Next Article in Journal
Microbial Contamination in the Coffee Industry: An Occupational Menace besides a Food Safety Concern?
Next Article in Special Issue
Quality of Life in Breast Cancer Survivors in Relation to Age, Type of Surgery and Length of Time since First Treatment
Previous Article in Journal
Development and Validation of a Nursing Work Interruption Scale
Previous Article in Special Issue
Social Support and Acculturative Stress of International Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Contemplative Practices Behavior Is Positively Associated with Well-Being in Three Global Multi-Regional Stanford WELL for Life Cohorts

1
Stanford Prevention Research Center, Department of Medicine, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94035, USA
2
Department of Social Policy and Intervention, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 2ER, UK
3
Penumbra, Inc., Alameda, CA 94502, USA
4
Quantitative Sciences Unit, Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94035, USA
5
Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
6
Chronic Disease Research Institute, The Children’s Hospital, and National Clinical Research Center for Child Health, School of Public Health, School of Medicine, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310058, China
7
Department of Nutrition and Food Hygiene, School of Public Health, School of Medicine, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310058, China
8
School of Medicine, Data Science Center, College of Medicine Fu-Jen Catholic University, New Taipei City 24205, Taiwan
9
Department of Gastroenterology and Hepatology, E-Da Hospital, Kaohsiung City 82445, Taiwan
10
Stanford Cancer Institute, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94035, USA
11
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(20), 13485; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192013485
Submission received: 6 September 2022 / Revised: 2 October 2022 / Accepted: 5 October 2022 / Published: 18 October 2022

Abstract

:
Positive associations between well-being and a single contemplative practice (e.g., mindfulness meditation) are well documented, yet prior work may have underestimated the strength of the association by omitting consideration of multiple and/or alternative contemplative practices. Moreover, little is known about how contemplative practice behavior (CPB) impacts different dimensions of well-being. This study investigates the relationship of CPB, consisting of four discrete practices (embodied somatic-observing, non-reactive mindfulness, self-compassion, and compassion for others), with multiple dimensions of well-being. As with other canonical lifestyle behaviors, multiple contemplative practices can be integrated into one’s daily routine. Thus, it is critical to holistically consider these behaviors, extending them beyond a simple uni-dimensional measure (e.g., daily mindfulness meditation practice). We developed an integrative measure of four types of contemplative practice and found it to be significantly associated with a multi-dimensional measure of well-being. Importantly, our findings were from three large global multi-regional cohorts and compared against better-understood lifestyle behaviors (physical activity). Data were drawn from California/San Francisco Bay Area, (n = 6442), Hangzhou City (n = 10,268), and New Taipei City (n = 3033). In all three cohorts, we found statistically significant (p < 0.05) positive associations between CPB and well-being, both overall and with all of the constituent domains of well-being, comparable to or stronger than the relationship with physical activity across most well-being outcomes. These findings provide robust and cross-cultural evidence for a positive association between CPB and well-being, illuminate dimensions of well-being that could be most influenced by CPB, and suggest CPB may be useful to include as part of fundamental lifestyle recommendations for health and well-being.

1. Introduction

It is well-established that multiple health behaviors compound and intersect over the course of an individual’s day. For instance, overall daily diet is comprised of different foods consumed as meals and snacks throughout the day, and physical activity is the summation of different forms of activities, including moderate (e.g., housework, gardening, walking to work) and vigorous (e.g., lifting weights, jogging) forms. Similarly, the construct of allostatic load has been developed to emphasize the deleterious effects of cumulative physiological “wear and tear”, rather than singular stressors or behaviors [1]. In each of these constructs, the sum of behaviors may be far more critical for health outcomes and overall well-being than the individual behaviors alone.
Contemplative practices include a set of activities that quiet the striving mind, cultivate awareness, develop conscious attention modulation capabilities, promote presence, connect the individual to something larger than their own life, and develop and sustain an experience of being known/seen, safe, soothed, and secure [2]. These practices deepen and expand awareness and discernment by cultivating the capacity to bear witness to lived experience—internally, relationally, and collectively. Furthermore, the strengthening of awareness and discernment by contemplative practice facilitates the expansion of healthy engagement with greater complexity in one’s individual life and the lives of others. Positive associations between well-being and a single contemplative practice (e.g., mindfulness or compassion) are well documented [3,4,5], but the association with combined multiple contemplative practice behavior (CPB) is less understood. In this study, we hypothesize that more frequent CPB, including, but not limited to, mindfulness meditation, may be associated with greater well-being. This paper simultaneously evaluates multiple aspects of CPB, yielding a more comprehensive measure of individuals’ CPB practice overall. The study also assesses multiple dimensions of well-being, providing a unique opportunity to determine specific elements (domains) of well-being that are most likely to be impacted by CPB. Information from a specific domain of well-being and its relationship with specific CPB will help inform the design of effective targeted intervention studies in the future to promote well-being in individuals and in communities.

1.1. Introducing Contemplative Practices

There is a growing interest in contemplative practices among professionals in public health, community mental health, wellness, and medicine in addition to the enduring interest among traditional spiritual religious and/or psychological practitioners. In 2021 Davidson called for a broader approach to the study of contemplative practices. He stated “I will conclude with a plea that mindfulness be situated within a more expansive framework to cultivate well-being and that interventions be appropriately broadened to include additional elements that are necessary for human flourishing. The cultivation of well-being will be framed as an urgent public health need and strategies to disseminate practices at scale require investigation” [6]. This study is a response to that call.
Davidson and Dahl define contemplative practices as “practical methods to bring about a state of enduring well-being or inner flourishing,” and include physical and mental behaviors that are thought to affect a variety of psychological constructs [7]. Contemplative practices emphasize self-awareness, self-regulation, and/or self-inquiry to enact a process of well-being, which may include psychological and/or spiritual transformation, and/or self-transcendence [7,8,9,10,11,12]. In addition to fostering states that promote individual well-being, CPB enhances traits that may also contribute to social welfare through prosociality, equanimity, altruism, compassion, and ethics [7,13,14].
Contemplative practices include tools and techniques from the world’s traditions of spirituality and religion, and indigenous systems of healing and health promotion. Thus, most contemplative practices originated as part of integrated coherent lifestyle systems intended to strengthen an individual’s ability to thrive, create innovations that address the needs of humanity and society, and serve the health and well-being of all of life [15]. The integrated systems provided philosophical and theoretical frameworks that have examined and offered explanations for the evolution and expression of the natural interplay of the mental, emotional, and spiritual facets of human life that support biopsychosocialspiritual development, health, and well-being [16,17,18]. These systems include but are not limited to the Taoist Five Element System, the Eight Limb teachings of Raja/Ashtanga Yoga, the Buddhist Eightfold Path, the Islamic Path of Dhikr practice, and the Christian path delineated in The Cloud of Unknowing [19], a spiritual guide to contemplative prayer that contributed to the development of Centering Prayer.
Unifying these culturally diverse traditions and systems is the principle that contemplation offers a sense of connection with the source of all of life and the direct experience of awe and feelings of reverence and gratitude. Furthermore, each system offers several contemplative practices that facilitate an increased frequency, duration, and depth of contemplation throughout daily life, not only during formal contemplative practice, thus giving meaning to every moment.
Today there are opportunities to learn a variety of contemplative practices from the diverse world traditions and systems. However, the modern dissemination of contemplative practices has frequently occurred in dispersed fragments rather than through the transfer of an entire tradition or system. Furthermore, the traditional systems were not developed within the context of modern life and all of its complexities. Instead, most of the systems emerged related to monastic life in agrarian societies. Ken Wilber’s Integral Life Practice [18] has offered a framework for the modern day. Nevertheless, there remains a need for further research to construct a cohesive comprehensive understanding of the means by which to best incorporate contemplative practices into modern day life. Our study aims to contribute to the further development of the theory and framework for the modern-day application of contemplation and contemplative practices. This study builds upon and expands beyond the evidence on “a la carte” contemplative practices (e.g., mindfulness meditation, compassion cultivation, or hatha yoga, etc., considered independently).

1.2. Contemplative Practices and Well-Being

It is well documented that a variety of different contemplative practices are related to an array of positive biopsychosocial outcomes, including support for connections between mindfulness meditation and immune system biomarkers [20]), and an association of self-reported mindfulness meditation practice with physical activity, with meditators less likely to be inactive, and more likely to meet guidelines for optimal physical activity [21,22,23]. A recent review of workplace-based mindfulness programs suggested that such efforts may help improve multiple dimensions of psychological functioning among employees [24]. In addition, a meta-analysis found that compassion-based interventions can improve self-reported psychosocial and interpersonal outcomes [25]. Furthermore, Western psychological interventions that incorporate classic Buddhist contemplative practices have been shown to promote a sense of purpose and meaning, thus fostering more enduring contentment [26].
Many studies of CPB have focused exclusively on mindfulness meditation, producing evidence of positive benefits [27,28]. In 2021, Davidson noted “MBIs are truly a model of a transdiagnostic intervention that may potentially have beneficial impact across a wide range of conditions and populations” [6]. Studies have found that even a short amount of meditation practice can reduce rumination and trait anxiety, increase empathy and self-compassion [29,30], develop healthy distress tolerance, beneficial emotional regulation and emotional stability [28,31,32], increase happiness [7], and foster conscious regulation of mental attention. MacCoon and colleagues found that in a randomized control trial, compared to a validated active control group [33], meditation-naive participants in an 8-week meditation intervention experienced decreased reactivity to affective stimuli and enhanced automatic emotion regulation [34]. Importantly, these benefits were further enhanced among participants who were long term meditation practitioners.
Prolonged contemplative practice training may have the potential to impact not only perceptions of well-being but also biological processes underlying health status. In a meditation study, participants in a three-month retreat program had both positive biological and psychological effects. Compared to the wait-list controls who were matched for age, body mass index, and prior meditation experience, individuals in the meditation retreat program had improved telomerase activity and immune cell functioning as well as decreased neuroticism and increased purpose in life [35].
Among studies that have investigated the impact of CPB among clinical populations, there is some evidence for both physical and psychological benefits. Research on contemplative practices in patients with cardiac disease has shown “encouraging results” for improving perceived physical and mental quality of life, as well as systolic and diastolic blood pressure [36]. Among cancer patients, a mindfulness-based stress reduction program, which included meditation and yoga training as well as interpersonal discussion exercises, was found to improve patients’ quality of life and decrease negative experiences of stress, as well as lower cortisol levels, systolic blood pressure, and pro-inflammatory cytokines [37,38]. A meta-analysis by Hoffman and colleagues found moderate support for mindfulness-based therapies’ effectiveness for reducing anxiety and improving mood among clinical populations [39]. Other reviews have found preliminary evidence to support mindfulness interventions to treat pain, depression, and addiction [40].

1.3. Understanding Multiple Practices: Contemplative Practice Behavior

Investigating the typology and combined effect of multiple contemplative practices on well-being will improve our insight into how CPB affects well-being and health. Most studies on contemplative practices to date have assessed the relationship between a single contemplative practice, such as hatha yoga, mindfulness meditation, or compassion cultivation, and a specific health or well-being outcome.
To fill this gap, our study uses a summary index measure of contemplative practices and a multi-dimensional measure of well-being to assess the individual and combined contributions of CPB on both overall well-being and on nine domains of well-being, leveraging survey responses from a total of 19,743 individuals from three global study sites. We used a set of four contemplative practices, including embodied observing meditation, non-reactive meditation, self-compassion, and compassion for others to measure CPB. We used the WELL for Life survey [41] to measure multi-dimensions of well-being to determine the associations between CPB and overall well-being and its nine constituents.
Prior research into the benefits of CPB has generally taken a reductionistic approach by focusing on one specific contemplative practice, such as mindfulness meditation, and by evaluating the impact on a relatively narrow range of well-being dimensions. The present research aims to extend this work by examining the association of a more expansive measure CPB with a broader range of well-being dimensions (e.g., financial well-being, creativity, and spirituality). By incorporating an inclusive definition of CPB in our assessment we were able to implement the same survey across three different cultural contexts in which engagement in specific CPB may vary to examine the robustness of the associations of CPB with nine specific dimensions of well-being.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Setting and Design

Begun in 2015, the Well for Life Study aims to quantify and contextualize individual well-being, investigate patterns and determinants of well-being in a large, multi-ethnic, and global multi-regional population, and to promote well-being in individuals and communities [41]. Participants provided informed consent and the study was approved by the Stanford University Institutional Review Board.
Participants were primarily recruited from three regions: the San Francisco Bay Area region of California, New Taipei City, and Hangzhou City in Zhejiang Province. In the US and in New Taipei City, participants were recruited via community partners, community outreach events, mailing lists, and social media. In the US, recruitment strategies also led to responses from outside the Bay Area and California, though 69.3% of the sample was from the Bay Area and a further 6.9% from California; thus, we subsequently use the label “CA/Bay Area”. In Hangzhou City, participants were recruited using stratified quota sampling from three of the city’s nine districts [42].
Data were collected via online questionnaires in the CA/Bay Area, or by self-administered surveys during a visit to a university lab in Hangzhou and New Taipei City. The questionnaire gathered information about participant demographics, medical history, contemplative practices, and well-being. All questionnaire items were presented in English (CA/Bay Area cohort) or in Mandarin Chinese (Hangzhou and New Taipei City cohorts) translated from the English version. In order to accommodate cultural differences (detailed below), some of the demographic items varied slightly across the cohorts.

2.2. Independent Variable

Contemplative Practice Behavior (CBP). Contemplative practice behavior (CPB) encompasses four distinct practices, each measured by a single item to reduce the participant burden. Our four CBP items reflected behaviors that cultivate each of the dimensions of contemplative practice included in the S-ART model forth by Vago and Silbersweig Self-Awareness, -Regulation, and -Transcendence, defined as a positive relationship between self and other that transcends self-focused needs and increases prosocial characteristics, such as compassion [11].
Our four items were based on the factors identified in prior research to be most representative of the salient processes associated with the benefits of contemplative practice [43,44,45,46,47]. The practice of embodied somatic self-awareness was measured by the frequency of embodied-observing practices (i.e., pausing routine activities for at least five minutes for breathing deeply, gently stretching, noticing your senses). The practice of mindfulness and self-regulation was measured by non-reactive practices (i.e., pausing routine activities for at least five minutes for observing emotions and thoughts as they arise rather than being caught up in them). Compassion practice was measured by the frequency of self-compassion practice (i.e., pausing routine activities for at least five minutes to observe and modify the way one is thinking to offer more compassion, love, or kindness to oneself) and compassion practice toward others (i.e., pausing routine activities for at least five minutes to observe and modify the way one is thinking to offer more compassion, love, or kindness toward others). The frequency of each contemplative practice was measured on a five-point scale (0–4; Never, Almost never, Sometimes, Fairly often, Very often). An overall CPB score was calculated as the sum of the four practice items.
Gu et al.’s [43] assessment of the Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire [48,49] identified that the following four facets load best into one score: Describing, Acting with Awareness, Nonjudging of Inner Experience, and Nonreactivity to Inner Experience function as four subscales, while the Observing factor functions as a separate measure. Observing refers to attending or noticing internal and external experiences (e.g., sounds, emotions, thoughts, bodily sensations, smells). Furthermore, Nonreactivity to Inner Experience has been identified as a significant component of the mechanism by which contemplative practices are beneficial [3,32,50,51,52,53,54,55]. Thus, we measured behaviors of embodied observing and behaviors of non-reactivity to inner awareness. Similarly, the self-compassion and compassion behaviors were measured because they have been identified to be dimensions of contemplative practice that contribute to benefits through mechanisms distinct from behaviors focused on embodied observing and non-reactivity to inner experiences [56,57,58,59].

2.3. Outcome Measure (Well-Being and Its Nine Domains)

Well-being was assessed using the 53-item WELL survey, the development of which has been described previously [41,60]. Briefly, the WELL survey was developed by the Stanford Well for Life Study through grounded theory and qualitative research that identified domains of well-being in various cultural groups to create a tool for understanding well-being that is valid across cultures. Standard questions in each domain from internationally validated surveys were used to construct the WELL survey in nine domains of well-being. In New Taipei City, a 52-item survey was administered, without the single-item self-rated health question used in the CA/Bay Area and Hangzhou cohorts. Formative assessment of the survey included cognitive interviews as recommended by Willis and colleagues [61]. Table 1 shows the list of the nine domains, associated definitions, and sample items.
For each of the nine well-being domains, a score from 0 to 10 was created based on the responses to the constituent items: 14 for stress and resilience, 13 items for social connectedness, 11 for experience of emotions, 5 for sense of self, 4 for physical health, 2 for purpose and meaning, 1 for financial security and satisfaction, 1 for spirituality and religiosity, and 1 for exploration and creativity. Higher scores on each domain indicate more optimal levels of well-being. For example, a higher score for the experience of emotions domain indicates more frequent positive emotions and less frequent negative emotions. The domain scores were summed to create the overall well-being score (WELL score). Each of the nine domains were scored 0–10, and an unweighted overall well-being score was calculated by summing each of the domain scores. For ease of interpretation, the score was re-scaled to 100.

2.4. Test–Retest Reliability and Convergent Validity

A sub-sample of initial survey participants in the US were invited to participate in a re-administration of the questionnaire one week later. The test–retest correlation for the WELL score was 0.92 (n = 92). Moreover, as part of the test–retest administration, participants were asked to complete the WHO-5 [62] in order to assess the association of the WELL score with the well-validated WHO-5. The correlation of the WELL score with the WHO-5 was 0.73. Results of a confirmatory factor analysis for the US WELL score had good model fit with rmsea = 0.059 and cfi = 0.852. Cronbach alphas for the domains that were measured with multi-item scales were: Resilience 0.92, Stress 0.78, Social connectedness 0.89, Negative emotions 0.85, Positive emotions 0.86, Sense of self 0.87, Purpose and Meaning 0.86, and Physical health 0.76.

2.5. Covariates

2.5.1. Physical Activity

Given the robust and long-standing evidence base for physical activity’s (PA) benefits to overall health status [63] and the evidence suggesting a positive relationship of PA with perceived physical health, quality of life, and well-being [64,65,66], PA was included as a covariate in all analyses. In the CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City, PA was measured using the Stanford Leisure-Time Activity Categorical Item (L-Cat 2.2) [67]. This is a single item measure that asks people to read through six descriptions of activity levels and choose the one that best describes their level of activity during the last month. Responses from 1 (“I did not do much physical activity […]”) to 6 (“Almost daily, that is, five or more times a week, I did vigorous activities such as running or riding hard on a bike for 30 min or more each time”). The full text of these survey questions is available in Supplemental Materials. In Hangzhou, PA was measured with the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ). This measure includes a number of questions regarding PA (e.g., time spent walking) and total time of moderate and vigorous activity every day in the past week. The PA score was generated by adding the times and weighting them based on activity to arrive at a categorical variable that could be classified as light, moderate or vigorous activity [68].

2.5.2. Demographic Characteristics

The WELL survey also included questions on age, gender, marital status, employment status, educational attainment, and ethnicity and race (in the Bay Area only). Several of these variables were measured in slightly different ways between cohorts, which are noted in Table 1.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

The distribution of each of the four CPB items was assessed as well as the Spearman correlation (pairwise) of items with one another. Continuous variables were centered at their median values, and binary variables were coded as −0.5 and 0.5 so that estimates represent averages. Dummy variables were created for each of the categorical variables and were coded as 1–1/m and −1/m, where m is the number of categories in each variable.
The association of CPB with the WELL score and the nine domain-specific scores were modeled separately for each study site (CA/Bay Area, Hangzhou, and New Taipei City), using hierarchical multivariate linear regression. For each outcome, a series of models were fitted: Model 1 (demographic covariates alone), Model 2 (Model 1 covariates and PA), and Model 3 (Model 2 covariates and CPB). Multivariate Wald tests were used to sequentially compare each model (e.g., Model 1 vs. Model 2, Model 2 vs. Model 3) and to determine the significance of the additional covariates [69]. Models were adjusted for gender, age (continuous), education (high school or less, some college or associate degree, bachelor’s degree, and post-graduates), marital status (married or cohabitating, single and other) and work status (working, students, retired, not working). In the Bay Area cohort, race (white/Caucasian, Asian or Pacific Islander, Black/African American, and Multiracial/other race) and ethnicity (Hispanic or not) covariates were also included; race/ethnicity variables were not surveyed in Hangzhou or New Taipei City as these populations are mostly Han Chinese (95% in New Taipei City and 98% in Hangzhou). For the Hangzhou cohort, the education categories were recategorized as high school or less, some college education, and college degree or above. The largest category for each variable served as the reference group in each cohort (see Table 2).
Missing values of all covariates and dependent variables were imputed via multiple imputation [70,71] using the R package [72] (R Core Team 2016), mice4 version 2.46.0 [73]. Ten iterations of imputation were carried out using predictive mean matching, logistic regression, and polytomous regression imputation for continuous, binary, and categorical data, respectively; summaries of the imputed data are presented alongside the original datasets as Supplemental Material. Mean estimates, 95% confidence intervals, and adjusted R2 values were calculated from the pooled regression estimates for all models. All analyses were performed in R version 3.3.3.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic Characteristics

Table 2 provides a descriptive summary of participant demographic characteristics by study site. Overall, 19,743 participants were included in this study from three separate cohorts: 6442 from the CA/Bay Area, 10,268 from Hangzhou, and 3033 from New Taipei City. All three cohorts had high proportions of female participants (between 60 and 71%). Compared to the Hangzhou and New Taipei City cohorts, the CA/Bay Area cohort was younger (mean age of 41.4 years versus 54.4 and 53.2, respectively), more highly educated (68.7% having a bachelor’s degree or higher), and mostly employed (67.6% versus 29.6% in Hangzhou and 53.6% in New Taipei City). The CA/Bay Area cohort also included a larger proportion of single individuals (42.8%), whereas among the Hangzhou and New Taipei City cohorts, most participants reported their status as married or cohabiting (85.2% and 75.6%, respectively).

3.2. Descriptive Statistics for CPB, PA, and Well-Being

Unadjusted overall scores of well-being were significantly (p < 0.001) higher among CA/Bay Area participants (mean = 59.1, SD = 12.0) compared to the other two cohorts, which had similar means (Hangzhou: mean = 55.9, SD = 9.2; New Taipei City: mean = 55.3, SD = 11.0). The highest average domain-specific score was financial security and satisfaction for the CA/Bay Area, social connectedness for Hangzhou, and sense of self for New Taipei City; the lowest average domain-specific score was spirituality and religiosity for all cohorts. The four CPB items were highly correlated in all three cohorts, with pairwise Spearman correlations ranging from 0.42 to 0.72 in the CA/Bay Area, 0.36 to 0.62 in Hangzhou, and 0.43 to 0.73 in New Taipei City. Average CPB was highest for the New Taipei City cohort (9.20, SD = 3.21), where the most frequent practice reported was embodied mindfulness (see Table 3). In the CA/Bay Area cohort, average CPB was 8.20 (SD = 3.72), and compassion toward others was the most frequent practice. The Hangzhou cohort had a roughly similar CPB mean (8.90, SD = 2.95), with compassion toward others as the most frequent practice. Physical activity scores (measured using L-Cat 2.2) were significantly (p < 0.001) lower among the New Taipei City cohort (2.60, SD = 1.23) compared to the CA/Bay Area (3.50, SD = 1.45). Among the Hangzhou cohort, 49.4% of participants reported recent physical activities that were classified as “vigorous” (measured using IPAQ). Table 3 describes these health behaviors and well-being outcomes across the three cohorts.

3.3. Contemplative Practice Behavior and Well-Being

Across all three cohorts, CPB was significantly (p < 0.001) associated with well-being (see Figure 1, Table 4). With every standard deviation increase in CPB, the overall WELL score increased by 1.22 points (SE = 0.03, 95% CI = 1.15–1.28) for the CA/Bay Area cohort, 1.16 (SE = 0.03, CI = 1.10–1.22) for the Hangzhou cohort, and 1.73 (SE = 0.05, CI = 1.63–1.83) for the New Taipei City cohort. The addition of CPB to overall WELL score models produced a notable effect in all three cohorts: adjusted R2 increased in Models 2 to 3 by 0.16 to 0.30 in the CA/Bay Area, from 0.04 to 0.17 in Hangzhou, and 0.12 to 0.36 in New Taipei City (see Table 4).
All of the WELL domains were significantly (p < 0.05) and positively associated with CPB score in each of the three cohorts. Figure 2 illustrates the coefficient estimates and confidence intervals for associations between CPB, PA, and the nine well-being domains among the three cohorts. The relative contribution of the CBP variable, as measured by each model’s adjusted R2 value, varied widely between domains (see Table 5); however, across the three cohorts, the domains of purpose and meaning, exploration and creativity, and spirituality and religiosity were most sensitive to the addition of the CPB variable.
As expected, physical activity, which was entered as a covariate, was also positively and significantly (p < 0.001) associated with overall well-being among the CA/Bay Area, Hangzhou, and New Taipei City cohorts (Table 4). Figure 1 illustrates the coefficients of association and 95% confidence intervals for PA and CPB in fully adjusted models of overall well-being. Significant positive associations were found for all well-being domains in the New Taipei City and CA/Bay Area cohorts, except for spirituality and religiosity, where no significant relationship was observed for the New Taipei City cohort, and a significant negative association was found for the CA/Bay Area cohort. Among the Hangzhou cohort, PA was significantly and positively associated with six domains of well-being. Figure 2 illustrates these associations across the three cohorts and nine domains of well-being.

4. Discussion

We found significant associations between a summary index of four distinct contemplative practices (CPB) and multi-dimensional well-being (WELL score) in a large cohort of individuals from three different global regions. The unique finding of the association between a combination of four CPBs with a multidimensional assessment of well-being across three cohorts from different global regions provides evidence that the effect of contemplative practice behaviors on well-being transcends regions and cultures. The magnitude of the positive associations was larger for the New Taipei City cohort, but findings were similar for the CA/Bay Area and Hangzhou cohorts, suggesting that culture may play a role in the size of the association of CPB with well-being.
The significant and positive associations between CPB and most WELL domains in all three cohorts suggest the importance of CPB for psychosocial and mental health and other health outcomes. Of the nine domains in WELL, six assess aspects of mental and psychosocial health: experience of emotions, exploration and creativity, purpose and meaning, sense of self, social connectedness, and perceived stress and resilience. Thus, positive associations with these domains echo the results from previous studies on contemplative practices and psychosocial outcomes [7,74,75]. Positive associations also emerged between CPB and three domains of well-being not traditionally included in measures of physical or mental health, including financial security and satisfaction (Hangzhou and New Taipei City), and physical health (CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City). These correlations suggest an association of CPB with outcomes across a wide range of factors related to well-being beyond those that are typically included in studies of mental and physical health.
In the context of previous research including both observational and experimental studies that has clearly documented a positive relationship between PA and well-being [65,66,76,77], we sought to examine the association between CPB and well-being over and above the contribution of PA to well-being. Our data show that that both PA and CPB were independently associated with well-being and its constituent domains. While a direct quantitative comparison of the regression coefficients for CPB when predicting well-being and those for PA when predicting well-being was not appropriate given the different levels of measurement of these two concepts in this study, our findings do suggest that the associations between CPB and well-being followed a comparable positive pattern to those between PA and well-being. Notably, the magnitude of these associations varied between domains. For instance, the coefficient of association for CPB was greater than that of PA within the purpose and meaning domain across all three cohorts.
Further research is needed to generate a clearly defined recommendation for the frequency and intensity of contemplative practice behaviors, similar to the recommendations for performing 10,000 steps for physical activity [78], sleeping for 7–9 h per night, and eating five servings of fruits and vegetables per day [79]). Until new studies have illuminated and clarified optimal contemplative practice behavioral recommendations, this study suggests that as with physical activity, sleep, and fruits and vegetables, “some contemplative practice is better than none”.
As evidence for the health-enhancing potential of CPB accumulates, government-sponsored surveillance systems have an opportunity to build assessments of CPB into their data collection and agenda-setting strategies, which may inspire greater attention to this aspect of lifestyle across the public and private sectors. Inclusion of contemplative practice behaviors as part of the fundamental lifestyle recommendations for health and well-being will likely lead to an increase in interventions and curricula that focus on contemplative practices behaviors in health promotion programs in schools as well as in public health and health care systems. Many current health promotion programs’ budgets mainly focus on physical activity and nutrition, and may offer access to gyms, pools, nutritious meals, and farmer’s markets, as well as fitness and nutrition assessments. However, there seems to be relatively fewer resources dedicated to contemplative spaces and contemplative practice behavior assessments and skill-building opportunities. For example, while the Healthy People 2020 materials included the newly added section “Health-related Quality of Life and Well-being” that measures components of well-being comparable to those found to be positively associated with CPB in this study, it does not specify an assessment of contemplative practices behaviors [80]. Similarly, the 2019 Center for Disease Control’s (CDC) Behavioral Risk Factor Survey did not ask about CPB [81]. The CDC’s redesigned National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) Sample Adult Questionnaire pain management assessment includes contemplative practices options (yoga or tai chi, meditation, guided imagery, or other relaxation techniques); however, it does not assess contemplative practices as one of the annually measured core health-related behaviors [82]. Nevertheless, an expert panel on community health promotion convened by the CDC suggested that more integrated and inclusive approaches to well-being were needed, including changes to research and funding priorities [83]
While this study speaks to the broader possible benefits of CPB, other research [15] suggests that health and well-being promotion interventions that cultivate and support contemplative practice behavior are feasible, affordable, and adaptable. Broader approaches such as these could be fundamental to achieving the broad visions set forth by national and international frameworks, such as Heathy People 2030 and the World Health Organization constitution, which states: “Health is a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” [84].

Limitations

Several limitations should be noted. First, the cross-sectional design precluded a causal understanding of the relationships identified in this study; future longitudinal designs and mediation analyses may help unpack any causal mechanisms at play in these relationships. Second, qualitative research methods may be better suited to illuminating the specific ways in which contemplative practices contribute to well-being for research participants. Third, when the single-item domains (three of nine) are used as the dependent variables in certain regression models, the residuals are not normally distributed; the relatively large size of the datasets employed in the analyses partially mitigated this issue [85]. Fourth, among the three cohorts, age and education distributions differed significantly, and could potentially have influenced the results within each cohort and the comparisons across cohorts. However, the diversity across sites did seem to provide some internal replications to support the robustness of the findings, which followed a generally consistent pattern between cohorts. Finally, biometric data were not included in this analysis, although the use of such measures, particularly those identified in prior research on the health benefits of contemplative practices, such as markers of immune system function [20,86], salivary cortisol, heart rate, heart rate variability [87], blood pressure [36], electroencephalogram (EEG) [88], and MRI and fMRI [89], would strengthen future investigations.

5. Conclusions

As with other canonical lifestyle behaviors, multiple contemplative practices can be integrated into one’s daily routine. Thus, it is critical to holistically consider these behaviors, extending them beyond a simple uni-dimensional measure (e.g., minutes of daily mindfulness mediation practice). We developed an integrative measure of four types of contemplative practice and found it to be significantly associated with a multi-dimensional measure of well-being. Importantly, our findings were from three large global multi-regional cohorts and compared against better-understood lifestyle behaviors (physical activity), broadening their applicability to settings around the globe.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/article/10.3390/ijerph192013485/s1, Materials S1: Physical Activity Survey Item (CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City) from L-Cat 2.2; Table S1: Race/Ethnicity Characteristics of CA/Bay Area Cohort; Code S1: R Code for CA/Bay Area Cohort; Code S2: R Code for Hangzhou Cohort; Code S3: R Code for New Taipei City Cohort.

Author Contributions

T.R., B.W.C., H.H., R.K. and C.H. contributed to the study conception and design of this report. J.-T.L., S.-L.Y., C.-A.S., Y.M., X.Z., S.Z., S.J.W., T.R., C.H. and A.W.H. contributed to the conception, design, data collection, and implementation of the Stanford WELL for Life Study in WELL CA/Bay Area, WELL Hangzhou, WELL New Taipei City. Data preparation and analyses were performed by R.K., H.H. and B.W.C. The first draft of the manuscript was written by T.R. and B.W.C. And all authors commented on various versions of the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Foundational funding for the Stanford Wellness Living Laboratory (WELL) was provided by Amway via the Nutrilite Health Institute Wellness Fund. This study was also supported by National Institutes of Health postdoctoral fellowship grant #T32 HL007034 funded by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. The Stanford REDCap platform (http://redcap.stanford.edu; Accessed 6 September 2022) is developed and operated by Stanford Medicine Research IT team. The REDCap platform services at Stanford are subsidized by (a) Stanford School of Medicine Research Office, and (b) the National Center for Research Resources and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through grant UL1 TR001085.Through Zhejiang University, the Cyrus Tang Foundation and Zhejiang University Education Foundation also provided important financial support for the study.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The questionnaire and methodology for this study were approved by the Human Subject Institutional Review Boards at Stanford University under protocol number 1RB-35020, and Zhejiang University under protocol number ZGL201507-3, and Fujen Catholic University, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants, as per approved protocols.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Individuals wishing to access the datasets used in this analysis should contact: Ann Hsing at [email protected].

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  1. Suvarna, B.; Suvarna, A.; Phillips, R.; Juster, R.-P.; McDermott, B.; Sarnyai, Z. Health risk behaviours and allostatic load: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 108, 694–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Siegel, D.J. Mindsight: The New Science of Personal Transformation; Bantam Books: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  3. Hoge, E.A.; Acabchuk, R.L.; Kimmel, H.; Moitra, E.; Britton, W.B.; Dumais, T.; Fulwiler, C. Emotion-related constructs engaged by mindfulness-based interventions: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Mindfulness 2021, 12, 1041–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Sala, M.; Rochefort, C.; Lui, P.P.; Baldwin, A.S. Trait mindfulness and health behaviours: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 14, 345–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Goleman, D.; Davidson, R.J. Altered Traits: Science Reveals How Meditation Changes Your Mind, Brain, and Body; Avery: New York, NY, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  6. Davidson, R.J. Mindfulness and more: Toward a science of human flourishing. Psychosom. Med. 2021, 83, 665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Davidson, R.J.; Dahl, C.J. Varieties of Contemplative Practice. JAMA Psychiatry 2017, 74, 121–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Fiske, E. Self-transcendence theory and contemplative practices. Holist. Nurs. Pract. 2019, 33, 266–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Reed, P.G. Theory of self-transcendence. In Middle Range Theory for Nursing; Smith, M.J., Liehr, P.R., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2018; Volume 3, pp. 119–146. [Google Scholar]
  10. Reed, P.G. Unitary Human Beings: Theory and Research. In Patterns of Rogerian Knowing; Madrid, M., Ed.; National League for Nursing Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; 15, pp. 187–196. ISBN 0-88737-688-6. [Google Scholar]
  11. Vago, D.R.; Silbersweig, D.A. Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): A framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Coward, D.D.; Reed, P.G. Self-transcendence theory. In Nursing Theorists and Their Work; Alligood, M.R., Tomey, A.M., Eds.; Elsevier/Mosby: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2014; pp. 574–592. [Google Scholar]
  13. Seppälä, E.M.; Simon-Thomas, E.; Brown, S.L.; Worline, M.C.; Cameron, C.D.; Doty, J.R. (Eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Compassion Science; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  14. McEntee, R.; Bucko, A. New Monasticism: An Interspiritual Manifesto for Contemplative Living; Orbis Books: Maryknoll, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  15. Chrisinger, B.W.; Rich, T. Contemplation by Design: Leveraging the “Power of the Pause” on a Large University Campus Through Built and Social Environments. Front. Public Health 2020, 8, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. DiPerna, D. Streams of Wisdom: An Advanced Guide to Integral Spirtual Development; Bright Alliance: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2017; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
  17. Wilber, K. A Brief History of Everything; Shambhala Publications: Boston, MA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  18. Wilber, K.; Patten, T.; Leonard, A.; Morelli, M. Integral Life Practice: A 21st-Century Blueprint for Physical Health, Emotional Balance, Mental Clarity, and Spiritual Awakening; Shambhala Publications: Boston, MA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  19. Underhill, E. (Ed.) A Book of Contemplation: The Which Is Called the Cloud of Unknowing, in Which the Soul Is Oned with God; John M. Watkins: London, UK, 1912. [Google Scholar]
  20. Black, D.S.; Slavich, G.M. Mindfulness meditation and the immune system: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2016, 1373, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Don, B.P.; Van Cappellen, P.; Fredrickson, B.L. Understanding Engagement in and Affective Experiences During Physical Activity: The Role of Meditation Interventions. Psychosom. Med. 2021, 83, 592–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Sala, M.; Geary, B.; Baldwin, A.S. A mindfulness-based physical activity intervention: A randomized pilot study. Psychosom. Med. 2021, 83, 615–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Strowger, M.; Kiken, L.G.; Ramcharran, K. Mindfulness meditation and physical activity: Evidence from 2012 National Health Interview Survey. Health Psychol. 2018, 37, 924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Janssen, M.; Heerkens, Y.; Kuijer, W.; van der Heijden, B.; Engels, J. Effects of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction on employees’ mental health: A systematic review. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0191332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kirby, J.N.; Tellegen, C.L.; Steindl, S.R. A Meta-Analysis of Compassion-Based Interventions: Current State of Knowledge and Future Directions. Behav. Ther. 2017, 48, 778–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Wallace, B.A.; Shapiro, S.L. Mental balance and well-being: Building bridges between Buddhism and Western psychology. Am. Psychol. 2006, 61, 690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Goldberg, S.B.; Riordan, K.M.; Sun, S.; Davidson, R.J. The empirical status of mindfulness-based interventions: A systematic review of 44 meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 17, 108–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Brown, K.W.; Ryan, R.M. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Chiesa, A.; Serretti, A. Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction for Stress Management in Healthy People: A Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2009, 15, 593–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Grossman, P.; Niemann, L.; Schmidt, S.; Walach, H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 2004, 57, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Bishop, S.R.; Lau, M.; Shapiro, S.; Carlson, L.; Anderson, N.D.; Carmody, J.; Segal, Z.; Abbey, S.; Speca, M.; Devins, G. Mindfulness: A Proposed Operational Definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 2004, 11, 230–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Leyland, A.; Rowse, G.; Emerson, L.-M. Experimental Effects of Mindfulness Inductions on Self-Regulation: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Emotion 2019, 19, 108–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. MacCoon, D.G.; Imel, Z.E.; Rosenkranz, M.A.; Sheftel, J.G.; Weng, H.Y.; Sullivan, J.C.; Bonus, K.A.; Stoney, C.M.; Salomons, T.V.; Davidson, R.J. The validation of an active control intervention for Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR). Behav. Res. Ther. 2012, 50, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Kral, T.R.A.; Schuyler, B.S.; Mumford, J.A.; Rosenkranz, M.A.; Lutz, A.; Davidson, R.J. Impact of short- and long-term mindfulness meditation training on amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli. NeuroImage 2018, 181, 301–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jacobs, T.L.; Epel, E.S.; Lin, J.; Blackburn, E.H.; Wolkowitz, O.M.; Bridwell, D.A.; Zanesco, A.P.; Aichele, S.R.; Sahdra, B.K.; MacLean, K.A.; et al. Intensive meditation training, immune cell telomerase activity, and psychological mediators. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011, 36, 664–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Younge, J.O.; Gotink, R.A.; Baena, C.P.; Roos-Hesselink, J.W.; Hunink, M.M. Mind–body practices for patients with cardiac disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. J. Prev. Cardiol. 2015, 22, 1385–1398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Carlson, L.E.; Speca, M.; Faris, P.; Patel, K.D. One-year pre–post intervention follow-up of psychological, immune, endocrine and blood pressure outcomes of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Brain Behav. Immun. 2007, 21, 1038–1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Carlson, L.E.; Speca, M.; Patel, K.D.; Goodey, E. Mindfulness-based stress reduction in relation to quality of life, mood, symptoms of stress and levels of cortisol dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS) and melatonin in breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004, 29, 448–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Hofmann, S.G.; Sawyer, A.T.; Witt, A.A.; Oh, D. The Effect of Mindfulness-Based Therapy on Anxiety and Depression: A Meta-Analytic Review. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 78, 169–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Goldberg, S.B.; Tucker, R.P.; Greene, P.A.; Davidson, R.J.; Wampold, B.E.; Kearney, D.J.; Simpson, T.L. Mindfulness-based interventions for psychiatric disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 59, 52–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Heaney, C.; Avery, E.; Rich, T.; Ahuja, N.; Winter, S.; Stanford WELL for Life Measures Work Group. Stanford WELL for Life: Learning What It Means to Be Well. Am. J. Health Promot. 2017, 31, 449–450. [Google Scholar]
  42. Min, Y.; Zhao, X.; Stafford, R.S.; Ma, X.; Chen, S.-H.; Gan, D.; Wei, C.; Huang, C.; Chen, L.; Gao, P.; et al. Cohort Profile: WELL Living Laboratory in China (WELL-China). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2021, 50, 1432–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Gu, J.; Strauss, C.; Crane, C.; Barnhofer, T.; Karl, A.; Cavanagh, K.; Kuyken, W. Examining the factor structure of the 39-item and 15-item versions of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire before and after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for people with recurrent depression. Psychol. Assess. 2016, 28, 791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Neff, K.D. Self-Compassion: An Alternative Conceptualization of a Healthy Attitude Toward Oneself. Self Identity 2003, 2, 85–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Ooi, S.L.; Pak, S. The Landscape of Current Meditation Research: An Overview to the Special Issue on ’Health Benefits of Meditation’. OBM Integr. Complement. Med. 2019, 4, 033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Seppälä, E.M.; Nitschke, J.B.; Tudorascu, D.L.; Hayes, A.; Goldstein, M.R.; Nguyen, D.T.; Perlman, D.; Davidsaon, R.J. Breathing-based meditation decreases posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in US Military veterans: A randomized controlled longitudinal study. J. Trauma. Stress 2014, 27, 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Vieten, C.; Wahbeh, H.; Cahn, B.R.; MacLean, K.; Estrada, M.; Mills, P.; Murphy, M.; Wahbeh, H.; Cahn, B.R.; MacLean, K.; et al. Future directions in meditation research: Recommendations for expanding the field of contemplative science. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0205740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Baer, R.A.; Smith, G.T.; Hopkins, J.; Krietemeyer, J.; Toney, L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 2006, 13, 27–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Baer, R.A.; Smith, G.T.; Lykins, E.; Button, D.; Krietemeyer, J.; Sauer, S.; Walsh, E.; Duggam, D.; Williams, J.M.G. Construct validity of the five-facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment 2008, 15, 329–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Berkovich-Ohana, A.; Jennings, P.A.; Lavy, S. Contemplative Neuroscience, Self-Awareness, and Education. Prog. Brain Res. 2019, 244, 355–385. [Google Scholar]
  51. Tang, Y.-Y.; Hölzel, B.K.; Posner, M.I. The Neuroscience of Mindfulness Meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 16, 213–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lutz, J.; Herwig, U.; Opialla, S.; Hittmeyer, A.; Jäncke, L.; Rufer, M.; Grosse Holtforth, M.; Brühl, A.B. Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation—An FMRI Study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 776–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Robins, C.J.; Keng, S.-L.; Ekblad, A.G.; Brantley, J.G. Effects of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction on Emotional Experience and Expression: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Clin. Psychol. 2012, 68, 117–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Goldin, P.R.; Gross, J.J. Effects of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) on Emotion Regulation in Social Anxiety Disorder. Emotion 2010, 10, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Chambers, R.; Gullone, E.; Allen, N.B. Mindful Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 29, 560–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Phillips, W.J.; Hine, D.W. Self-compassion, physical health, and health behaviour: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2021, 15, 113–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ferrari, M.; Hunt, C.; Harrysunker, A.; Abbott, M.J.; Beath, A.P.; Einstein, D.A. Self-compassion interventions and psychosocial outcomes: A meta-analysis of RCTs. Mindfulness 2019, 10, 1455–1473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Luberto, C.M.; Shinday, N.; Song, R.; Philpotts, L.L.; Park, E.R.; Fricchione, G.L.; Yeh, G.Y. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of meditation on empathy, compassion, and prosocial behaviors. Mindfulness 2018, 9, 708–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Strauss, C.; Taylor, B.L.; Gu, J.; Kuyken, W.; Baer, R.; Jones, F.; Cavanagh, K. What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 47, 15–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Chrisinger, B.W.; Gustafson, J.A.; King, A.C.; Winter, S.J. Understanding Where We Are Well: Neighborhood-Level Social and Environmental Correlates of Well-Being in the Stanford Well for Life Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Willis, G.; Schechter, S.; Whitaker, K. A Comparison of Cognitive Interviewing, Expert Review, and Behavior Coding: What Do They Tell Us? American Statistical Association: Alexandria, VA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  62. Topp, C.W.; Østergaard, S.D.; Søndergaard, S.; Bech, P. The WHO-5 Well-Being Index: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Psychother. Psychosom. 2015, 84, 167–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. US Department of Health and Human Services. 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee Scientific Report; US Department of Health and Human Services: Washington, DC, USA, 2018; p. 779.
  64. Gillison, F.B.; Skevington, S.M.; Sato, A.; Standage, M.; Evangelidou, S. The effects of exercise interventions on quality of life in clinical and healthy populations; a meta-analysis. Soc. Sci. Med. 2009, 68, 1700–1710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Paluska, S.A.; Schwenk, T.L. Physical Activity and Mental Health. Sport. Med. 2000, 29, 167–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Penedo, F.J.; Dahn, J.R. Exercise and well-being: A review of mental and physical health benefits associated with physical activity. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2005, 18, 189–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Kiernan, M.; Schoffman, D.E.; Lee, K.; Brown, S.D.; Fair, J.M.; Perri, M.G.; Haskell, W.L. The Stanford Leisure-Time Activity Categorical Item (L-Cat): A single categorical item sensitive to physical activity changes in overweight/obese women. Int. J. Obes. 2013, 37, 1597–1602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Ainsworth, B.E.; Haskell, W.L.; Whitt, M.C.; Irwin, M.L.; Swartz, A.M.; Strath, S.J.; O’Brien, W.L.; Bassett, D.R.; Schmitz, K.H.; Emplaincourt, P.O.; et al. Compendium of physical activities: An update of activity codes and MET intensities. Med. Sci. Sport. Exerc. 2000, 32 (Suppl. S9), S498–S504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Van Buuren, S. Flexible Imputation of Missing Data, 2nd ed.; Chapman and Hall: London, UK, 2018; Available online: https://stefvanbuuren.name/fimd/ (accessed on 16 April 2021).
  70. Kontopantelis, E.; White, I.R.; Sperrin, M.; Buchan, I. Outcome-sensitive multiple imputation: A simulation study. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2017, 17, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Wulff, J.N.; Jeppesen, L.E. Multiple imputation by chained equations in praxis: Guidelines and review. Electron. J. Bus. Res. Methods 2017, 15, 41–56. [Google Scholar]
  72. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2016; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 6 September 2022).
  73. Van Buuren, S.; Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. Mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. J. Stat. Softw. 2011, 45, 1–67. Available online: https://econpapers.repec.org/article/jssjstsof/v_3a045_3ai03.htm (accessed on 10 November 2018). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Achepohl, G.; Heaney, C.; Rosas, L.G.; Moore, J.; Rich, T.; Winter, S.J. The Value of Contemplative Practices: A Mixed Methods Approach Exploring Associations between Resilience and Experiences of the COVID-19 Pandemic among Older Adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 10224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Chrisinger, B.W.; Rich, T.; Lounsbury, D.; Peng, K.; Zhang, J.; Heaney, C.A.; Lu, Y.; Hsing, A.W. Coping with the COVID-19 pandemic: Contemplative practice behaviors are associated with better mental health outcomes and compliance with Shelter-In-Place orders in a prospective cohort study. Prev. Med. Rep. 2021, 23, 101451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Fox, K.R. The influence of physical activity on mental well-being. Public Health Nutr. 1999, 2, 411–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Ivandic, I.; Freeman, A.; Birner, U.; Nowak, D. A systematic review of brief mental health and well-being interventions in organizational settings. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2017, 43, 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  78. Tudor-Locke, C.; Bassett, D.R. How Many Steps/Day Are Enough? Sport. Med. 2004, 34, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Office of Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Healthy People 2030 Framework; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Washington, DC, USA, 2020.
  80. Office of Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Healthy People 2020 Framework; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Washington, DC, USA, 2010.
  81. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System—Questionnaires. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/brfss/questionnaires/index.htm (accessed on 17 August 2020).
  82. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (March 2019). Detailed Outline of Topics in the Redesigned National Health Interview Survey (NHIS): Sample Adult Questionnaire. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhis/AdultNHISRedesignTopics201903.pdf (accessed on 17 August 2020).
  83. Navarro, A.M.; Voetsch, K.P.; Liburd, L.C.; Giles, H.W.; Collins, J.L. Charting the Future of Community Health Promotion: Recommendations. Natl. Expert Panel Community Health Promot. 2007, 4, 7. [Google Scholar]
  84. World Health Organization. Constitution of the World Health Organization.2005. Available online: http://apps.who.int/gb/bd/PDF/bd47/EN/constitution-en.pdf?ua=1 (accessed on 12 November 2018).
  85. Schmidt, A.F.; Finan, C. Linear regression and the normality assumption. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2018, 98, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Thibodeaux, N.; Rossano, M.J. Meditation and immune function: The impact of stress management on the immune system. OBM Integr. Complement. Med. 2018, 3, 032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Gamaiunova, L.; Brandt, P.-Y.; Bondolfi, G.; Kliegel, M. Exploration of psychological mechanisms of the reduced stress response in long-term meditation practitioners. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2019, 104, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Bostanov, V.; Ohlrogge, L.; Britz, R.; Hautzinger, M.; Kotchoubey, B. Measuring Mindfulness: A Psychophysiological Approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  89. Travis, F.; Nash, J.; Parim, N.; Cohen, B.H. Does the MRI/fMRI Procedure Itself Confound the Results of Meditation Research? An Evaluation of Subjective and Neurophysiological Measures of TM Practitioners in a Simulated MRI Environment. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Associations between overall WELL scores, contemplative practice behavior, and physical activity. Note: A different physical activity measure was used for the Hangzhou cohort (International Physical Activity Questionnaire; categorical variable with PA-Low as reference group) than for the CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City cohorts (L-Cat 2.2; ordinal variable, 1–6).
Figure 1. Associations between overall WELL scores, contemplative practice behavior, and physical activity. Note: A different physical activity measure was used for the Hangzhou cohort (International Physical Activity Questionnaire; categorical variable with PA-Low as reference group) than for the CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City cohorts (L-Cat 2.2; ordinal variable, 1–6).
Ijerph 19 13485 g001
Figure 2. Associations between Contemplative Practice Behavior, Physical Activity, and Well-Being Domains. Note: A different physical activity measure was used for the Hangzhou cohort (International Physical Activity Questionnaire; categorical variable with PA-Low as reference group) than for the CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City cohorts (L-Cat 2.2; ordinal variable, 1–6).
Figure 2. Associations between Contemplative Practice Behavior, Physical Activity, and Well-Being Domains. Note: A different physical activity measure was used for the Hangzhou cohort (International Physical Activity Questionnaire; categorical variable with PA-Low as reference group) than for the CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City cohorts (L-Cat 2.2; ordinal variable, 1–6).
Ijerph 19 13485 g002
Table 1. Stanford WELL for Life: Constituent Domains of Well-Being.
Table 1. Stanford WELL for Life: Constituent Domains of Well-Being.
DomainDefinitionExample ItemsNumber of Items
Social ConnectednessPositive or negative relationships with others and how they influence your well-being. During the last two weeks, how often did you feel …
  • …that you lacked companionship?
  • …that there were people you could talk to?
  • … that you were a part of a group of friends?
13
Stress and ResilienceStress: Feelings of overload and an inability to balance or manage tasks
Resilience: Ability to adapt to change and bounce back after hardship.
  • During the last two weeks, how often have you felt that you were not able to give enough time to the important things in your life?
  • How confident are you that you can bounce back quickly after hard times?
14
Experience of EmotionsHow often you experience both pleasant and unpleasant emotions. During the last two weeks, how often did you feel
  • … calm?
  • … drained?
11
Physical HealthPerception of your own health status, i.e., energy levels, ability to resist illness, physical fitness, and experience of pain.
  • Compared to others of your own age, how would you rate your health?
  • During the last two weeks, how often did your energy level allow you to do the things you WANT to do, as opposed to only the things you have to do?
4
Purpose and MeaningHaving a sense that aspects of your life provide purpose and meaning, i.e., goals, dreams, and being part of something larger than yourself. How often does your daily life include experiences that give your life
  • … purpose?
  • … meaning?
2
Sense of SelfThe extent to which you feel you know yourself, can express your true self, have self-confidence, and feel good about who you are.During the last two weeks, how often did you feel
  • … accepting of yourself?
  • … that you were interested in your daily activities?
5
Financial Security and SatisfactionYour perception of having enough money to meet your needs.
  • During the last year, how often have you had enough money to meet your needs?
11
Spirituality and ReligiosityThe extent to which spiritual and religious beliefs, practices, communities, and traditions are important in your life.
  • How important are spiritual or religious beliefs in your day-to-day life?
1
Exploration and CreativityHaving opportunities to grow as a person and to explore new experiences and ways of thinking.
  • How often do you engage with opportunities to challenge yourself and grow … as a person?
1
Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants 1.
Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants 1.
CA/Bay Area 2
(n = 6442)
Hangzhou
(n = 10,268)
New Taipei City
(n = 3033)
Age, mean (SD)41.4 (17.2)53.2 (14.1)54.4 (11.5)
Gender 3
Female4586 (71.2)6187 (60.3)2064 (68.1)
Male1754 (27.2)4081 (39.7)969 (31.9)
Missing28 (0.4)0 (0.0)0 (0.0)
Educational attainment
High school or less819 (12.7)7499 (73.0)1593 (52.5)
Some college1221 (19.0)1255 (12.2)
Bachelor’s degree 42084 (32.4)1208 (11.8)1173 (38.7)
Post-graduate/professional degree2273 (35.3)NA264 (8.7)
Missing45 (0.7)306 (3.0)3 (0.1)
Employment status
Working4356 (67.6)3036 (29.6)1627 (53.6)
Not Working539 (8.4)2171 (21.1)507 (16.7)
Retired472 (7.3)4723 (46.0)835 (27.5)
Student1047 (16.3)32 (0.3)13 (0.4)
Missing28 (0.4)306 (3.0)51 (1.7)
Marital status
Married or cohabiting2758 (42.8)8748 (85.2)2293 (75.6)
Single2606 (40.5)555 (5.4)407 (13.4)
Other1047 (16.3)659 (6.4)329 (10.8)
Missing31 (0.5)306 (3.0)4 (0.1)
1 Figures reported are from pre-imputation datasets. 2 Race/ethnicity data were collected for the Bay Area cohort and appear in Supplemental Table S1. 3 Transgender participants (n = 74, 1.1%) were also recorded in the CA/Bay Area WELL survey.4 For the Hangzhou cohort, the highest option for educational attainment was “college and above”, and is presented here under bachelor’s degree.
Table 3. Health Behaviors and Well-Being Outcomes across the Study Sample 1.
Table 3. Health Behaviors and Well-Being Outcomes across the Study Sample 1.
CA/Bay Area (n = 6442)Hangzhou (n = 10,268)New Taipei City (n = 3033)
VariablesMeansdn Missing% missingMeansdn Missing% MissingMeansdn Missing% Missing
Contemplative Practice Behavior (CPB)8.203.721001.558.902.953613.509.203.2130.10
  • Embodied Mindfulness
2.201.17681.062.301.023603.502.600.9410.03
  • Non-Reactive Mindfulness
1.801.21701.091.900.993613.502.101.0500.00
  • Self-Compassion
1.901.08661.022.300.903603.502.200.9810.03
  • Compassion toward Others
2.401.06691.072.300.913603.502.300.9310.03
  Physical activity (L-Cat)3.501.45921.43 2.601.23601.98
  Physical activity (IPAQ) 2 6.104.26120111.70
   WELL overall score59.1011.961472.2855.909.163603.5055.3011.03100.33
Domain-specific scores
  • Experience of emotions
5.901.58360.566.701.113543.406.501.3550.17
  • Exploration and creativity
6.902.23711.105.002.293603.505.302.3210.03
  • Financial security and satisfaction
7.702.62751.167.002.393603.506.402.6100.00
  • Physical health
6.801.61270.426.201.553323.206.101.5800.00
  • Purpose and meaning
6.802.15881.376.301.813603.505.902.2310.03
  • Spirituality and religiosity
4.703.58570.883.703.043583.504.702.8200.00
  • Social connectedness
6.701.67410.647.301.213543.407.001.4410.03
  • Stress and resilience
6.301.50350.546.601.343503.406.301.4950.17
  • Sense of self
7.301.89510.797.201.513583.507.201.8410.03
1 Numbers reported are from pre-imputation datasets and are unadjusted for demographic covariates; 2 A different physical activity measure was used for the Hangzhou cohort (International Physical Activity Questionnaire) than for the CA/Bay Area and New Taipei City cohorts (L-Cat 2.2, score: 1–6).
Table 4. Regression coefficient estimates for CPB and PA in the models for the overall WELL score.
Table 4. Regression coefficient estimates for CPB and PA in the models for the overall WELL score.
CohortEstimateStd. ErrorLower CIUpper CI
Contemplative
Practice
Behavior
CA/Bay Area1.220.031.151.28
New Taipei City1.730.051.631.83
Hangzhou1.160.031.101.22
Physical
Activity 1
CA/Bay Area2.060.091.882.23
New Taipei City1.070.140.791.34
Hangzhou
Vigorous
1.450.201.061.85
Hangzhou
Moderate
1.110.240.631.58
1 A different physical activity measure was used for the Hangzhou cohort (International Physical Activity Questionnaire) than for the SF Bay Area and New Taipei City cohorts (L-Cat 2.2), and Light physical activity was used as the reference group.
Table 5. Adjusted R2 Values with Confidence Intervals and Wald Tests of Significance for the Models in Hierarchical Regressions.
Table 5. Adjusted R2 Values with Confidence Intervals and Wald Tests of Significance for the Models in Hierarchical Regressions.
Model 1Model 2Model 3
DomainDemographic CovariatesDemographic Covariates + PA 1Demographic Covariates + PA 1 + CPB
CA/Bay AreaAdj R2(95% CI)Adj R2(95% CI), p-valueAdj R2(95% CI), p-value
WELL overall score0.09 (0.08–0.1)0.16 (0.15–0.18), <0.0010.30 (0.29–0.32), <0.001
Experience of Emotions0.07 (0.06–0.08)0.11 (0.1–0.13), <0.0010.16 (0.14–0.18), <0.001
Exploration and Creativity0.03 (0.02–0.03)0.06 (0.05–0.08), <0.0010.16 (0.15–0.18), <0.001
Financial Sec. and Satisfaction0.11 (0.09–0.12)0.13 (0.11–0.14), <0.0010.13 (0.11–0.15), <0.001
Physical Health0.08 (0.07–0.09)0.29 (0.27–0.31), <0.0010.31 (0.29–0.33), <0.001
Purpose and Meaning0.04 (0.03–0.05)0.07 (0.06–0.08), <0.0010.18 (0.16–0.2), <0.001
Sense of Self0.06 (0.05–0.07)0.11 (0.09–0.12), <0.0010.19 (0.17–0.21), <0.001
Social Connectedness0.06 (0.05–0.07)0.1 (0.08–0.11), <0.0010.13 (0.12–0.15), <0.001
Spirituality and Religiosity0.06 (0.05–0.08)0.06 (0.05–0.08), 0.050.17 (0.16–0.19), <0.001
Stress and Resilience0.08 (0.07–0.09)0.14 (0.13–0.16), <0.0010.19 (0.17–0.21), <0.001
HangzhouAdj R2 (95% CI)Adj R2 (95% CI), p-val.Adj R2 (95% CI), p-val.
WELL overall score0.03 (0.02–0.03)0.04 (0.03–0.04), <0.0010.17 (0.16–0.19), <0.001
Experience of Emotions0.05 (0.04–0.06)0.05 (0.04–0.06), <0.0010.07 (0.06–0.08), <0.001
Exploration and Creativity0.04 (0.03–0.05)0.04 (0.03–0.05), <0.0010.16 (0.15–0.17), <0.001
Financial Sec. and Satisfaction0.1 (0.09–0.12)0.1 (0.09–0.12), 0.2120.11 (0.1–0.13), <0.001
Physical Health0.01 (0.01–0.02)0.03 (0.02–0.04), <0.0010.05 (0.04–0.06), <0.001
Purpose and Meaning0.01 (0.01–0.01)0.01 (0.01–0.02), <0.0010.19 (0.18–0.21), <0.001
Sense of Self0.05 (0.04–0.06)0.06 (0.05–0.07), <0.0010.11 (0.1–0.12), <0.001
Social Connectedness0.04 (0.03–0.04)0.04 (0.03–0.05), <0.0010.06 (0.05–0.07), <0.001
Spirituality and Religiosity0.02 (0.01–0.02)0.02 (0.01–0.02), 0.3120.03 (0.03–0.04), <0.001
Stress and Resilience0.02 (0.01–0.02)0.02 (0.02–0.03), <0.0010.06 (0.05–0.07), <0.001
New Taipei CityAdj R2(95% CI)Adj R2(95% CI), p-val.Adj R2(95% CI), p-val.
WELL overall score0.08 (0.06–0.1)0.12 (0.1–0.14), <0.0010.36 (0.33–0.38), <0.001
Experience of Emotions0.08 (0.06–0.1)0.1 (0.08–0.12), <0.0010.15 (0.13–0.18), <0.001
Exploration and Creativity0.03 (0.02–0.04)0.05 (0.04–0.07), <0.0010.26 (0.24–0.29), <0.001
Financial Sec. and Satisfaction0.08 (0.06–0.1)0.1 (0.08–0.12), <0.0010.13 (0.11–0.16), <0.001
Physical Health0.04 (0.02–0.05)0.07 (0.05–0.09), <0.0010.11 (0.09–0.13), <0.001
Purpose and Meaning0.02 (0.01–0.03)0.04 (0.02–0.05), <0.0010.37 (0.35–0.4), <0.001
Sense of Self0.06 (0.04–0.08)0.07 (0.06–0.09), <0.0010.17 (0.15–0.2), <0.001
Social Connectedness0.05 (0.03–0.06)0.06 (0.05–0.08), <0.0010.12 (0.1–0.15), <0.001
Spirituality and Religiosity0.03 (0.02–0.04)0.03 (0.02–0.04), 0.8260.07 (0.05–0.09), <0.001
Stress and Resilience0.07 (0.05–0.09)0.09 (0.07–0.11), <0.0010.16 (0.14–0.19), <0.001
1 Different physical activity measures were used for the Hangzhou cohort than for the CABay Area and New Taipei City cohort.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rich, T.; Chrisinger, B.W.; Kaimal, R.; Winter, S.J.; Hedlin, H.; Min, Y.; Zhao, X.; Zhu, S.; You, S.-L.; Sun, C.-A.; et al. Contemplative Practices Behavior Is Positively Associated with Well-Being in Three Global Multi-Regional Stanford WELL for Life Cohorts. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 13485. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192013485

AMA Style

Rich T, Chrisinger BW, Kaimal R, Winter SJ, Hedlin H, Min Y, Zhao X, Zhu S, You S-L, Sun C-A, et al. Contemplative Practices Behavior Is Positively Associated with Well-Being in Three Global Multi-Regional Stanford WELL for Life Cohorts. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(20):13485. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192013485

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rich, Tia, Benjamin W. Chrisinger, Rajani Kaimal, Sandra J. Winter, Haley Hedlin, Yan Min, Xueyin Zhao, Shankuan Zhu, San-Lin You, Chien-An Sun, and et al. 2022. "Contemplative Practices Behavior Is Positively Associated with Well-Being in Three Global Multi-Regional Stanford WELL for Life Cohorts" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 20: 13485. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192013485

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop