognition of prejudice or empathy. At the same time, cooperation promotes student interaction, where students would share common homework and goals. Cooperative Learning has benefits as a didactic method with the potential to positively affect the intergroup relations of students [46]. Working in small groups allows students to join forces and share resources to enhance their own learning and that of other team members [47]. The assignment of a role to each member of the cooperative base groups [48] ensures interaction and social skills development, such as leadership, decision-making, trust-building, communication, and skills related to conflict management. An intervention, which combined both didactic methods, was carried out in Primary Education [49], and it promoted social and emotional competencies, where both empathy and literacy competence were included.
Cooperative Project-Based Learning, as a combined method, could also be an effective way to develop moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management in students in Grades Four, Five, and Six of Primary Education. For this reason, the main objective of the study is to analyse the impact of a specific intervention to enhance moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management. Before the analysis of the interventions’ impact, the three variables will also be analysed, taking into account personal factors such as age, sex, or ethnic-cultural group. Moreover, these personal factors will be analysed through their relationship with each other and with other variables, such as social and emotional competencies, empathy, and literacy competence, which have been described in an earlier study [50].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Two different samples have participated in this research according to the two studies: descriptive and quasi-experimental (see Table 1). Both samples were chosen by incidental sampling from teachers who were interested in participating in the research.
The descriptive study has a sample of 516 students (50.4% boys and 49.5% girls) from six Primary Education schools in Spain, from Grades 4, 5, and 6. The students were distributed as follows: 226 4th graders (43.8%, M = 9.17, SD = 0.53), 137 5th graders (26.6%, M = 10.07, SD = 0.31), and 153 6th graders (29.7%, M = 11.05, SD = 0.34). The students’ age range was 8 to 12 years old (M = 9.97, SD = 0.88). The sample was divided into two groups, according to their ethnic-cultural group. The majority group consisted of 429 (83.1%) students, and the minority group consisted of 87 students (16.9%). The ethnic-cultural minority group included the gipsy ethnic group and immigrants: 24 (4.7%) students self-identified as members of the gipsy ethnic group (13 with both parents identifying as gipsy and 11 with at least one gipsy parent), and 63 students were immigrants (12.2% of the sample), including those who do not have Spanish nationality or children who, even though they have the Spanish nationality, have at least one of their parents with a foreign nationality. The rest of the students, not included in the minority group, were part of the majority group. The ethnic-cultural component is very similar to the average in the entire region of Andalusia, where there are approximately half a million people of the gipsy ethnic group (6.2%) and where the immigrant population represents 13.5% of the entire population [51,52].
The quasi-experimental study sample consisted of 145 children (47.6% boys and 52.41% girls), with an age range of 8 to 12 years (M = 10.03 years, SD = 0.92), enrolled in two lines of Grade 4, 5, and 6 during the 2017/2018 academic year. The control group consisted of 71 children (48.6% boys and 51.4% girls), and the experimental group consisted of 74 children (46.5% boys and 53.5% girls). In the same schools, the control group (49%, M = 10.04 years, SD = 0.89) included those in Grades 4 (n = 24), 5 (n = 21) and 6 (n = 26), and the experimental group (51%, M = 10.01 years, SD = 0.96) also included those in Grades 4 (n = 27), 5 (n = 21) and 6 (n = 26).

2.2. Design and Procedure

The first and descriptive part of the research has an ex-post facto design carried out through a survey of a convenience sample of students in Grades 4, 5, and 6 of Primary Education in the first quarter of the 2017/2018 academic year. The researchers selected six schools from contacts with different teachers who were interested in the scientific study. The survey was anonymous, and the students could refuse completion; it was not completed by 31 students. This research has published its first results about social and emotional competencies, including empathy and literacy competence [18,50]. The data was used to perform correlation analyses with all of the variables of the current study and to reach a more complete view of the profile of the Primary Education students.
The second part of the research develops a quasi-experimental study, with a pre-test and post-test, with a control and an experimental group. The pre-test and post-test were carried out with the participating students at the beginning and at the end of the 2017/2018 academic year. The pre-test questionnaire (Time 1) was completed for all research participants. At the end of that academic year, the questionnaire was given to both groups again, post-test (Time 2). At the beginning of the academic year, the researchers analysed the lesson plan of both groups. Additionally, the researchers were in contact with the teachers during the whole academic year, supervising the lesson plan in both groups. The intervention was carried out only in the experimental group by the regular teachers in the classroom, during regular lessons. The teachers of the experimental group received a three-hour training session for the adequate implementation of the intervention. The teachers were in permanent contact with the researchers during the intervention to facilitate a common experience in the different groups. In the control group, the intervention was not implemented, and students followed textbook activities according to the grade. The control group did not develop any cooperative activity or project-based methods during the academic year.
In both studies, the students completed the questionnaires individually, as one more class activity, during school hours. Data collection was always carried out by a member of the research team, in the presence of the teachers who were responsible for each group. Teachers did not have access to individual questionnaires at any time. The researchers obtained the necessary authorisations, and the participants were informed of the objective of the research, the process, and the expected duration. Ethical Standards of American Psychological Association’s Ethics Code and the Ethical Committee of University of Córdoba (Cordoba, Spain) were followed for the study.

2.3. The Intervention in the Quasi-Experimental Study

The teachers of the experimental group students carried out an intervention of Cooperative Project-Based Learning [49] during the 2017/2018 academic year. The intervention is carried out in the literacy area schedule and structured weekly in two phases. Phase 1 is carried out during four sessions of one hour, and its objective is the curricular learning of the specific contents of the literacy area. In these lessons, the teacher uses explanations based on lectures and completing tasks by the students. Phase 2 is developed during a two-hour session that is dedicated to the realisation of the cooperative project.
The cooperative project has as its main objective to create several stories that teach prosocial values: respect, forgiveness, friendship, generosity, tolerance, effort, and peace. Creating a story is sequenced in four activities. The first activity consists of describing characters; in the second activity, the students have to describe a place; in the third activity, they have to make up the story using the characters and the place from the previous activities; and, finally, in the fourth activity, they have to divide the story into scenes, and they could learn and teach the values using a kamishibai (a name that refers to material of Japanese origin that is used to tell stories through illustrations). In a periodical inter-class activity, these groups of students tell the stories to groups of students of lower grades. When a group finishes creating a story, they begin creating another. At the end of the academic year, all of these stories created by the different groups were compiled into a book.
The students of the experimental group were grouped in cooperative base groups of 3–4 students. Each cooperative base group includes four roles: spokesperson, evaluator, evaluators’ assistant (when there were four members in the group), and material manager. These roles rotated when project activity was changed. Individually, each student has to propose a personal objective before each of the activities that made up the project. At the end of the activity, each student carries out a self-reflection exercise to achieve this objective. These reflective objectives deal with attitudes and behaviours that the students had with the rest of the partners in the cooperative base group during the interaction of the activities.
The control group students maintained the usual curriculum and did not carry out any similar intervention activities.

2.4. Instruments

Information has been collected through an instrument composed of seven parts: personal data (age, sex, nationality, and nationality of the parents and whether or not they belong to the gipsy ethnic-cultural group), Moral Emotions Scale, Online Empathy Questionnaire, Anger Management Questionnaire, Social and Emotional Competencies Questionnaire, Basic Empathy Scale, and Literacy Proficiency Test.
The Moral Emotions Scale [53] is made up of 5 items that present Likert type responses from 1 to 5, where 1 is “Totally disagree” and 5 is “Totally agree”. The reliability of the instrument is adequate for the descriptive study sample (α = 0.75 and Ω = 0.77) and for the quasi-experimental study sample, pre-test (α = 0.68 and Ω = 0.70), and post-test (α = 0.79 and Ω = 0.80).
The Online Empathy Questionnaire [54] is an instrument based on the Basic Empathy Scale [15], and is used to evaluate empathy through electronic devices. The instrument is made up of seven items that are Likert type answers from 1 to 5, where 1 is “Totally disagree” and 5 is “Totally agree”. This scale has two factors: online affective empathy and online cognitive empathy. The reliability of this research was high, in both the descriptive study (α = 0.80 and Ω = 0.80) and the quasi-experimental study, pre-test (α = 0.83 and Ω = 0.83), and post-test (α = 0.73 and Ω = 0.73).
The Anger Management Questionnaire is made up of four Likert type response items from 1 to 5, where 1 is “Totally disagree” and 5 is “Totally agree”. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed with the sample of 516 children and shows that the data obtained good scores in the different fit indices (χ2 = 12.4102 df = 2; p = 0.002; NFI = 0.99; NNFI = 0.97; CFI = 0.965; RMSEA = 0.102; 90% CI = 0.053 to 0.159). The reliability of the instrument is adequate (α = 0.76 and Ω = 0.76) for the descriptive study sample and for the quasi-experimental study sample, pre-test (α = 0.72 and Ω = 0.73), and post-test (α = 0.77 and Ω = 0.78).
The Social and Emotional Competencies Questionnaire (SEC-Q) [17] is composed of 16 items that make up four factors of social and emotional competencies: self-awareness (four items), self-management and motivation (three items), social awareness and prosocial behaviour (six items), and decision-making (three items). The items are presented with Likert type responses from 1 to 5, where 1 is “Totally disagree” and 5 is “Totally agree”. The instrument has a high reliability in the descriptive (α = 0.80 and Ω = 0.86) and in the quasi-experimental study, pre-test (α = 0.82 and Ω = 0.82), and post-test (α = 0.85 and Ω = 0.85).
The Basic Empathy Scale (BES) [15], validated in Spanish by [23], consists of 20 items and includes two factors: affective empathy (11 items) and cognitive empathy (nine items). The items are presented on a Likert scale where 1 is “Totally disagree”, and 5 is “Totally agree”. The reliability is high in the descriptive study sample (α = 0.83 and Ω = 0.82); quasi-experimental study, pre-test (α = 0.83 and Ω = 0.84), and post-test (α = 0.81 and Ω = 0.82).
The Literacy Proficiency Test [18] is based on the PIRLS tests [55] and assesses listening, reading, and writing. The listening part begins with the teacher reading a story, and the children answer various questions about the story. In the reading part, children answer written questions about a text that they had previously read. In the third part of the test—writing—students write a story based on an image. These tests are evaluated using two scales: the first scale measured comprehension (listening and reading), and the second scale measured expression (writing). Both scales were divided into six levels, including A1 (the most basic/introductory level), A2, B1, B2, C1, and C2 (mastery of proficiency level) based on the criteria from [56]. For example, writing A1 level is “He/she vaguely describes tangible realities that know or see using words”, and C2 level is “He/she rebuilds information and arguments from various sources, and presents them in a coherent and originally summarised way”. This instrument has been used by other researchers with the same samples [18,50]. The reliability is high in the descriptive study (α = 0.93 and Ω = 0.93), pre-test (α = 0.86 and Ω = 0.87), and post-test (α = 0.97 and Ω = 0.97). The reliability was calculated by adding up the score of each of the three factors, because it is a polytomous scale.

2.5. Data Analysis

The psychometric properties of the questionnaires were checked. The reliability coefficient was analysed through Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s Omega, using Factor 10.5.2. In the case of the Anger Management Scale, due to no previous use of the scales in Primary Education children, confirmatory factor analyses were performed with EQS 6.2, which were adjusted to the robust maximum likelihood method and polychoric correlations (Satorra–Bentler chi-square). The data analyses were carried out using SPSS 25, and the effect size was estimated with the Campbell Collaboration calculator.
Descriptive and comparative analyses were carried out with the first sample. The means of the variables with two groups were compared using the Student’s t-test, and Levene’s test was performed to identify the homogeneity of the variances. The groups were compared with Cohen’s d with 95% CI, to show the strength of the relationships of moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management.
Correlations were calculated between the main study variables (moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management) and age, social and emotional competencies, empathy, and literacy competence to find out possible relationships. The prediction of literacy competence and its factors, as dependent variables, were analysed using a cross-sectional linear regression (all variables at time 1) and with a longitudinal linear regression (predictor variables at time 1 and literacy competence and its factors at time 2).
The progress of the variables after the intervention was analysed with the second sample. The effect size of between-group differences in the pre-test was calculated to know if there were between-group differences both before and after the intervention. This analysis was calculated using a web-based effect-size calculator (the Campbell Collaboration Calculator). The difference in moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management in the longitudinal analyses of the groups—control and experimental—was studied using the ANOVA test of repeated measures. The Mauchly sphericity test was performed to decide if corrections were necessary in the repeated measures ANOVA test, considering p < 0.05 significant. The effect size of the ANOVA test of repeated measures was tested with the partial eta squared.

3. Results

3.1. Moral Emotions by Age, Sex, and Ethnic-Cultural Group in Primary Education

Age did not show a significant correlation with moral emotions (r = −0.03, p = 0.576). According to sex, no significant differences were found in moral emotions (Mboys = 21.90, SDboys = 3.23 vs. Mgirls = 22.94, SDgirls = 2.96, d = 0.02, 95% CI = −0.15, 0.20). The scores of the students of the ethnic-cultural majority group did not show significant differences from the students of the ethnic-cultural minority group (Mmaj = 22.5, SDmaj = 3.10 vs. Mmin = 21.91, SDmin = 3.36, d = 0.19, CI 95% = −0.05, 0.43).

3.2. Online Empathy by Age, Sex, and Ethnic-Cultural Group in Primary Education

Age did not show a significant correlation with online empathy (r = −0.05, p = 0.049), including its two factors; online empathy affective (r = −0.05, p = 0.31) and online empathy cognitive (r = −0.03, p = 0.47). Scores for online empathy (Mboys = 22.21, SDboys = 5.02 vs. Mgirls = 22.26, SDgirls = 4.67, d = 0.05, 95% CI = −0.13, 0.24) and their online affective empathy factor (Mboys = 8.89, SDboys = 3.04 vs. Mgirls = 8.80, SDgirls = 3.06, d = −0.03, 95% CI = −0.20, 0.14) showed no significant differences between boys and girls. However, scores for the online cognitive empathy factor showed significant differences between girls and boys (Mboys = 13.27, SDboys = 3.08 vs. Mgirls = 13.39, SDgirls = 2.67, d = 0.04, 95% CI = −0.14, 0.22). The scores of the students of the ethnic-cultural majority group did not show significant differences from the students of the ethnic-cultural minority group (Mmaj = 22.21, SDmaj = 4.92 vs. Mmin = 22.51, SDmin = 4.41, d = −0.06, 95% CI = −0.32, 0.19) and its factors: online affective empathy (Mmaj = 8.92, SDmaj = 3.06 vs. Mmin = 8.49, SDmin = 2.90, d = 0.15, 95% CI = −0.10, 0.39) and online cognitive empathy (Mmaj = 13.24, SDmaj = 2.90 vs. Mmin = 13.90, SDmin = 2.77, d = −0.33, 95% CI = −0.49, 0.03).

3.3. Anger Management by Age, Sex, and Ethnic-Cultural Group in Primary Education

Anger management (r = −0.14, p = 0.002) had a significant and negative correlation with age. No significant differences were found between boys and girls in anger management (Mboys = 12.69, SDboys = 3.89 vs. Mgirls = 14.36, SDgirls = 3.89, d = 0.03, 95% CI = −0.15, 0.20). The scores of the students of the ethnic-cultural majority group did not show significant differences from the students of the ethnic-cultural minority group (Mmaj = 13.51, SDmaj = 4.07 vs. Mmin = 13.48, SDmin = 3.53, d = 0.01, CI 95% = −0.23, 0.25).

3.4. Correlations between Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, and Anger Management, and Empathy, Social, and Emotional Competencies, and Literacy Competence

Moral emotions, online empathy, including its online affective empathy and online cognitive empathy factors, and anger management correlated positively and significantly. The correlations also show that moral emotions and anger management are significantly and positively related to social and emotional competencies, including all of its factors, and to empathy. Moral emotions correlated significantly with both factors: online affective empathy and online cognitive empathy. However, anger management only correlated significantly and positively with online cognitive empathy. Online empathy and its two factors correlated significantly and positively with empathy and its two factors, and with social and emotional competencies and some of its factors. Moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management did not significantly correlate with literacy competence (see Table 2).

3.5. Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, and Anger Management as Predictors of Literacy Competence

In the descriptive study, a first linear regression was performed to investigate the independent relationships between the predictors and literacy competence, controlling for moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, social and emotional competencies, empathy, and personal variables (age, sex, and ethnic-cultural group membership). Four statistically significant cross-sectional models were estimated (see Table 3): literacy competence (R2 = 0.32, F = 12.65, p < 0.001), listening (R2 = 0.23, F = 8.59, p < 0.001), reading (R2 = 0.24, F = 8.30, p < 0.001), and writing (R2 = 0.31, F = 11.87, p < 0.001). Personal variables (older, being a girl, and ethnic-cultural majority) and self-awareness were predictors in four models. Besides these variables, affective empathy was a predictor of literacy competence, and social-awareness and prosocial behaviour were predictors of listening.
Longitudinal predictors of literacy competence were studied through a second linear regression, which was performed with moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, social and emotional competencies, empathy, and personal variables (age, sex, ethnic-cultural group membership, and intervention group). Four statistically significant longitudinal models were estimated (see Table 4): literacy competence (R2 = 0.36, F = 3.60, p < 0.001), listening (R2 = 0.39, F = 4.07, p < 0.001), reading (R2 = 0.40, F = 4.19, p < 0.001), and writing (R2 = 0.29, F = 2.54, p < 0.01). Intervention group was a predictor in all four models. Besides this variable, age and self-awareness were predictors of literacy competence, listening, and reading.

3.6. Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, and Anger Management in the Pre- and Post-Tests of the Control and Experimental Groups

No significant differences between pre-test control and experimental groups were found in three variables (see Table 5). However, the post-tests were different; moral emotions (F1,136 = 6.70, p = 0.01, η2p = 0.05) and anger management (F1,141 = 4.10, p = 0.045, η2p < 0.01) increased significantly after intervention in the experimental group compared to the control group. Online empathy (F1,110 = 4.38, p = 0.59, η2p < 0.01) and its online affective empathy factor (F1,126 = 3.95, p = 0.46, η2p < 0.01) showed a slight decrease in the scores of both groups, but these were not significant. Online cognitive empathy (F1,115 < 0.01, p = 0.96, η2p < 0.01) showed an increase in the scores of the control and experimental groups, but there are also no significant differences.

3.7. Change in Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, and Anger Management by Sex

No significant differences at pre-test between control and experimental groups were found in three variables in boys (see Table 6) or girls (see Table 7). The results show that there were no differences between the boys in the control and the experimental groups in any of the variables; moral emotions (F1,62 = 2.21, p = 0.14, η2p = 0.03), online empathy (F1,52 = 0.09, p = 0.76, η2p < 0.01), nor in their factors; online affective empathy (F1,58 = 0.23, p = 0.64, η2p < 0.01) and online cognitive empathy (F1,54 = 0.17, p = 0.68, η2p < 0.01); and anger management (F1,65 = 1.36, p = 0.25, η2p = 0.02).
As in the boys’ scores, there were no significant differences among the girls in both groups in the online empathy scores (F1,56 = 0.18, p = 0.67, η2p < 0.01), including their factors online affective empathy (F1,66 = 0.35, p = 0.56, η2p < 0.01) and online cognitive empathy (F1,59 = 0.17, p = 0.69, η2p < 0.01), and anger management (F1,74 = 2.70, p = 0.11, η2p < 0.01). However, the scores of the girls in the experimental group show significant differences with respect to the scores of the girls in the control group in moral emotions (F1,72 = 4.39, p = 0.04, η2p = 0.06).

4. Discussion

Moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management are important variables in the social [23], emotional [34], and moral [2] development of students. For this reason, it is necessary to deepen the knowledge of these three variables in Primary Education students. The study’s objective is to assess the impact of an intervention based on Cooperative Project-Based Learning (which adds the development of prosocial values to the regular curriculum of students in Grades Four, Five, and Six of Primary Education) on moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management. Before assessing the impact of the intervention, these three variables were related to age, sex, ethnic-cultural group membership, and variables of educational interest, including two key competencies; social and emotional competencies, including empathy and literacy competence.
Moral emotions have diverse relations with personal characteristics. Moral emotions were not correlated with age. This result would be in line with another study [57], which did not find changes related to age in self-attributed moral emotions of adolescents. In the current study, sex is not related to moral emotions. The results by sex would not be in line with the study results carried out in early childhood [58], where kindergarten girls showed more development of some moral emotions, such as guilt, than boys. Another study [10] also points out that boys in first and second grade are less prosocial in emotional and moral attributions as compared to girls. Probably, the different results among these studies could be explained by the diverse childhood stage; in the current study, the students are in late-Primary Education, in contrast with the early ages of the cited studies. Moral emotions do not show differences by ethnic-cultural group, as related variables such as social and emotional competencies in Primary Education [18]. Thus, the diverse maturation of children could be implicated in moral development. More studies are required to improve learning in the moral area.
There was no relation between online empathy and age. Significant correlation between age and face-to-face empathy were found in 14–25 year olds [19], and children scored higher in cognitive empathy as they grew [59]. The results for online empathy do not show differences between boys and girls. Online empathy has been analysed with face-to-face empathy by sex in adults [25], but this study did not compare differences between boys and girls in online empathy, and boys and girls showed higher scores in face-to-face empathy than online empathy. In the same line, significant relations have been described between face-to-face empathy and online empathy in adults [25], like the results of the current study in Primary Education students. Significant differences in face-to-face empathy were not found between boys and girls in Primary Education [18]. However, other studies on face-to-face empathy in Primary Education [59,60] and adolescence [15,19] showed significant differences between boys and girls, where girls showed significantly higher scores. There were no significant differences in online empathy between ethnic-cultural group membership, in the same way with other studies about face-to-face empathy in Primary Education [18] or in adolescents and early adults [19]. However, low socio-economic status was associated with low face-to-face empathy [15], and associated to a higher face-to-face empathy with a positive family climate [59]. Ethnic-cultural group membership could be associated with socio-economic status or family climate. The inconsistency of these results obtained in relation to online empathy and its relationship with face-to-face empathy highlights the need to initiate more studies, especially longitudinal studies.
Anger management has shown a significant and negative correlation with age, but no significant differences between boys and girls. Boys were more aggressive than girls, and older students were more aggressive than younger ones [10]. The Integrated Cognitive Antisocial Potential (ICAP) theory [61] indicates that antisocial potential is the key construct for committing antisocial acts. The review of the ICAP theory [62] indicates that, depending on age, different models are specified considering various risk factors (tension, modelling, socialisation, impulsivity, and life events), with the influence of parents or colleagues as a factor depending on children’s age. The manifestation of negative emotions, such as anger, can be different at each age, and depend on various factors. Belonging to an ethnic-cultural group did not show significant differences, and this could be explained by many other personal factors, such as risk factors for violent behaviours. Consequently, anger management can be changeable as well. Anger management requires more studies that deepen knowledge about it.
A previous study [50], with the same sample as the descriptive study, seems to indicate that social and emotional competencies, where empathy is included, are predictive variables for literacy competence. This previous evidence has been complemented by moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management. The correlations show relationships between moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management and social and emotional competencies and face-to-face empathy, but not with literacy competence, and neither with any of its factors. The results of the present study confirm the previous relations among variables [50], even after introducing moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management. The cross-sectional linear regression showed that the self-awareness factor is a predictor of literacy competence, as well as in the longitudinal linear regression. These results support the previous results [50] about the relationships between social and emotional competencies and literacy competence. However, moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management are not predictive variables of the literacy competence level after one academic year, while experimental group membership is a predictor of literacy competence. These results reveal the interconnections among social and emotional variables and the importance of specific lesson plans to develop literacy competence.
The results of the current research show that the use of Cooperative Project-Based Learning has produced benefits in the moral emotions and anger management of the experimental group by including prosocial values in the literacy area. In the same way, this happened in another study [49] with social and emotional competencies and face-to-face empathy. Cooperative learning has a positive impact on intergroup relationships [46]. Cooperative base groups improved social skills [63]. Hence, including cooperative base groups in a Project-Based Learning programme could be a positive way to improve social, emotional, and moral competencies. The inclusion of the self-assessment of behaviour and the inclusion of tasks that explicitly incorporate social, emotional, and moral content and values in the school curriculum could also contribute to the social, emotional, and moral development of students. A cooperative programme in Primary Education [64] improved students’ emotional control and regulation, and empathy. However, online empathy did not show significant differences between the control and experimental groups. This may be because the intervention did not incorporate the use of technological and digital media or applications, which are present issues to consider in future school practice and educational research.
The intervention shows more unexpected results, such as that girls in the experimental group showed significant differences in the scores of moral emotions compared with the girls in the control group, while the boys in the experimental group did not show significant differences from the scores of the boys in the control group in any of the variables. A quasi-experimental study [49] also found this difference by sex in social and emotional competencies and empathy in the pre-test and post-test comparisons of the intervention. Along the same lines, a study of a two-year social and emotional intervention [65] showed that the differences between boys and girls persisted even after the intervention. A longitudinal research study [66] demonstrated that male sex was one of the predictors of future violence. Education could facilitate sex equality and reduce the differences between boys and girls, as suggested by other studies about teacher training [67]; however, a way to generate a significant impact on boys would have to be found.

5. Conclusions

The descriptive study shows the development of moral emotions, online empathy, and anger management in Primary Education, detailing analyses by sex and age. This study also showed the existing correlation with social and emotional competencies, including face-to-face empathy. The quasi-experimental study showed the positive impact of a Cooperative Project-Based Learning intervention on moral emotions and anger management when elements of social, emotional, and moral learning are introduced into literacy plan lessons.
Schools must contribute to the total development of students. For this reason, the Council of Europe [56] advises the incorporation of key competencies in national curricula, as a base for the teaching–learning process. Social, emotional, and moral competencies are included in these key and basic competencies, but their incorporation into the school curriculum has difficulties, because they are not associated with any particular subject. The inclusion of social and emotional competencies in the school curriculum, in areas such as language, has been shown to produce benefits for students [68,69]. The intervention in the current study presents added value, since it incorporates social, emotional, and moral competencies in the curriculum, specifically in the literacy subject. The results have implications for school management to guarantee the acquisition of the key competencies in lesson plans.
The present study yields hopeful findings for improving the education of Primary Education students. The sample size is a limitation, which does not allow wide generalisation of the conclusions. Therefore, it is necessary to continue more studies on Primary Education, with more participants involved in other educational stages, to expand the knowledge of the study variables. The findings open new lines of research.
The first line of research would be to analyse the possible causes of personal differences—as well as among different variables—and effects of new proposals that affect social, emotional, and moral development. The differences by sex were shown in the descriptive part of the study. New research is needed to verify didactic and organisational innovations that also allow enhancing the development of the social, emotional, and moral aspects in boys in pursuit of gender equality. Roseth [70] suggests that, if a cooperative base team would be formed by people who were committed to the wellbeing of other partners, the programme would be more successful. It is possible that present results depended on the personal skills of cooperative base team members. The results in general are inconsistent according to sex, and more research is needed, especially including other personal factors. In the case of the current descriptive study, differences are shown by sex. For this reason, social, emotional, and moral development will probably be different. A guideline to improve social, emotional, and moral competencies in Primary Education students could be organised, taking into account the cooperative base groups according to social skills, instead of other personal characteristics, such as sex.
The second research line would provide more information on the relationship between moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, and antisocial behaviours at school. A study carried out in middle schools [71] indicated that the cooperative methodology facilitates the reduction of bullying and violence in the classroom. Other studies [72,73] have shown that the development and promotion of social and emotional competencies is protective against antisocial behaviours and health problems at school. Therefore, it would be interesting and pertinent to continue studying Cooperative Project-Based Learning, including other study variables related to antisocial behaviours, such as bullying or cyberbullying, which are usually connected to health problems. These studies would indicate whether this intervention can be used in the prevention or reduction of antisocial behaviours. Aggression and antisocial behaviours can be considered an expression of social exclusion. Therefore, social and emotional learning integrated into the curriculum of the key competencies can promote an inclusive education for all students. For this reason, it is important to carry out more studies about the development of key competencies through Cooperative Project-Based Learning. The results of future studies could provide evidence to propose didactic innovations that contribute to the greater development of social, emotional, and moral competencies, especially in boys, promoting gender equality and inclusive education.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.L.G.-G. and V.J.L.; methodology, A.L.G.-G., D.P.F. and V.J.L.; software, V.J.L.; validation, D.P.F. and V.J.L.; formal analysis, A.L.G.-G. and V.J.L.; investigation, A.L.G.-G. and V.J.L.; resources, V.J.L.; data curation, D.P.F.; writing—original draft preparation, A.L.G.-G. and V.J.L.; writing—review and editing, D.P.F.; visualization, A.L.G.-G.; supervision, V.J.L.; project administration, V.J.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of University of Córdoba (report ad hoc for this research).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy police and data protecttion.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Rutter, M.; Giller, H.; Hagell, A. Antisocial Behavior by Young People; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  2. Malti, T.; Latzko, B. Children’s moral emotions and moral cognition: Towards an integrative perspective. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2010, 129, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Haidt, J. The moral emotions. In Handbook of Affective Sciences; Davidson, R.J., Scherer, K.R., Goldsmith, H.H., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; pp. 852–870. [Google Scholar]
  4. Eisenberg, N. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2000, 51, 665–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Rodríguez-Barbero, M.S. Percepción y Atribución Causal Sobre el Bullying y Sus Efectos Emocionales en Escolares de Primaria. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  6. Perren, S.; Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E. Cyberbullying and traditional bullying in adolescence: Differential roles of moral disengagement, moral emotions, and moral values. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 9, 195–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Tangney, J.P.; Stuewig, J.; Mashek, D.J. Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007, 58, 345–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Malti, T.; Krettenauer, T. The relation of moral emotion attributions to prosocial and antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2013, 84, 397–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Miller, P.A.; Eisenberg, N.; Fabes, R.A.; Shell, R. Relations of moral reasoning and vicarious emotion to young children’s prosocial behavior toward peers and adults. Dev. Psychol. 1996, 32, 210–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Malti, T.; Gasser, L.; Buchmann, M. Aggressive and prosocial children’s emotion attributions and moral reasoning. Aggress. Behav. 2009, 35, 90–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Arsenio, W.; Ramos-Marcuse, F. Children’s moral emotions, narratives, and aggression: Relations with maternal discipline and support. J. Genet. Psychol. 2014, 175, 528–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Zych, I.; Llorent, V.J. Affective empathy and moral disengagement related to late adolescent bullying perpetration. Ethics Behav. 2019, 29, 547–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Del Barrio, M.V.; Aluja, A.; García, L.F. Relationship between empathy and the big five personality traits in a sample of Spanish adolescents. Soc. Behav. Pers. Int. J. 2004, 32, 677–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Cohen, D.; Strayer, J. Empathy in conduct-disordered and comparison youth. Dev. Psychol. 1996, 32, 988–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Jolliffe, D.; Farrington, D.P. Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. J. Adolesc. 2006, 29, 589–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Retuerto, A. Desarrollo del Razonamiento Moral, Razonamiento Moral Prosocial y Empatía en la Adolescencia y Juventud. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  17. Zych, I.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Muñoz-Morales, R.; Llorent, V.J.; de Córdoba, U. Dimensions and psychometric Properties of the Social and Emotional Competencies Questionnaire (SEC-Q) in youth and adolescents. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 2018, 50, 98–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Llorent, V.J.; González-Gómez, A.L. Literacy competence and social and emotional competencies in primary education. Individual and ethnic-cultural factors. Espacios 2020, 41, 8–19. Available online: https://bit.ly/3e9bsEl (accessed on 26 September 2021).
  19. Villadangos, M.; Errasti, J.; Amigo, I.; Jolliffe, D.; García-Cueto, E. Characteristics of Empathy in young people measured by the Spanish validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. Psicothema 2016, 28, 323–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Nasaescu, E.; Marín-López, I.; Llorent, V.J.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Zych, I. Abuse of technology in adolescence and its relation to social and emotional competencies, emotions in online communication, and bullying. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 88, 114–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sorrentino, A. Epidemiology of cyberbullying across Europe: Differences between countries and genders. Educ. Sci. Theory Pract. 2019, 19, 74–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Smith, P.K. The nature of cyberbullying and what we can do about it. J. Res. Speec. Educ. Needs 2015, 15, 176–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Zych, I.; Baldry, A.C.; Farrington, D.P.; Llorent, V.J. Are children involved in cyberbullying low on empathy? A systematic review and meta-analysis of research on empathy versus different cyberbullying roles. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2019, 45, 83–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Brewer, G.; Kerslake, J. Cyberbullying, self-esteem, empathy and loneliness. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 48, 255–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Carrier, M.; Spradlin, A.; Bunce, J.P.; Rosen, L.D. Virtual empathy: Positive and negative impacts of going online upon empathy in young adults. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 52, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Small, G.; Vorgan, G. IBrain: Surviving the Technological Alteration of the Modern Mind; HarperCollins: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  27. Caplan, S.; Turner, J.S. Bringing theory to research on computer-mediated comforting communication. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2007, 23, 985–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Marín-López, I.; Zych, I.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Monks, C.P.; Llorent, V.J. Empathy online and moral disengagement through technology as longitudinal predictors of cyberbullying victimization and perpetration. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2020, 116, 105–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kim-Spoon, J.; Holmes, C.; Deater-Deckard, K. Attention regulates anger and fear to predict changes in adolescent risk-taking behaviors. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 56, 756–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Izard, C.E. Human Emotions; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  31. Boman, P.; Curtis, D.; Furlong, M.J.; Smith, U.C. Cross-validation and rasch analyses of the Australian version of the multidimensional school anger inventory—Revised. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2006, 24, 225–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hernawati, L.; Rahayu, E.; Soejowinoto, P. The predictors of Indonesian senior high school students’ anger at school. J. Educ. Pract. 2015, 6, 108–109. [Google Scholar]
  33. Puskar, K.; Ren, D.; Bernardo, L.M.; Haley, T.; Stark, K.H. Anger correlated with psychosocial variables in rural youth. Issues Compr. Pediatr. Nurs. 2008, 31, 71–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Eisenberg, N.; Fabes, R.A.; Guthrie, I.K.; Reiser, M. Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 136–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Eisenberg, N.; Ma, Y.; Chang, L.; Zhou, Q.; West, S.G.; Aiken, L. Relations of effortful control, reactive undercontrol, and anger to Chinese children’s adjustment. Dev. Psychopathol. 2007, 19, 385–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Creemers, B.; Kyriakides, L. School factors explaining achievement on cognitive and affective outcomes: Establishing a dynamic model of educational effectiveness. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 2010, 54, 263–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Council of the European Union. Council Recommendation of 22 May 2018 on Key Competences for Lifelong Learning. Off. J. Eur. Union 2018, C 189/01, 1–13. Available online: https://bit.ly/2XRyr0l (accessed on 26 September 2021).
  38. Llorent, V.J.; Farrington, D.P.; Zych, I. School climate policy and its relations with social and emotional competencies, bullying and cyberbullying in secondary education. Rev. Psicodidáct. 2021, 26, 35–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Espejo-Siles, R.; Zych, I.; Llorent, V.J. Empathy, social and emotional competencies, bullying perpetration and victimization as longitudinal predictors of somatic symptoms in adolescence. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 271, 145–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Taylor, R.D.; Oberle, E.; Durlak, J.A.; Weissberg, R.P. Promoting positive youth development through school-based social and emotional learning interventions: A meta-analysis of follow-up effects. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 1156–1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Yang, W.; Datu, J.A.; Lin, X.; Lau, M.M.; Li, H. Can early childhood curriculum enhance social-emotional competence in low-income children? A meta-analysis of the educational effects. Early Educ. Dev. 2019, 30, 36–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Cocco, S. Student Leadership Development: The Contribution of Project-Based Learning. Ph.D. Thesis, Royal Roads University, Victoria, BC, Canada, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  43. Al-Balushi, S.M.; Al-Aamri, S.S. The effect of environmental science projects on students’ environmental knowledge and science attitudes. Int. Res. Geogr. Environ. Educ. 2014, 23, 213–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Wurdinger, S.; Haar, J.; Hugg, R.; Bezon, J. A qualitative study using project-based learning in a mainstream middle school. Improv. Sch. 2007, 10, 150–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Seçgin, F. Experiences of prospective social studies teachers in project-based values education. Pegem J. Educ. Instr. 2020, 10, 1001–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Keramati, M.R.; Gillies, R.M. Perceptions of undergraduate students on the effect of cooperative learning on academic achievement. J. Appl. Res. High. Educ. 2021. ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Johnson, D.W.; Johnson, R.T.; Holubec, E.J. Cooperation in the Classroom; Interaction: Edina, MN, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  48. Johnson, D.W.; Johnson, F. Joining Together: Group Theory and Group Skills; Allyn & Bacon: Boston, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  49. Llorent, V.J.; González-Gómez, A.L.; Farrington, D.P.; Zych, I. Cooperative Project-Based Learning improves literacy competence and social and emotional competencies in Primary Education. Psicothema 2021, in press. [Google Scholar]
  50. Llorent, V.J.; González-Gómez, A.L.; Farrington, D.P.; Zych, I. Social and emotional competencies and empathy as predictors of literacy competence. Psicothema 2020, 32, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Instituto de Estadística y Cartografía de Andalucía. Padrón Municipal de Habitantes. Cifras Oficiales de Población Municipal [Municipal Population Census. Official Data of Municipal Population]. 2017. Available online: https://bit.ly/39hMlzI (accessed on 26 September 2021).
  52. Junta de Andalucía. Gitanas y Gitanos en el Siglo XXI. Plan. Integral Para la Inclusión de la Comunidad Gitana 2017–2020 [Gypsies in the 21st Century. Comprehensive Plan for the Inclusion of the Gipsy Community 2017–2020]; Consejería de Igualdad y Políticas Sociales: Sevilla, Spain, 2017; Available online: https://bit.ly/2V9CZgW (accessed on 26 September 2021).
  53. Álamo, M.; Llorent, V.J.; Nasaescu, E.; Zych, I. Validación de la Escala de Emociones Morales en adolescentes. [Validation of the Moral Emotions Scale in adolescents]. In Proceedings of the Congreso Internacional de Transferencia de Conocimientos y Sensibilización Social; “Islam y Paz a Través de Voces Musulmanas”, Seville, Spain, 11–12 November 2020; Llorent-Bedmar, V., Cobano-Delgado Palma, V., Eds.; GIECSE-Universidad de Sevilla: Seville, Spain, 2020; pp. 16–20. [Google Scholar]
  54. Marín-López, I.; Zych, I.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Monks, C. Validación y propiedades psicométricas del Cuestionario de Empatía Online y el Cuestionario de Desconexión Moral a través de las Tecnologías. [Validation and psychometric properties of the Online Empathy Questionnaire and the Moral Disengagement through Technologies Questionnaire]. In Creando Redes Doctorales Vol. VII: “Investiga y Comunica”; Chica Pérez, A.F., Mérida García, J., Eds.; UCOPress: Cordoba, Spain, 2019; pp. 525–528. [Google Scholar]
  55. Mullis, I.V.S.; Martin, M.O. PIRLS 2021. Assessment Frameworks. TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Lynch School of Education, Boston College and International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA). 2019. Available online: https://bit.ly/2YdVUJi (accessed on 26 September 2021).
  56. Council of Europe. Common European Framework of Languages. Learning, Teaching, Assessment (CEFR); Council of Europe: Paris, France, 2018; Available online: https://bit.ly/3oaLCrp (accessed on 26 September 2021).
  57. Krettenauer, T.; Eichler, D. Adolescents’ self-attributed moral emotions following a moral transgression: Relations with delinquency, confidence in moral judgment and age. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 24, 489–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kochanska, G.; Gross, J.N.; Lin, M.-H.; Nichols, K.E. Guilt in young children: Development, determinants, and relations with a broader system of standards. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 461–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Sanchez-Perez, N.; Fuentes, L.J.; Jolliffe, D.; Gonzã¡lez-Salinas, C. Assessing children’s empathy through a Spanish adaptation of the Basic Empathy Scale: Parent’s and child’s report forms. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Geng, Y.; Xia, D.; Qin, B. The Basic Empathy Scale: A chinese validation of a measure of empathy in adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2012, 43, 499–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Farrington, D.P. The Integrated Cognitive Antisocial Potential Theory (ICAP). In Integrated Developmental and Life-Course Theories of Offending; Farrington, D.P., Ed.; Transaction: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 73–92. [Google Scholar]
  62. Farrington, D.P. The Integrated Cognitive Antisocial Potential (ICAP) theory: Past, present, and future. J. Dev. Life Course Criminol. 2020, 6, 172–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Mendo-Lázaro, S.; León-del Barco, B.; Felipe-Castaño, E.; Polo-del-Río, M.-I.; Iglesias-Gallego, D. Cooperative team learning and the development of social skills in higher education: The variables involved. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Rivera-Pérez, S.; Fernandez-Rio, J.; Gallego, D.I. Uncovering the nexus between cooperative learning contexts and achievement goals in physical education. Percept. Mot. Skills 2021, 128, 1821–1835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Elias, M.J.; Gara, M.A.; Schuyler, T.F.; Branden-Muller, L.R.; Sayette, M.A. The promotion of social competence: Longitudinal study of a preventive school-based program. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 1991, 61, 409–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Espejo-Siles, R.; Zych, I.; Farrington, D.P.; Llorent, V.J. Moral disengagement, victimization, empathy, social and emotional competencies as predictors of violence in children and adolescents. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2020, 118, 105337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Llorent, V.J.; Álamo, M. Actitudes hacia la diversidad cultural de los estudiantes de Grado de Magisterio de Educación Primaria. Análisis entre el primer y el últimocurso. [Attitudes towards the cultural diversity of Primary Education Teaching Degree students. Analysis between first and last course]. Cadmo 2016, 2, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Jones, S.M.; Brown, J.L.; Hoglund, W.; Aber, J.L. A school-randomized clinical trial of an integrated social–emotional learning and literacy intervention: Impacts after 1 school year. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 78, 829–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Jones, S.M.; Brown, J.L.; Aber, J.L. Two-year impacts of a universal school-based social-emotional and literacy intervention: An experiment in translational developmental research. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 533–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Roseth, C.J. Character education, moral education, and moral-character education. In Handbook of Educational Psychology; Corno, L., Anderman, E.M., Eds.; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: Oxford, UK, 2016; pp. 213–225. [Google Scholar]
  71. Van Ryzin, M.J.; Roseth, C.J. Cooperative learning in middle school: A means to improve peer relations and reduce victimization, bullying, and related outcomes. J. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 110, 1192–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Zych, I.; Beltrán-Catalán, M.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Llorent, V.J. Social and emotional competencies in adolescents involved in different bullying and cyberbullying roles. Rev. Psicodidáct. 2018, 23, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Zych, I.; Farrington, D.P.; Llorent, V.J.; Ttofi, M.M. Protecting Children against Bullying and Its Consequences; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2017. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Participants.
Table 1. Participants.
Descriptive StudyQuasi-Experimental Study
N516145
Age8 to 12 years (M = 9.97, SD = 0.88)8 to 12 years (M = 10.03 years, SD = 0.92)
Boys50.4% (n = 260)47.6% (n = 69)
Girls49.5% (n = 256)52.4% (n = 76)
Grade 4226 (43.8%, M = 9.17, SD = 0.53)Control group (n = 24, 16.6%, M = 9.04; SD = 0.20)
Experimental group (n = 27, 18.6%, M = 8.96, SD = 0.34)
Grade 5137 (26.6%, M = 10.07, SD = 0.31)Control group (n = 21, 14.5%, M = 10.05, SD = 0.22)
Experimental group (n = 21, 14.5%, M = 10.05, SD = 0.22)
Grade 6153 (29.7%, M = 11.05, SD = 0.34)Control group (n = 26, 17.9%, M = 10.96, SD = 0.53)
Experimental group (n = 26, 17.9%, M = 11.08, SD = 0.40)
Ethnic-cultural groupMajority group = 429There are no ethnic-cultural participants.
Minority group = 87
Table 2. Correlations among moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, literacy competence, social and emotional competencies, and empathy.
Table 2. Correlations among moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, literacy competence, social and emotional competencies, and empathy.
Moral EmotionsOnline Affective EmpathyOnline Cognitive EmpathyOnline EmpathyAnger Management
Moral Emotions
Online Affective Empathy0.14 **
Online Cognitive Empathy0.13 **0.33 **
Online Empathy0.17 **0.83 **0.81 **
Anger management0.22 **0.14 **0.11 *0.15 **
Self-awareness0.22 **0.060.080.080.28 **
Self-management and motivation0.25 **0.060.10 *0.11 *0.22 **
Social awareness and prosocial behaviour0.41 **0.10 *0.18 **0.17 **0.41 **
Decision-making0.23 **0.060.12 **0.10 *0.33 **
Social and emotional competencies0.38 **0.11*0.17 **16 **0.42 **
Affective Empathy0.25 **0.34 **0.13 **0.30 **0.07
Cognitive Empathy0.32 **0.11 *0.15 **0.14 **0.27 **
Empathy0.34 **0.30 **0.17 **0.29 **0.18 **
Listening0.06−0.03−0.04−0.04−0.01
Reading0.080.01−0.05−0.030.02
Writing0.09 *0.01−0.04−0.02−0.02
Literacy competence0.09<0.01−0.05−0.03<0.01
* Correlations are significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). ** Correlations are significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Table 3. Linear regression analyses with moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, socio-emotional competencies, empathy, and personal variables (age, gender, and ethnic-cultural group membership) as cross-sectional predictors of literacy competence (n = 516).
Table 3. Linear regression analyses with moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, socio-emotional competencies, empathy, and personal variables (age, gender, and ethnic-cultural group membership) as cross-sectional predictors of literacy competence (n = 516).
Literacy CompetenceListeningReadingWriting
βtβtβtΒt
Moral emotions−0.00−0.33−0.00−0.17−0.00−0.38−0.00−0.09
Online affective empathy−0.000.26−0.00−0.350.010.830.000.05
Online cognitive empathy−0.01−1.03−0.00−0.23−0.01−1.01−0.01−0.96
Anger management−0.00−0.43−0.00−0.350.000.32−0.01−1.04
Self-awareness0.03 *2.820.03 **2.670.02 *2.140.03 *2.31
Self-management and motivation−0.01−0.94−0.02−1.33−0.02−1.420.000.96
Social-awareness and prosocial behaviour0.021.80−0.02 *2.220.021.580.021.06
Decision-making−0.07−0.560.00−1.42−0.00−0.320.000.30
Affective empathy0.01 *2.130.001.170.011.680.01 *2.32
Cognitive Affective0.000.130.000.16−0.00−0.180.000.19
Age0.26 **9.790.20 **7.310.22 **7.790.36 **9.80
Sex−0.14 *−2.88−0.14 **−2.74−0.14 *−2.68−0.16 *−2.29
Ethnic-cultural group−0.27 **−3.85−0.23 **−3.18−0.23 *−3.12−0.37 **−3.47
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Linear regression analyses with moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, socio-emotional competencies, empathy, and personal variables (age, gender, ethnic-cultural group membership, and intervention group) as longitudinal predictors of literacy competence (n = 145).
Table 4. Linear regression analyses with moral emotions, online empathy, anger management, socio-emotional competencies, empathy, and personal variables (age, gender, ethnic-cultural group membership, and intervention group) as longitudinal predictors of literacy competence (n = 145).
Literacy CompetenceListeningReadingWriting
ΒtβtβtΒt
Moral emotions0.140.910.050.940.020.490.071.11
Online affective empathy−0.13−1.05−0.04−1.06−0.05−1.29−0.04−0.75
Online cognitive empathy−0.160.98−0.06−1.45−0.05−1.18−0.06−0.98
Anger management0.040.400.020.630.020.78−0.00−0.01
Self-awareness0.32 *2.130.11 *2.250.10 *2.190.111.79
Self-management and motivation−0.09−0.59−0.03−0.60−0.03−0.53−0.04−0.56
Social-awareness and prosocial behaviour−0.05−0.38−0.02−0.55−0.02−0.59−0.00−0.05
Decision-making−0.25−1.75−0.08−1.91−0.08−1.90−0.08−1.34
Affective empathy0.071.530.021.390.031.94−0.021.17
Cognitive empahy0.080.980.031.010.030.350.030.88
Age1.17 **3.380.44 **4.180.45 **4.190.281.89
Sex−0.55−0.84−0.05−0.22−0.10−0.52−0.40−1.44
Ethnic-cultural group−2.68−1.66−0.56−1.12−0.86−1.71−1.27−1.84
Intervention group2.56 **−1.660.79 **4.020.82 **4.180.95 **3.50
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 5. Differences in pre-and post-test scores in moral emotions, anger management, and online empathy in the control and experimental groups.
Table 5. Differences in pre-and post-test scores in moral emotions, anger management, and online empathy in the control and experimental groups.
Control
(n = 71)
Experimental
(n = 74)
Pre-Test
M (SD)
Post-Test
M (SD)
Pre-Test
M (SD)
Post-Test
M (SD)
Differences between the Pre-Tests
d (IC)
Differences between the Post-Tests
d (IC)
Differences between the Pre- and Post-Tests F(p)
Moral Emotions22.52 (2.38)21.26 (4.04)22.63 (2.54)22.83 (2.36)0.03
(−0.30, 0.35)
0.48
(0.15, 0.82)
6.70
(0.01) *
Online Affective Empathy8.55 (3.24)7.61 (2.65)8.97 (2.82)8.52 (2.68)0.14
(−0.19, 0.47)
0.39
(0.04, 0.73)
0.56
(0.46)
Online Cognitive Empathy12.60 (2.54)12.84 (2.20)13.13 (2.63)13.40 (2.65)0.14
(−0.20, 0.47)
0.12
(−0.23, 0.48)
<0.01
(0.96)
Online Empathy21.26 (4.76)20.53 (3.85)22.06 (4.28)21.88 (4.39)0.17
(−0.16, 0.51)
0.06
(−0.30, 0.41)
0.29
(0.59)
Anger management13.73 (3.82)12.67 (4.08)12.68 (3.99)13.03 (4.26)−0.21
(−0.54, 0.011)
0.09
(−0.24, 0.41)
4.10
(0.045) *
* p < 0.05.
Table 6. Differences in pre-and post-test scores in moral emotions, anger management, and online empathy in boys.
Table 6. Differences in pre-and post-test scores in moral emotions, anger management, and online empathy in boys.
ControlExperimental
Pre-Test
M (SD)
Post-Test
M (SD)
Pre-Test
M (SD)
Post-Test
M (SD)
Differences between the Pre-Tests
d (IC)
Differences between the Pre- and Post-Tests F(p)
Moral Emotions22.22 (2.49)21.29 (3.48)21.67 (2.83)21.97 (2.69)0.21 (−0.27, 0.68)2.21 (0.14)
Online Affective Empathy8.94 (3.22)7.87 (2.83)9.36 (2.728.73 (2.65)−0.14 (−0.62, 033)0.23 (0.64)
Online Cognitive Empathy13.10 (2.58)13.28 (2.37)13.23 (2.66)13.64 (2.76)−0.05 (−0.54, 0.44)0.17 (68)
Online Empathy22.23 (4.89)21.21 (4.41)22.21 (4.48)22.28 (4.42)−0.07 (−0.56, 0.42)0.09 (0.76)
Anger management13.12 (4.02)12.03 (3.90)12.87 (4.06)12.58 (4.22)0.28 (−0.20, 0.75)1.36 (0.25)
Table 7. Differences in pre-and post-test scores in moral emotions, anger management, and online empathy in girls.
Table 7. Differences in pre-and post-test scores in moral emotions, anger management, and online empathy in girls.
Control (n = 37)Experimental
(n = 38)
Pre-Test
M (SD)
Post-Test
M (SD)
Pre-Test
M (SD)
Post-Test
M (SD)
Differences between the Pre-Tests
d (IC)
Differences between the Pre- and Post-Tests F(p)
Moral Emotions22.68 (2.29)21.24 (4.45)23.34 (2.22)23.61 (1.70)−0.30 (−0.75, 0.1594)4.39 (0.04) *
Online Affective Empathy8.08 (3.19)7.35 (2.48)8.44 (2.90)8.53 (2.93)−0.12 (−0.58, 0.34)0.35 (0.56)
Online Cognitive Empathy12.19 (2.49)12.41 (2.11)12.68 (2.73)12.50 (2.77)−0.19 (−0.65, 0.28)0.16 (0.69)
Online Empathy20.32 (4.73)19.83 (3.37)21.29 (4.50)20.92 (4.42)−0.21 (−0.68, 0.26)0.18 (0.67)
Anger management13.87 (3.95)12.50 (4.14)13.29 (4.04)13.45 (4.31)0.15 (−0.31, 0.60)2.70 (0.10)
* p < 0.05.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

González-Gómez, A.L.; Farrington, D.P.; Llorent, V.J. Descriptive and Quasi-Experimental Studies about Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, Anger Management, and Their Relations with Key Competencies in Primary Education. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11584. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111584

AMA Style

González-Gómez AL, Farrington DP, Llorent VJ. Descriptive and Quasi-Experimental Studies about Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, Anger Management, and Their Relations with Key Competencies in Primary Education. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(21):11584. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111584

Chicago/Turabian Style

González-Gómez, Antonio L., David P. Farrington, and Vicente J. Llorent. 2021. "Descriptive and Quasi-Experimental Studies about Moral Emotions, Online Empathy, Anger Management, and Their Relations with Key Competencies in Primary Education" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 21: 11584. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111584

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop