Next Article in Journal
Measuring Problematic Facebook Use among Adolescents and Young Adults with the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale: A Psychometric Analysis by Applying Item Response Theory
Previous Article in Journal
Special Issue on Mental Health and Well-Being in Adolescence: Environment and Behavior
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support in Late Childhood: A Latent Growth Modeling Approach

1
School of Psychology, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, and Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
2
School of Management, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei 230026, China
3
Department of Psychology, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, China
4
School of Management, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361005, China
5
Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(6), 2978; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18062978
Submission received: 1 March 2021 / Accepted: 10 March 2021 / Published: 14 March 2021

Abstract

:
This study explored the change trajectory of schoolchildren’s ego-resiliency and perceived social support and investigated the effect of perceived social support on ego-resiliency across four time points. A sample of 437 children aged 8–13 years (M = 10.99, SD = 0.70, 51.5% boys) completed assessments at four time points. The results indicated that ego-resiliency showed an increasing linear trend and perceived social support showed a declining linear trend. Perceived social support had a positive effect on ego-resiliency over time. In addition, the initial status of perceived social support negatively predicted the growth trend of ego-resiliency, and the initial status of ego-resiliency negatively predicted the declining trend of perceived social support. The implications for theory and practice are discussed.

1. Introduction

Ego-resiliency is the dynamic capacity to modify one’s own level of ego-control according to situational contexts [1]. Ego-control refers to the “threshold or operating characteristic of an individual with regard to the expression or containment of impulses, feelings, and desires” [2]. Both ego-resiliency and ego-control are key factors in understanding motivation, emotion, and behavior [1,3]. Individuals high in ego-resiliency easily change their level of ego-control, adapt to changing situations, and perform flexible problem-solving strategies, while individuals low in ego-resiliency have difficulty modifying their level of ego-control, tend to show anxiety, and show little adaptive flexibility [2,4,5,6].
A large body of research has explored the development of ego-resiliency, and most of the existing research has focused on particular age stages. Among them, Block and Block [4] examined the development of ego-resiliency from toddlerhood to young adulthood. Chuang, Lamb, and Hwang [7] studied ego-resiliency’s development from early childhood to adolescence. Some studies have focused on the development of ego-resiliency during the toddlerhood and preschool periods [8,9], some have focused on early childhood [10,11], and others have focused on the period from late adolescence to young adulthood [12,13]. In the present study, we focused on late childhood, which is the transition period in psychological development from childhood to early adolescence [14]. Children in the transition period are vulnerable to negative events. Ego-resiliency has been shown to play a crucial role during the transition period, which provides the resources necessary to self-regulate academic behaviors [15]. Ego-resiliency has also been associated with social competence [9,16] and low levels of internalizing and externalizing behaviors [7,17]. Therefore, exploration of the development of ego-resiliency during this period will be beneficial to prevention and intervention work. This present study examines the change trajectory of ego-resiliency and perceived social support during late childhood and explores the potential relationships between perceived social support and ego-resiliency across four time points during late childhood.

1.1. Change Trajectory of Ego-Resiliency

Many researchers have revealed the different developmental trends of ego-resiliency. For example, Taylor et al. [9] found ego-resiliency’s growth trend in children from the ages of three and a half to seven. Chuang and colleagues [7] found a declining trend of ego-resiliency between the ages of seven and eight. Alessandri et al. [12] found that ego-resiliency’s development was stable until the end of high school, and later, an increasing trend from the ages of 19 to 25 was observed. These findings suggest that ego-resiliency shows particular change tendencies at different age stages. Developmental transitions may be the sources of changes in ego-resiliency [2] because individuals have to adapt to changes in their environment and social expectations [15]. The period from late childhood to early adolescence (approximately ages 9 to 11) is an important transition in cognitive and emotional development [18,19]. Ego-resiliency is assumed to be affected by other personality characteristics and learning, such as emotionality and coping skills [8]. As a personality characteristic, effortful control has an important impact on ego-resiliency. Effortful control refers to “the efficiency of executive attention, including the ability to inhibit a dominant response and/or to activate a subdominant response, to plan, and to detect errors” [20], and is closely related to ego-resiliency. Previous studies have found that effortful control has a positive effect on ego-resiliency [17,21,22]. In the early years of life, effortful control develops quickly [17,20], which may foster the development of ego-resiliency. However, with the gradual maturation of cognitive and emotional abilities from late childhood to early adolescence, children may have increased ego-resiliency [15]. Previous research has shown that children can easily gain the resources to cope with stressors from family during middle childhood [23]. Some researchers found that the use of problem-solving skills showed an increasing trend during the period from childhood to early adolescence [18,24], which may foster the development of ego-resiliency. Therefore, we propose that during the time period of this study, the trajectory of the children’s ego-resiliency will show an increasing trend (hypothesis 1).

1.2. Change Trajectory of Perceived Social Support

Two major aspects of social support are received and perceived social support. Received social support refers to the actual support behaviors provided by networks. Perceived social support is defined as the subjective perceptions or beliefs that one can receive social support from others when necessary. Individuals with high perceived social support are prone to seek external resources [25]. Compared with received social support, perceived social support has been regarded as having a more consistent relationship with social adaptation [26,27].
Perceived social support is a potentially important factor during childhood, a period of social vulnerability and mental reliance in multiple settings (e.g., family, school, and peer groups). Research on children’s perceived social support typically focuses on emotional support, informative support or advice, instrumental or material support, and social companionship [28]. Different supportive relationships provide different kinds of social support [29]. Researchers have demonstrated that children have a well-differentiated sense of their social support network [30]. They know that they have distinctive social needs and that different types of support can be met by different people [31]. Children perceive their mothers as the best available multipurpose social support provider compared to friends and teachers. Friends are perceived as the best source of companionship support and emotional support. Teachers are perceived as good providers of informational support or advice. Fathers are also rated as excellent sources of informational support.
Individuals seek specific types of social support in their relationships with others [32]. For example, schoolchildren experience a shift in life and learning from family to school. The size of the children’s social support network becomes larger than before, and the available supportive behaviors from the network also change. As children move into middle childhood, parent–child attachment remains critical for children’s development. In particular, the availability of parents becomes more important than proximity [33]. Availability refers to open communication between the parent and child and the parents’ responses to the child’s needs [34]. In addition to social support from family, children spend the most time in school during middle childhood. Teachers and peers are important components of children’s social relationships. Teachers play a powerful and influential role in children’s lives [35]. Teacher–child relationships provide a resource for children’s development [36]. A supportive teacher–child relationship will help children receive a good learning experience [37]. Additionally, peer relationships are salient during the adolescent years [30]. Positive peer relationships play a great role in children’s development [38]. Children have been shown to perceive the same amount of social support from parents and friends in late childhood [39].
Considering changes in perceived social support sources, such as attachment relationships with their parents, teacher–child relationships, and peer relationships, we propose that during the time period of this study, the trajectory of children’s perceived social support will show an increasing trend (hypothesis 2).

1.3. Reciprocal Relationships between Perceived Social Support and Ego-Resiliency

Ego-resiliency as a dynamic capacity can be acquired and shaped over time in response to environmental demands [40]. According to the ecological systems theory [41], family and school are microsystems that have a direct effect on children’s development. Because the scope of children’s lives is mainly family and school, ego-resiliency may be shaped by protective factors from family and school. On the one hand, parental support behaviors can meet children’s psychological needs for autonomy and competence [42]. Existing evidence has shown that perceived supportive parental behaviors are positively related to ego-resiliency [43,44,45]. On the other hand, research has indicated that positive peer and teacher–student relationships play vital roles in fostering resilience [35]. With increased interactions with teachers and peers, children may develop increasingly resilient behaviors by modeling and internalizing adaptive behaviors, such as problem-solving skills [35]. The existing literature suggests that received social support is positively related to resiliency. We thus infer that perceived social support may be positively associated with ego-resiliency. As previously mentioned, perceived social support has a more important effect on children’s social adaptation. When facing difficulties, children with a high level of perceived social support believe that they can receive social support from others. These beliefs may help them gain more confidence to solve problems. Even without the help of others, they may succeed in solving problems. Therefore, perceived social support may be positively related to ego-resiliency. However, the level of perceived social support relies on actual support behaviors from others. That is, when children possess enough actual social support, perceived social support may promote the development of ego-resiliency.
Ego-resiliency, as a personality trait [2], may influence perceived social support. Personality traits affect perceived social support by influencing how perceived supportive behaviors are received, eliciting different responses from others and fostering individuals’ positive behaviors in accordance with their personalities [46]. Compared with ego-brittle children, ego-resilient children, typified by a high-level of confidence and optimism, easily elicit a supportive response from others [47]. Ego-resilient children have positive relations with others [48]. In the face of challenging or threatening circumstances, they are likely to seek out social resources [49]. Researchers have found that ego-resiliency predicts perceived social support [50]. Therefore, it further suggests that ego-resiliency may be positively related to perceived social support.
Exploring bidirectional longitudinal relationships between ego-resiliency and perceived social support is particularly worthwhile because, to our knowledge, there has been no research done on such relationships during late childhood. Hill et al. [32] found that the reciprocal relationships between the initial levels of the Big Five personality traits effect different aspects and changes in perceived social support in adulthood. It is possible that other personality characteristics and perceived social support interact with each other. Therefore, we predict that perceived social support may be positively associated with ego-resiliency at every time point, that the initial status of perceived social support at Time 1 may be positively associated with the rate of increasing ego-resiliency, and that the initial status of ego-resiliency at Time 1 may be positively associated with the rate of increasing perceived social support (hypothesis 3).
In the present research, we investigated relationships between ego-resiliency and perceived social support using a four-wave longitudinal design. First, we examined the change trajectory of ego-resiliency and perceived social support across four time points. Then, we examined how perceived social support would be related to ego-resiliency.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A group of primary school students in Guangdong Province, China, participated in the present study. Cluster random sampling was used. Data were first collected in December 2015 (Time 1 (T1)), and the data from other waves (Time 2 (T2), Time 3 (T3), and Time 4 (T4)) were collected every 6 months. A total of 681 students finished self-report questionnaires at T1. Of those, we received 617 usable questionnaires. Among them, 47.8% were in year 3, and 52.2% were boys (Mage = 8.87 years, SD = 0.71). A total of 591 of the T1 students finished the survey at T2 (96%), 541 of the T2 students finished the survey at T3 (92%), and 437 of the T3 students finished the survey at T4 (81%). Attrition was mainly because (1) some participants had transferred to another school and (2) some students were absent from school during the assessment. We compared students who completed measures across all waves with students who dropped out of at least one wave. There was no significant difference in ego-resiliency (t = −0.836, p > 0.05), perceived social support (t = 1.013, p > 0.05), or gender (χ2 = 0.295, p > 0.05). The missing completely at random (MCAR) test was used to clarify the trend of the missing data [51]. The result was not significant (χ2/df = 1, p > 0.05), suggesting that the data were missing at random. The final sample of 437 participants had 51.5% boys, and 33.2% were in year 4 (Mage = 10.99 years, SD = 0.70).

2.2. Procedure

Before conducting the study, we obtained approval from the Research Ethics Committee of the respective university. All participants and their parents provided consent forms before completing each assessment across the four time points. Trained teachers conducted every survey in the participants’ classrooms. Participants were instructed to complete all the measures and then provided their demographic information (i.e., age, gender, and grade) at every time point. At the beginning of each assessment, the trained teachers read and explained the standardized instructions to guide students to complete the assessments. The participants were told that the data were collected and analyzed anonymously. The students could take as long as they needed to finish the survey.

2.3. Measures

Ego-resiliency. Ego-resiliency was measured by the ego-resiliency scale [47], which consists of fourteen items. Responses were given on a five-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). An example item is “I quickly get over and recover from being startled”. The coefficient alpha at every time point was 0.71, 0.76, 0.78, and 0.80.
Perceived social support. Perceived social support was measured by the adapted version [52] of the perceived social support scale [53]. It consisted of twelve items, and an example item is “My family is willing to help me make decisions”. This scale was scored on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). It included three dimensions: family support, support from friends, and support from other significant people. Here, we aggregated all item scores to form a composite score. The values of the coefficient alpha of the whole scale at every time point were 0.80, 0.85, 0.85, and 0.89.

2.4. Analytic Strategies

We used latent growth modelling (LGM) to model the change trajectories of ego-resiliency and perceived social support [54]. First, we selected an optimal model from the considered models (including linear and quadratic growth models). For the linear growth model, the factor loading of the latent intercept factors was set to 1 to reflect the initial status. The factor loading of the latent slope factors was fixed at 0, 1, 2, and 3 for four repeated measures, representing linear growth. For the quadratic growth model, in addition to intercept factors and linear slope factors, quadratic slope factors were fixed to the square values of linear slope factors [55]. Then, based on the best-fitted model, we considered the effect of time-varying covariates (i.e., perceived social support) on the change trajectory of ego-resiliency. Finally, we specified a parallel process latent growth model to test the relationship between the change trend of perceived social support and the change trend of ego-resiliency [55,56]. The parallel process latent growth model has been widely applied in many areas of psychology [57,58,59,60].
Descriptive statistics and correlations for all study variables were calculated using SPSS Version 21.0. All latent growth models were conducted using Mplus Version 7.4. Model fit was evaluated by five fit indices: chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio (χ2/df), the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). A χ2/df < 3, a CFI and TLI of > 0.90, and an RMSEA and SRMR of < 0.08 are considered indications of good model fit [61].

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among all observed variables are presented in Table 1. The results indicated that ego-resiliency was positively correlated with perceived social support from T1 to T4. Given the significant correlations between gender and perceived social support, we used gender as a control variable in the latent growth model.

3.2. Latent Growth Models for Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support

Table 2 summarizes the model fit indices of latent growth models for ego-resiliency and perceived social support. Table 3 provides parameter estimates of latent growth models for ego-resiliency and perceived social support. For ego-resiliency, both the linear growth model and the quadratic growth model fit the data very well (for the linear growth model, χ2(df) = 13.454(5), χ2/df = 2.691, CFI = 0.985, TLI = 0.982, RMSEA = 0.062, and SRMR = 0.044; for the quadratic growth model, χ2(df) = 3.009(1), χ2/df = 3.009, CFI = 0.996, TLI = 0.978, RMSEA = 0.068, and SRMR = 0.016). Because the linear growth model is nested under the quadratic growth model [54], we compared ego-resiliency’s linear growth model and quadratic growth model by using the chi-square test. The result was significant (Δχ2 = 10.45, Δdf = 4, and p = 0.03). Although the linear growth model and quadratic growth model for ego-resiliency were significantly different, the slope factors of the quadratic growth model were not statistically significant, so we used the linear growth model of ego-resiliency. The rate of change (slope factor) was positive and significant (μS = 0.35, p < 0.05), suggesting that ego-resiliency increased linearly across the time period of this study. Therefore, hypothesis 1 was confirmed. The slope factor variances were statistically significant (σ2S = 6.31, p < 0.001), indicating that there were individual differences in the rate of change. The intercept factor variances were statistically significant (σ2I = 41.84, p < 0.001), revealing that there were individual differences in initial status at Time 1. Finally, the factor covariance between the intercept and the slope for ego-resiliency was negative and statistically significant (σIS = −0.40, p < 0.001), which indicates that students who had a higher level of ego-resiliency at Time 1 increased in ego-resiliency at much slower rates over time than those with a lower level at Time 1.
For perceived social support, the fit indices of both the linear and quadratic growth models were acceptable (for the linear growth model, χ2(df) = 21.018(7), χ2/df = 3, CFI = 0.975, TLI = 0.964, RMSEA = 0.068, and SRMR = 0.085; for the quadratic growth model, χ2(df) = 3.899(2), χ2/df = 1.950, CFI = 0.997, TLI = 0.983, RMSEA = 0.047, and SRMR = 0.011). In the quadratic growth model of perceived social support, a nonpositive definite problem appeared in the latent variable covariance matrix. The quadratic slope of the quadratic growth model was statistically nonsignificant, so we retained the linear growth model of perceived social support. The rate of change (slope factor) was negative and significant (μS = −0.81, p < 0.001), suggesting that perceived social support decreased linearly across the time period of this study. Hypothesis 2 was not supported. The slope factor variances were statistically significant (σ2S = 6.58, p < 0.001), indicating that there were individual differences in the rate of change. The intercept factor variances were statistically significant (σ2I = 40.70, p < 0.001), revealing that there were individual differences in the initial statuses at Time 1. In addition, gender was negatively related to the intercept (initial status) of perceived social support (γ = −0.16, p < 0.01), showing that girls perceived more support than boys at Time 1. Finally, the factor covariance between the intercept and the slope for perceived social support was negative and statistically significant (σIS = −0.33, p < 0.001), which indicates that students who had a higher level of perceived social support at Time 1 experienced smaller decreases in perceived social support over time than those with a lower level at Time 1.

3.3. Linear Growth Model for Ego-Resiliency with a Time-Varying Covariate

Based on a linear growth model for ego-resiliency, we tested the roles of perceived social support on ego-resiliency at every time point. The model showed acceptable fit (χ2(df) = 53.535(17), χ2/df = 3.149, CFI = 0.958, TLI = 0.946, RMSEA = 0.070, and SRMR = 0.092). Perceived social support was positively related to ego-resiliency at every repeated measure (T1: γ = 0.38, p < 0.001; T2: γ = 0.40, p < 0.001; T3: γ = 0.37, p < 0.001; T4: γ = 0.37, p < 0.001), which suggests that perceived social support had a stable influence on ego-resiliency.

3.4. Parallel Process Latent Growth Model for Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support

On the basis of a linear growth model for ego-resiliency and perceived social support, we specified a parallel process latent growth model. Figure 1 provides the results of the parameter estimates. The model had acceptable fit (χ2(df) = 127.740(24), χ2/df = 5.323, CFI = 0.927, TLI = 0.915, RMSEA = 0.099, and SRMR = 0.079). The intercept factor of perceived social support was negatively related to the slope factor of ego-resiliency (γ = −0.14, p < 0.001), which shows that students who perceived a higher level of social support at Time 1 increased in ego-resiliency at much slower rates over time. The intercept factor of ego-resiliency was negatively related to the slope factor of perceived social support (γ = −0.13, p < 0.001), which indicates that students who had a higher level of ego-resiliency at Time 1 decreased in perceived social support at much faster rates over time. Based on the results above, hypothesis 3 was partly supported.

4. Discussion

The present study explored the change trajectory of ego-resiliency and perceived social support across four time points, as well as the positive effect of perceived social support on ego-resiliency. We also examined the reciprocity between ego-resiliency and perceived social support. School children’s ego-resiliency increased during the time period of the current study, while perceived social support decreased. However, perceived social support had a positive influence on ego-resiliency at every time point. Furthermore, the initial status of ego-resiliency negatively predicted the declining trend of perceived social support, and the initial status of perceived social support negatively predicted the growth trend of ego-resiliency.
As we hypothesized, ego-resiliency showed a growth trend over time. As an adaptation ability, ego-resiliency showed an increasing trend in late childhood. This suggests that the developmental trajectories in different developmental stages may have unique patterns. For example, Alessandri et al. [12] found that the developmental window of ego-resiliency was age 19. We found that ego-resiliency significantly changed over time, at least during late childhood. For preadolescents, cognitive and emotional competences were gradually developing to cope with the challenges in their lives. Their frequent interactions with teachers and peers in school may promote their ability to solve a variety of problems. Therefore, their ego-resiliency may increase with age.
However, perceived social support showed a declining trend, which was inconsistent with our hypothesis. One recent study also demonstrated that children exhibited lower support seeking tendencies in late childhood [62]. At the end of primary school, students experience the transition from childhood to adolescence, characterized by independence and autonomy. In addition, during middle childhood, children often face an imbalance between unrealistic expectations of success and real ability levels, and receive more failure feedback [14]. This may lead them to becoming more independent and having a lower subjective perception of social network support. Consequently, they may feel less social support from others. In addition, the difference in perceived social support across genders is consistent with previous findings [63]. Girls are more likely to seek social support than boys [64]. Compared to boys, girls are more likely to show self-disclosure and emotional expressiveness [65], which may enhance social interactions where girls are more willing to seek help and perceive more social support than boys.
Our findings indicated that perceived social support predicted ego-resiliency. Thus, hypothesis 3 was partly supported. During late childhood, children spend most of their time with family and at school. Although they perceived lower levels of social support from family and school, social support still played an important role in their development of ego-resiliency. Children can readily gain external help from parents, teachers, and peers when facing troubles. Protection from immediate family members can foster ego-resiliency [2]. In addition, support from teachers and peers may be associated with ego-resiliency [35]. Our findings confirmed the protective role of perceived social support on ego-resiliency. In addition, we found that the initial status of ego-resiliency increased the decline rate of perceived social support. Highly resilient individuals often have a high level of self-confidence and good psychological adjustment [47]. Ego-resilient children possess abundant interpersonal skills [2], which may play a crucial role in gaining social support. Ego-resilient children may perceive declining social support due to their high level of self-confidence in gaining social support. Interestingly, the initial status of perceived social support inhibited the growth rate of ego-resiliency. This means that excessive social support is not always beneficial to the development of ego-resiliency, especially during late middle childhood. Interpersonal relationships play a dual role in adolescents; they not only provide social support but also bring stressors, such as parents’ attention to academic performance and acceptance by peers [66]. Social support that meets recipients’ actual needs may be more practical and acceptable. In addition to received and perceived social support, there are different kinds of classifications. As noted above, Cohen and Wills [28] divided social support into four categories: emotional support, social companionship, instrumental support, and informative support. Different kinds of social support may play different roles in ego-resiliency. Camara et al. [66] found that emotional support is the most important support for adolescents when facing stress events. The effects of different types of social support on ego-resiliency should be explored in the future.
This study has several limitations that need to be addressed in future research. First, although we used a longitudinal design to explore the causal relations between ego-resiliency and perceived social support, change in the short-term is easily affected by unexpected factors. Future research should attempt to use a more long-term longitudinal design to gain clear causality. For example, studies exploring the whole primary school stage may provide more information about the developmental trajectory. Second, self-report measures were used in the current study. This may increase common method variance. Therefore, future research should also include other sources of ratings, such as parents, teachers, and peers. Finally, we focused on only one protective factor for ego-resiliency. There are multiple factors that may promote ego-resiliency, including family factors such as parenting styles [22] and children’s individual factors such as the Big Five personality traits [67].

5. Conclusions

By using a latent growth modelling approach, we found a linear increasing change in ego-resiliency and a linear declining change in perceived social support. Perceived social support had a positive effect on ego-resiliency at every time point. The initial status of perceived social support negatively predicted the increasing rate of ego-resiliency. The initial status of ego-resiliency negatively predicted the declining rate of perceived social support. This research suggests that children’s ego-resiliency may be influenced by perceived social support during late childhood when it is often prone to decline.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Q.C. and W.G.; methodology, Q.C. and W.G.; software, W.G.; validation, Q.C., W.G., B.-B.C., and Y.K.; formal analysis, Q.C.; investigation, Q.C. and W.G.; data curation, Q.C. and W.G.; writing—original draft preparation Q.C. and W.G.; writing—review and editing, B.-B.C., Y.K., L.L., and S.Y.; visualization, Q.C. and W.G.; supervision, Q.C.; project administration, Q.C.; funding acquisition, Q.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31771245), 13th Five-Year Plan Research Project of Philosophy and Social Science in Guangdong, China (GD17XXL02), Innovation Project of Educational Science in Guangdong, China (2016GXJK032).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of South China Normal University (protocol code SCNU-PSY-334, 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Letziring, T.; Block, J.; Funder, D. Ego-control and ego-resiliency: Generalization of self-report scales based on personality description from acquaintances, clinicians, and the self. J. Res. Personal. 2004, 39, 395–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Block, J.H.; Block, J. The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior. In Development of Cognition, Affect, and Social Relations, the Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology; Collins, W.A., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1980; Volume 13, pp. 39–101. [Google Scholar]
  3. Causadias, J.M.; Salvatore, J.E.; Sroufe, L.A. Early patterns of self-regulation as risk and promotive factors in development: A longitudinal study from childhood to adulthood in a high-risk sample. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2012, 36, 293–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Block, J.; Block, J.H. Venturing a 30-year longitudinal study. Am. Psychol. 2006, 61, 315–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Eisenberg, N.; Haugen, R.; Spinrad, T.L.; Hofer, C.; Chassin, L.; Zhou, Q.; Kupfer, A.; Smith, C.L.; Valiente, C.; Liew, J. Relations of Temperament to Maladjustment and Ego Resiliency in At-risk Children. Soc. Dev. 2009, 19, 577–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Taylor, Z.E.; Ruiz, Y.; Nair, N. A mixed-method examination of ego-resiliency, adjustment problems, and academic engagement in children of Latino migrant farmworkers. Soc. Dev. 2018, 28, 200–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chuang, S.S.; Lamb, M.E.; Hwang, C.P. Personality development from childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal study of ego-control and ego-resiliency in Sweden. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2006, 30, 338–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Taylor, Z.E.; Eisenberg, N.; Spinrad, T.L.; Eggum, N.D.; Sulik, M.J. The relations of ego-resiliency and emotion socialization to the development of empathy and prosocial behavior across early childhood. Emotion 2013, 13, 822–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Taylor, Z.E.; Eisenberg, N.; Vanschyndel, S.K.; Eggum-Wilkens, N.D.; Spinrad, T.L. Children’s negative emotions and ego-resiliency: Longitudinal relations with social competence. Emotion 2014, 14, 397–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Liew, J.; Cao, Q.; Hughes, J.N.; Deutz, M.H.F. Academic Resilience Despite Early Academic Adversity: A Three-Wave Longitudinal Study on Regulation-Related Resiliency, Interpersonal Relationships, and Achievement in First to Third Grade. Early Educ. Dev. 2018, 29, 762–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Liew, J.; McTigue, E.M.; Barrois, L.; Hughes, J.N. Adaptive and effortful control and academic self-efficacy beliefs on achievement: A longitudinal study of 1st through 3rd graders. Early Child. Res. Q. 2008, 23, 515–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Alessandri, G.; Eisenberg, N.; Vecchione, M.; Caprara, G.V.; Milioni, M. Ego-resiliency development from late adolescence to emerging adulthood: A ten-year longitudinal study. J. Adolesc. 2016, 50, 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Vecchione, M.; Alessandri, G.; Barbaranelli, C.; Gerbino, M. Stability and change of ego resiliency from late adolescence to young adulthood: A multiperspective study using the ER89-R Scale. J. Personal. Assess. 2010, 92, 212–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Eccles, J.S. The Development of Children Ages 6 to 14. Future Child. 1999, 9, 30–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Alessandri, G.; Zuffianò, A.; Eisenberg, N.; Pastorelli, C. The role of ego-resiliency as mediator of the longitudinal relationship between family socio-economic status and school grades. J. Youth Adolesc. 2017, 46, 2157–2168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Spinrad, T.L.; Eisenberg, N.; Cumberland, A.; Fabes, R.A.; Valiente, C.; Shepard, S.A.; Reiser, M.; Losoya, S.H.; Guthrie, I.K. Relation of emotion-related regulation to children’s social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion 2006, 6, 498–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Hofer, C.; Eisenberg, N.; Reiser, M. The Role of Socialization, Effortful Control, and Ego Resiliency in French Adolescents’ Social Functioning. J. Res. Adolesc. 2010, 20, 555–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Eschenbeck, H.; Schmid, S.; Schröder, I.; Wasserfall, N.; Kohlmann, C.-W. Development of coping strategies from childhood to adolescence. Eur. J. Health Psychol. 2018, 25, 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J.; Skinner, E.A. Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2010, 35, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Rothbart, M.K.; Bates, J.E. Temperament. In Handbook of Child Psychology, Volume 3—Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 6th ed.; Eisenberg, N., Damon, V., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 99–166. [Google Scholar]
  21. Eisenberg, N.; Valiente, C.; Fabes, R.A.; Smith, C.L.; Reiser, M.; Shepard, S.A.; Losoya, S.H.; Guthrie, I.K.; Murphy, B.C.; Cumberland, A.J. The relations of effortful control and ego control to children’s resiliency and social functioning. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 761–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Taylor, Z.E.; Eisenberg, N.; Spinrad, T.L.; Widaman, K.F. Longitudinal Relations of Intrusive Parenting and Effortful Control to Ego-Resiliency During Early Childhood. Child Dev. 2013, 84, 1145–1151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Rudolph, K.D.; Dennig, M.D.; Weisz, J.R. Determinants and consequences of children’s coping in the medical setting: Conceptualization, review, and critique. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 118, 328–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Vierhaus, M.; Lohaus, A. Children’s Perception of Relations between Anger or Anxiety and Coping: Continuity and Discontinuity of Relational Structures. Soc. Dev. 2009, 18, 747–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Johnson, J.H.; Gooding, P.; Tarrier, N. Suicide risk in schizophrenia: Explanatory models and clinical implications, The Schematic Appraisal Model of Suicide (SAMS). Psychol. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. 2008, 81, 55–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Melrose, K.L.; Brown, G.D.; Wood, A.M. When is received social support related to perceived support and well-being? When it is needed. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015, 77, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Vaingankar, J.A.; Abdin, E.; Chong, S.A. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support in patients with schizophrenia. Compr. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 286–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cohen, S.; Wills, T.A. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol. Bull. 1985, 98, 310–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cantin, S.; Boivin, M. Change and stability in children’s social network and self-perceptions during transition from elementary to junior high school. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2004, 28, 561–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ross, A.C.; Simons, L.E.; Feinstein, A.B.; Yoon, M.I.A.; Bhandari, R.P. Social Risk and Resilience Factors in Adolescent Chronic Pain: Examining the Role of Parents and Peers. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2017, 43, 303–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Shute, R.; De Blasio, T.; Williamson, P. Social support satisfaction of Australian children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2002, 26, 318–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hill, P.L.; Weston, S.J.; Jackson, J.J. The co-development of perceived support and the Big Five in middle and older adulthood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017, 42, 26–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kerns, K.A. Attachment in middle childhood. In Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd ed.; Cassidy, J., Shaver, P.R., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 366–382. [Google Scholar]
  34. Lieberman, M.; Doyle, A.-B.; Markiewicz, D. Developmental Patterns in Security of Attachment to Mother and Father in Late Childhood and Early Adolescence: Associations with Peer Relations. Child Dev. 1999, 70, 202–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Noble, T.; McGrath, H. Wellbeing and Resilience in Young People and the Role of Positive Relationships. In Positive Relationships: Evidence Based Practice across the World; Roffey, S., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 17–33. [Google Scholar]
  36. Eccles, J.S.; Midgley, C.; Wigfield, A.; Buchanan, C.M.; Reuman, D.; Flanagan, C.; Mac Iver, D. Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in families. Am. Psychol. 1993, 48, 90–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Chen, B.-B.; Wiium, N.; Dimitrova, R.; Chen, N. The Relationships between Family, School and Community Support and Boundaries and Student Engagement among Chinese Adolescents. Curr. Psychol. 2017, 38, 705–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hartup, W.W. Critical issues and theoretical viewpoints. In Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups; Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W.M., Laursen, B., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 3–19. [Google Scholar]
  39. Bokhorst, C.L.; Sumter, S.R.; Westenberg, P.M. Social Support from Parents, Friends, Classmates, and Teachers in Children and Adolescents Aged 9 to 18 Years: Who Is Perceived as Most Supportive? Soc. Dev. 2010, 19, 417–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Philippe, F.L.; LaVenture, S.; Beaulieu-Pelletier, G.; Lecours, S.; Lekes, N. Ego-Resiliency as a Mediator between Childhood Trauma and Psychological Symptoms. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2011, 30, 583–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Bronfenbrenner, U. The Ecology of Human Development; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  42. Ratelle, C.F.; Morin, A.J.S.; Guay, F.; Duchesne, S. Sources of evaluation of parental behaviors as predictors of achievement outcomes. Motiv. Emot. 2018, 42, 513–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Markstrom, C.A.; Marshall, S.K.; Tryon, R.J. Resiliency, social support, and coping in rural low-income Appalachian adolescents from two racial groups. J. Adolesc. 2000, 23, 693–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Önder, A.; Dagal, A.B.; Bayindir, D. The Predictive Effect of Preschool Children’s Temperament Characteristics and Parenting Styles of Mothers on Ego Resiliency Level of Children. Educ. Sci. 2018, 43, 79–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Swanson, J.; Valiente, C.; Lemery-Chalfant, K.; O’Brien, T.C. Predicting early adolescents’ academic achievement, social competence, and physical health from parenting, ego resilience, and engagement coping. J. Early Adolesc. 2011, 31, 548–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Allemand, M.; Schaffhuser, K.; Martin, M. Long-Term Correlated Change between Personality Traits and Perceived Social Support in Middle Adulthood. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 41, 420–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Block, J.; Kremen, A.M. IQ and ego-resiliency: Conceptual and empirical connections and separateness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 349–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Klohnen, E.C. Conceptual analysis and measurement of the construct of ego-resiliency. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 1067–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mak, W.W.S.; Ng, I.S.W.; Wong, C.C.Y. Resilience: Enhancing well-being through the positive cognitive triad. J. Couns. Psychol. 2011, 58, 610–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Taylor, Z.E.; Doane, L.D.; Eisenberg, N. Transitioning from high school to college: Relations of social support, ego-resiliency, and maladjustment during emerging adulthood. Emerg. Adulthood 2014, 2, 105–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Little, R.J.; Rubin, D.B. Missing Data. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2015; Volume 15, pp. 602–607. [Google Scholar]
  52. Wang, X.; Wang, X.; Ma, H. Handbook of Mental Health Assessment Scale; Chin. Ment. Health Press: Beijing, China, 1999; pp. 131–133. [Google Scholar]
  53. Zimet, G.D.; Dahlem, N.W.; Zimet, S.G.; Farley, G.K. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. J. Pers. Assess. 1988, 52, 30–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Chan, D. The Conceptualization and Analysis of Change Over Time: An Integrative Approach Incorporating Longitudinal Mean and Covariance Structures Analysis (LMACS) and Multiple Indicator Latent Growth Modeling (MLGM). Organ. Res. Methods 1998, 1, 421–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. Mplus User’s Guide, 7th ed.; Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  56. McArdle, J.J. Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 577–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Birkeland, M.S.; Holt, T.; Ormhaug, S.M.; Jensen, T.K. Perceived social support and posttraumatic stress symptoms in children and youth in therapy: A parallel process latent growth curve model. Behav. Res. Ther. 2020, 132, 103655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Bravo, A.J.; Kelley, M.L.; Swinkels, C.M.; Ulmer, C.S. Work stressors, depressive symptoms and sleep quality among US Navy members: A parallel process latent growth modelling approach across deployment. J. Sleep Res. 2017, 27, e12624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Kieffer, M.J.; Lesaux, N.K. Development of morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in Spanish-speaking language minority learners: A parallel process latent growth curve model. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2012, 33, 23–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Selig, J.P.; Preacher, K.J. Mediation Models for Longitudinal Data in Developmental Research. Res. Hum. Dev. 2009, 6, 144–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Marsh, H.W.; Hau, K.-T.; Wen, Z. In Search of Golden Rules: Comment on Hypothesis-Testing Approaches to Setting Cutoff Values for Fit Indexes and Dangers in Overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) Findings. Struct. Equ. Model. 2004, 11, 320–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Vierhaus, M.; Lohaus, A.; Ball, J. Developmental changes in coping: Situational and methodological influences. Anxiety Stress. Coping 2007, 20, 267–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Cheng, S.-T.; Chan, A.C.M. The multidimensional scale of perceived social support: Dimensionality and age and gender differences in adolescents. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2004, 37, 1359–1369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Grotberg, E.H. Resilience programs for children in disaster. Ambul. Child Health 2001, 7, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Fehr, B. Intimacy Expectations in Same-Sex Friendships: A Prototype Interaction-Pattern Model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 265–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Camara, M.; Bacigalupe, G.; Padilla, P. The role of social support in adolescents: Are you helping me or stressing me out? Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 2017, 22, 123–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Oshio, A.; Taku, K.; Hirano, M.; Saeed, G. Resilience and Big Five personality traits: A meta-analysis. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2018, 127, 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Parallel process latent growth model for ego-resiliency and perceived social support. Notes. PSS = perceived social support, ER = ego-resiliency, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Parallel process latent growth model for ego-resiliency and perceived social support. Notes. PSS = perceived social support, ER = ego-resiliency, *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 18 02978 g001
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations.
VariablesMSD123456789
1. Gender0.51-1
2. Ego-resiliency(T1)48.018.48 −0.041
3. Ego-resiliency(T2)48.788.76 −0.020.53 **1
4. Ego-resiliency(T3)48.498.26 −0.030.33 **0.49 **1
5. Ego-resiliency(T4)49.178.37 −0.040.33 **0.49 **0.68 **1
6. Perceived Social Support(T1)45.858.24 −0.12 *0.44 **0.36 **0.18 **0.21 **1
7. Perceived Social Support(T2)44.099.09 −0.10 *0.26 **0.50 **0.36**0.34**0.51 **1
8. Perceived Social Support(T3)44.048.70 −0.11 *0.20 **0.30 **0.44 **0.38 **0.37 **0.58 **1
9. Perceived Social Support(T4)43.429.49 −0.080.21 **0.31 **0.42 **0.53 **0.29 **0.51 **0.63 **1
n = 437; T = Time; 1 = boys, 0 = girls, the mean represented the proportion of boys. * p < 0.05, and ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. Model Comparisons for Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support.
Table 2. Model Comparisons for Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support.
Modelsχ2/dfCFITLIRMSEASRMR
Ego-resiliency
Linear growth model2.6910.9850.9820.0620.044
Quadratic growth model3.0090.9960.9780.0680.016
Perceived social support
Linear growth model3.0000.9750.9640.0680.085
Quadratic growth model1.9500.9970.9830.0470.011
Table 3. Parameter Estimates of Latent Growth Models for Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support.
Table 3. Parameter Estimates of Latent Growth Models for Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support.
ModelsMeans of Growth FactorsVariances of Growth Factors
Intercept FactorLinear FactorQuadratic FactorIntercept FactorLinear FactorQuadratic Factor
Ego-resiliency
Linear growth model48.09 ***0.35 * 41.84 ***6.31 ***
Quadratic growth model48.02 ***0.350.00770.56 ***45.32 ***2.17 *
Perceived social support
Linear growth model46.55 ***−0.81 *** 40.70 ***6.58 ***
Quadratic growth model46.84 ***−1.49 *0.2358.10 ***38.83 **1.06
n = 437; gender was considered a control variable for the latent growth model of perceived social support. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, Q.; Gao, W.; Chen, B.-B.; Kong, Y.; Lu, L.; Yang, S. Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support in Late Childhood: A Latent Growth Modeling Approach. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2978. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18062978

AMA Style

Chen Q, Gao W, Chen B-B, Kong Y, Lu L, Yang S. Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support in Late Childhood: A Latent Growth Modeling Approach. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(6):2978. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18062978

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Qishan, Wenyang Gao, Bin-Bin Chen, Yurou Kong, Liuying Lu, and Shuting Yang. 2021. "Ego-Resiliency and Perceived Social Support in Late Childhood: A Latent Growth Modeling Approach" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 6: 2978. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18062978

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop