Next Article in Journal
Perceived Factors Contributing to the Subjective Wellbeing of Undergraduate Engineering Students: An Exploratory Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Effects of Music-Based Interventions on Motor and Non-Motor Symptoms in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Pb(II) Ion Removal Potential in Chemically Modified Ziziphus joazeiro Barks
Previous Article in Special Issue
Long-Term Multi-Sensory Gamma Stimulation of Dementia Patients: A Case Series Report
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Criterion-Related Validation of a Music-Based Attention Assessment for Individuals with Traumatic Brain Injury

1
Department of Music Therapy, Graduate School, Ewha Womans University, Seoul 03760, Republic of Korea
2
Uhealth Jackson Rehabilitation Care, Miami, FL 33136, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(23), 16285; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192316285
Submission received: 24 October 2022 / Revised: 15 November 2022 / Accepted: 24 November 2022 / Published: 5 December 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Music for Health Care and Well-Being)

Abstract

:
The music-based attention assessment (MAA) is a melody contour identification task that evaluates different types of attention. Previous studies have examined the psychometric and physiological validity of the MAA across various age groups in clinical and typical populations. The purpose of this study was to confirm the MAA’s criterion validity in individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and to correlate this with standardized neuropsychological measurements. The MAA and various neurocognitive tests (i.e., the Wechsler adult intelligence scale DST, Delis–Kaplan executive functioning scale color-word interference test, and Conner’s continuous performance test) were administered to 38 patients within two weeks prior to or post to the MAA administration. Significant correlations between MAA and neurocognitive batteries were found, indicating the potential of MAA as a valid measure of different types of attention deficits. An additional multiple regression analysis revealed that MAA was a significant factor in predicting attention ability.

1. Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is defined as damage to brain tissue caused by an external mechanical force and is characterized by loss of consciousness, post-traumatic amnesia, or objective neurological findings that are attributable to TBI upon a physical or mental examination [1]. Such damage may lead to permanent or temporary cognitive, socio-emotional, and/or sensory-motor impairment [2,3,4,5,6,7]; however, attention deficit has been reported to be the most common [8,9,10,11]. Impaired attention types vary from sustained attention [12,13,14,15] to selective attention [14,16,17,18] and more complex information processing, such as supervisory attention [18,19,20,21,22,23,24]. From a neuropathological perspective, primary and secondary head injuries (e.g., contusions in the sensory and frontal cortices, widespread axonal disruption) cause sensory disturbances and increased distractibility [2,25,26,27,28]. These disturbances result in attenuated and inaccurate attention performance [6,29,30,31]. Auditory information processing is considerably challenging [32,33,34,35,36,37] and attention performance rapidly decreases in the presence of auditory distraction [12,18,33,37,38,39].
Owing to this diverse range of attention deficits and their changes during the recovery process, a tailored approach to rehabilitation is necessary. However, the majority of existing attention batteries focus on attention in the visual modality, while a few measures utilize arithmetic information presented verbally, such as the WAIS-IV DST [40] and paced auditory serial addition test [41]. More importantly, attention involving the processing of incoming multi-layered auditory stimuli has not been well evaluated because the measures employ a single auditory stream which does not simulate the complexity of distracting sounds in auditory environments. Current attention batteries also lack assessment tasks that are hierarchically organized through the course of information processing, leading to inefficiency when multiple tests are administered. Therefore, attention measures for individuals with TBI need to embrace assessment tasks comprising various types and levels of cognitive demand embedded in a simulated realistic auditory environment.
Music-based attention assessment (MAA) is a melodic contour identification task, of which stimuli include pitches and rhythms. Both musical components are known to activate and utilize the cognitive as well as motor systems of patients with neurological disorders [18,42,43]. Specifically, melody perception requires involuntary and voluntary involvement of attention. Snyder and Alain [44] found that attention occurs involuntarily to process psychoacoustic features of sounds, and then voluntary attention follows to yield auditory stream segregation. Behavioral responses to music revealed the involvement of attention in music processing [45,46,47,48,49,50]. Additionally, another group of studies claimed that depending on the manner of listening to music (i.e., melody), specific brain regions modulating different types of attention are activated [51,52,53]. The brain regions are (a) the reticular formation, cerebellum, and thalamus (sub-cortical areas), primary sensory areas, motor areas, anterior cingulate and the prefrontal areas (cortical areas) for sustained attention, (b) the polysensory association areas (i.e., the junction of the occipital, parietal, and temporal regions) for selective attention, and (c) the anterior cingulate cortex, the parietal sulcus, and most of the frontal cortex for divided attention. Such findings together suggest that behavioral responses to music have a potential for measuring attentional ability and, thus, the appropriate use of music can be an effective measure of different types of attention ability [52,54,55,56,57,58,59].
The MAA was developed based on the neuroanatomical commonality between music perception and attention and aimed to measure different type and levels of attention involving auditory information processing in environmental contexts. The MAA was piloted with a small group of patients diagnosed with TBI (n = 15), reporting a wide range of item difficulty and good reliability [60]. The authors then revised the MAA to vary item difficulty and reflect a wider range of the continuum of attention ability existing in typical and clinical populations. Jeong [61] administered a revised 54-item version of the MAA to a larger group of young adults (n = 165) and patients with TBI (n = 22). Statistical validation showed promising construct validity and reliability, suggesting the elimination of nine problematic items and the partial reconstruction of the measure. In addition, comparisons of neurophysiological responses using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) were investigated in different age groups in typical [62,63] and clinical populations, such as patients with acquired brain injury and clinical liver disease [64,65]. This study investigated the criterion validity of the MAA as correlated with current neuropsychological attention measurements, that is, the Wechsler adult intelligence scale (WAIS-IV) digit span test, Conner’s continuous performance test, and the Delis–Kaplan executive functioning scale (D-KEFS) color-word interference test. The study also explored the potential of using the MAA as a predictor of cognitive capacity in neurorehabilitation.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Thirty-eight patients with TBI were voluntarily recruited from the health system of a hospital in South Florida according to convenience sampling. Participation was available to individuals diagnosed with traumatic brain injury and who scored less than 13 on the Glasgow coma scale (GCS) or, alternatively, a Rancho Los Amigos scale (RLAS) score greater than V at the time of brain injury assessment. Eligibility was also limited to patients who were non-professional musicians and had no visual or hearing impairment. The average age of the participants was 35.45 (SD = 14.79), and there was an average of 22.42 (SD = 33. 28) months since the date of brain injury. The participants’ levels of awareness as measured by GCS and cognitive functioning as measured by RLAS was an average of 5.78 (SD = 3.96) and 6.79 (SD = 2.44), respectively. The participants’ average years of education was 14.15 (SD = 3.77), with an average of 2.09 years of music education (SD = 2.31). Table 1 presents additional demographic characteristics.

2.2. Measures

Music-based attention assessment (MAA). MAA is a melodic contour identification task consisting of sustained, selective, and divided attention subtests. The melodic contours used to generate the test items included a series of five tones moving in one of three directions: ascending, stationary, or descending. One to three series of five tones were arrayed to form a single auditory stream, of which the presentation time ranged from 3–10 s. In the sustained attention-basic subtest, a set of five tones was presented as target contour. The number of tones in the target contour increases to include two to three sets of five tones in the sustained attention-advanced subtest. In the selective attention subtest, one to three sets of fives tones were presented with distraction (i.e., environmental sounds). In the divided attention subtest, two streams of one to two sets of five tones were presented concurrently, with one serving as the target contour and the other serving as a target-like distractor. Participants were asked to identify the target contour only in the divided attention-basic, while they were asked to identify both contours in the divided attention-advanced. In the present study, a 45-item MAA was utilized after eliminating 9 test items that showed statistical inconsistency [61]. The sound items of the MAA were produced using a music synthesizer (Korg N5EX, Korg Inc., Tokyo, Japan) and were programmed into a media player (iTunes version 10. Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA) using a laptop computer amplified with speakers. Table 2 shows the number of test items, stimuli, context characteristics, and the given tasks.
Digit Span Test (DST). The DST, a subtest of the working memory index of the Wechsler Adults Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) [40], is one of the frequently used attention assessment in a neurorehabilitation setting [66,67]. The DST consists of digit span forward (DSF), digit span backward (DSB), and newly added digit span sequencing (DSS) tests. In the DST, participants recalled a spoken sequence of numbers: (1) in the same order that the examiner presented (DSF), (2) in inverse order (DSB), and (3) in ascending order (DSS). Each item in the DSF, DSB, and DSS consisted of two trials. If the participant responded correctly, one point was assigned for the success of each trial. A maximum score of 16 was obtained, with a maximum summed score of 48. The completion time of the DST was approximately ten minutes.
Color-Word Interference Test (CWIT). The CWIT is one of the nine subtests of the Delis–Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS) [68], which is often used in clinical settings to assess mild brain damage in general and mild frontal lobe involvement in particular [69]. The CWIT comprises four conditions: naming, word reading, inhibition, and inhibition/switching. It assesses the ability to inhibit an overlearned verbal response using colors and letters (i.e., names of colors printed in a congruent or incongruent color ink). The total number of uncorrected errors, corrected errors, and time for completion was recorded. The scaled score for the completion time was used in this study. The time required to complete the CWIT was 5–10 min.
Conner’s Continuous Performance Test. The Conner’s continuous performance test (CPT) is widely used to measure various aspects of attentional behavior, such as inattentiveness, impulsivity, activation, and vigilance. The given task is to press a space bar or click when letters appear on the computer screen, excluding the letter “X”. For this study, eight measures that are sensitive to explain the response patterns of inattentiveness were used, including omissions, commissions, hit reaction time, hit reaction time standardized error, variability, detectability, hit reaction time inter-stimulus-interval change, and hit standardized error inter-stimulus-interval change. The CPT consisted of six blocks and three sub-blocks, each containing 20 letters [70], and the completion time was approximately 14 min.

2.3. Procedures

The Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the University of Miami approved this study (no. 20081058). Once permission was obtained from the patient recruitment site, a neurorehabilitation programme director was identified as eligible to participate in the study. The researcher and staff in the neurorehabilitation programme met with the patients and/or caregivers and explained the purpose of the study. If the patients and/or caregivers showed an interest in participating, the programme staff arranged a time and place for the experiments. At the time of administration, the participants completed a demographic questionnaire and completed the MAA. The MAA started with a practice session in which participants were asked to identify the directions of the contours until they could correctly identify more than 80% of the directions. In a test session, each of the five subtests was initiated with a brief instruction in terms of the task characteristics given in each subtest and how to respond to test items. Participants were also instructed to identify the directions of the target contours by circling the arrow corresponding to the contour direction given in the answer sheet. In the sustained attention-basic and -advanced, selective attention, and divided attention-basic, participants were asked to identify the direction of a target contour, while they were asked to identify the directions of two target contours. A total amount of time to complete the MAA was approximately 30 min. The volume was the same for each participant, and the researcher was able to provide guidance and answer all questions. The experiment was performed in a sound-proof room to control for other noises. The ambient light and temperature remained constant throughout the experimental sessions. Within two weeks prior to or after administration of the MAA, the staff in the neurorehabilitation programme conducted a psychological evaluation with the patients using the WASI-IV DST, D-KEFS CWIT, and Conners’ CPT.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

This study employed a descriptive-correlational design to examine the criterion validity between the MAA and different types of cognitive function. Data from the score of MAA and cognitive measures were subjected to correlation analysis. Owing to the patients’ availability for psychological evaluation or cancellation, an unequal number of participants were administered the experimental measures, where 22 patients underwent CCPT and 38 patients took the WAIS-IV DST and D-KEFS CWIT. In addition, a regression analysis was performed to examine whether the MAA predicted cognitive capacity using demographic variables.

3. Results

3.1. Correlation Analysis

A total of 38 participants underwent the MAA and a set of neurocognitive assessments. Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics of the measurements.
A correlation analysis was performed between the MAA and attention batteries. The variables taken from each of the four measurements included: (1) the five subtests of the MAA, reconstructed by Jeong (2013); (2) scaled scores of the three subtests and total score of the WAIS-IV DST; (3) scaled scores of the four conditions of the D-KEFS CWIT; and (4) T scores of eight inattention measures of the CCPT (compared with the typical norm). The results showed significant correlations between the tests (Table 4).
Each of the three subtests of the DST was significantly correlated with the four subtests of the MAA, except for sustained attention-basic. These findings indicate the potential of the MAA in measuring attention and working memory capacity; however, the type and level of attention that the researchers obtained were not as sensitively detected by the DST, as expected. The task characteristics given in the DST and MAA were different in terms of information processing strategy, yielding a significant correlation, but were not task-specific. The sustained attention-basic subtest was an exception, possibly because of the low information load compared to each of the three DSTs.
The three conditions of the CWIT, color naming, word reading, and inhibition, were significantly correlated with sustained attention-advanced, selective attention, divided attention-basic, and divided attention-advanced on the MAA. As shown by the DST, sustained attention-basics were not significantly correlated. There was a tendency for the p-values to become more significant between the MAA and cognitive tests requiring a lower or higher level of cognitive demand. For example, color naming and word reading conditions were more highly correlated with sustained attention-advanced and selective attention than between inhibition and inhibition/switching conditions and divided attention-basic. The divided attention-advanced of the MAA was correlated only with the inhibition/switching condition, indicating shared task characteristics and high cognitive demand between the two subtests. The inhibition condition correlated with the overall subtests of the MAA, indicating shared task characteristics requiring a focus on the target over irrelevant stimuli.
The eight inattention measures of the CCPT were subjected to a correlation analysis and yielded a significant correlation between hit RT SE, variability, hit RT ISI change, and hit SE ISI change. The selective attention and divided attention-advanced subtests correlated with hit RT SE and variability, indicating that the two music subtests were more sensitive to measurements of response speed consistency and variability over the course of the assessment. The divided attention-basic subtest correlated with the hit RT ISI change and hit SE ISI change, indicating changes in reaction time and its consistency over time. Overall, a higher level of subtests of the MAA, in terms of attention hierarchy, was significantly related to the inattention measures of the CCPT, indicating the potential of the MAA as an indicator of auditory inattention.

3.2. Multiple Regression Analysis

A multiple regression (MR) analysis was conducted to determine whether the MAA performance and demographic variables predicted the total value of cognitive capacity. The MAA performance was calculated by summing the four subtest scores, and cognitive capacity was scaled to the total scores of the WAIS-IV DST. Prior to the MR analysis, a correlation analysis was performed. Table 5 presents the correlations among the demographic variables, MAA performance, and cognitive capacity.
The findings showed that the MAA performance and years of education explained a significant amount of the variance in the cognitive capacity as measured by the WAIS-IV DST (F (2, 34) = 6.31, p < 0.01, R2 = 0.52, R2adjusted = 0.23). The analysis showed that MAA performance significantly predicted general cognitive capacity (β = 0.39, t (34) = 2.46, p < 0.05), whereas years of education did not (β = 0.23, t (34) = 1.47, p = 0.15). The years of music education were subjected to an MR analysis because there was a significant correlation between education and music education. The change in R2 was not significant (F (3, 33) = 0.02, p = 0.35), indicating that the addition of this variable did not increase the total variance in cognitive capacity. Table 6 shows the MAA performance with the other variables, accounting for the variance in general cognitive capacity.

4. Discussion

This study investigated the criterion validity of the MAA and existing neurocognitive batteries. The results showed that melody identification tasks with different textures, which mimic a real auditory environment, can measure attention ability in general. Significant correlations with several subtests of neuropsychological measures explained the criterion validity of the MAA. A significant correlation between the CWIT inhibition/switching condition and the MAA divided attention-advanced subtest indicated that the MAA may have the potential to differentiate which type and level of attention is affected in individuals with TBI. In addition, the hit RT SE and variability measure of the CCPT were significantly correlated only with the MAA selective attention and divided attention subtests. This finding indicates that dysfunctions in inattentiveness and variability that contribute to increased distractibility were possibly detected by the MAA. Additionally, the MR analysis demonstrated the potential of using MAA as a predictor of cognitive capacity.

4.1. MAA as a Measure of Attention and Working Memory

Significant correlations with the WAIS-IV DST suggest the potential of the MAA to measure attention and working memory capacity. Acoustic properties and structural elements of music in the MAA utilize bottom-up resources in the brain and activate regions of attention and working memory [52,71,72]. The given tasks designed for the purpose of subtests of the MAA increase the relevance of the stimuli and may attract more attention resources to the stimuli [73]. Stimulus- and goal-driven attention interact in a reciprocal manner [74,75], and the intercommunication between the two processes is key to providing a rationale for using the MAA as an attention assessment tool for TBI.
Despite evidence of the MAA being an appropriate assessment task, the type and level of attention were not sufficiently differentiated through the administration of the DST only. Considering that the DST utilizes a single auditory stream that is verbally presented, the task characteristics of the DST may yield significant correlations but may not be specific to the subtests addressing the assessment of different attention types. In addition, a single subtest (sustained attention-basic) of the five subtests of the MAA did not show a statistically significant correlation with the DST. This exception was possibly due to the relatively low cognitive load of the sustained attention-basic, suggesting the need for revision of the items in the subtest.

4.2. MAA as a Measure of Different Types of Attention

The color–word interference test is a measure of selective and alternating attention. It was significantly correlated with the four subtests of the MAA. The coefficient values tended to increase as the hierarchy increased in attention types. Tasks with a relatively low cognitive load, such as the sustained attention-advanced and word reading condition and the divided attention-basic and inhibition/switching condition, were more highly correlated than the other pairs. Importantly, the divided attention-advanced subtest was correlated solely with the inhibition/switching condition, which required the highest cognitive demand among the four conditions of the CWIT. These findings suggest that the tasks in each of the five subtests of the MAA are hierarchically organized to sensitively measure the level and type of attention.
Individuals utilize and activate different attention strategies while listening to various textures in which musical structures are arrayed. According to the music attention model [45], listening to two or more music streams simultaneously activates different attention types, such as alternating and divided attention. Neurological evidence also supports the view that multi-voice music listening can lead to the activation of neural regions that house each type of attention [51,52,72,76]. These findings support the aforementioned studies reporting the connection between music and attention, and further suggest its use as a different type of auditory attention. Specifically, the correlation with the CWIT articulated the role of the MAA in measuring different levels and types of attention, which was not shown in the relationship with the DST. The given tasks included various musical elements of the MAA.

4.3. MAA and the Distractibility Assessment

The eight inattention measures of the CCPT were subjected to a correlation analysis and yielded a significant correlation between hit RT SE, variability, hit RT ISI change, and hit SE ISI change. The selective attention and divided attention-advanced subtests were correlated with hit RT SE and variability, indicating that the two subtests of the MAA were more sensitive in measuring response speed consistency and its variability over the course of the assessment. The divided attention-basic subtest was correlated with hit RT ISI change and hit SE ISI changes, indicating changes in reaction time and its consistency over time. Overall, the higher level of the subtests of the MAA in terms of attention hierarchy was significantly related to the inattention measures of the CCPT, suggesting the potential of MAA as an indicator of auditory inattention.

4.4. The MAA as a Predictor of Cognitive Capacity

The MAA performance with years of education, rather than severity, age, or time since brain injury, was found to predict general cognitive capacity. Several factors are strongly associated with cognitive deficits and recovery following a TBI. Among various individual variables, brain injury severity is considered the main factor that determines cognitive performance out of various individual variables [77,78]. The significant correlation found between cognitive capacity and level of education was inconsistent with previous studies and may indicate that individuals with a higher level of education may utilize a more focused effort learned from their educational experience [79]. However, since the MAA utilizes novel materials and tasks rather than numbers, words, or geometrical figures with complex verbal instructions, it is difficult to interpret the influence of learned knowledge.
Interestingly, the years of music education were not found to account for the additional variance in existing cognitive capacity, although the MAA primarily utilized music-based materials. This finding may indicate that the processing of music-based information given in the MAA does not necessarily require a specific level of music sensitivity or talent and, thus, suggests the potential of using the MAA for people who do not have experience in musical training.

5. Conclusions

Evidently, the MAA utilizes melodic identification tasks with different musical textures and task demands to measure general cognitive capacity, different types of attention, as well as distractibility which is a causal factor of inattentiveness. Melodic contours weaved into different textures in the MAA may closely mimic a real-world auditory environment and thus provide an ecologically valid measure of auditory attention. Additional structural elements of music, such as instrumental timbres and pitch range, provide a method for randomizing and creating music assessment items and further extends them to the generalized item pool of attention training. One of the advantages of the MAA is that the receptive modality of music requires minimal prerequisite abilities and, therefore, may be applied to individuals who have either temporary or permanent difficulty in understanding complex verbal instructions. In addition, music processing enhances cognitive functions by affecting elevation, which helps maintain an optimized arousal state for assessment and prevents cognitive fatigue during the assessment process.
This study has several limitations, including its small sample size (n = 38) and the skewed female-to-male ratio. It seems to reflect the epidemiology of TBI, reporting that men are approximately 40% more likely to suffer a TBI compared with women [80,81,82], In this study, we found no sex-related differences in any of the musical or cognitive–behavioral responses. However, in future studies, it would be necessary to balance the gender ratio with an increase in sample size. Another limitation of the study could be a non-probability convenience sampling from a single medical center. Although it is often used in clinical research [83], care should be taken in attempting the generalization of data.
Future suggestions include reliability and validity investigations with a larger group of people in which the normal distribution of the recovery phase and injury severity is assumed and who are from various sociocultural backgrounds. A comparison of attention performance depending on the variability of neuropathology could be evaluated to validate the use of the MAA. Parallel observation using brain imaging techniques would also contribute to establishing clinical evidence and a model of music and attention. Lastly, cognitive performance is measured by externalized behaviors; therefore, a greater variety of behavioral responses related to attention function, such as reaction time and task completion time, must be included in the MAA. The development of a computerized version of the MAA would provide appropriate solutions for a multifaceted measurement of attention function.
Neurocognitive science has illuminated the cognitive abilities that underlie music-related activities. Music is believed to be a powerful tool for people with and without previous music training and talent, and therefore it is important to identify the basic units of behavior involving listening and making music, and investigating their relationship with cognitive behaviors. The development, validation, and frequent use of a tangible measure of music behavior could provide alternatives to prove the efficacy of music therapy in enhancing a broad spectrum of functional behaviors.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.J.; Methodology, E.J. and S.J.I.; Software, E.J.; Validation, E.J.; Formal analysis, E.J. and S.J.I.; Investigation, E.J. and S.J.I.; Resources, E.J.; Data curation, E.J. and S.J.I.; Writing—original draft, E.J.; Writing—review & editing, E.J. and S.J.I.; Visualization, E.J.; Supervision, E.J.; Project administration, E.J. and S.J.I.; Funding acquisition, E.J. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea(NRF) grant funded by the Korea government(MSIT) (NRF-2020R1F1A1076024).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of University of Miami ethics committee (HSRO 20081058).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kashluba, S.; Hanks, R.A.; Casey, J.E.; Millis, S.R. Neuropsychologic and functional outcome after complicated mild traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2008, 89, 904–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Arciniegas, D.B.; Held, K.; Wagner, P. Cognitive impairment following TBI. Curr. Treat. Options Neurol. 2002, 4, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ashby, F.G.; Isen, A.M.; Turken, A.U. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol. Rev. 1999, 106, 529–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Barrow, I.M.; Collins, J.N.; Britt, L.D. The influence of an auditory distraction on rapid naming after a mild traumatic brain injury: A longitudinal study. J. Trauma Acute Care Surg. 2006, 61, 1142–1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Holmqvist, K.; Kamwendo, K.; Ivarsson, A.B. Occupational therapists’ descriptions of their work with persons suffering from cognitive impairment following acquired brain injury. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 2009, 16, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Satoh, M.; Takeda, K.; Nagata, K.; Hatazawa, J.; Kuzuhara, S. Activated brain regions in musicians during an ensemble: A PET study. Cogn. Brain Res. 2001, 12, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Tsaousides, T.; Gordon, W.A. Cognitive rehabilitation following traumatic brain injury: Assessment to treatment. Mt. Sinai J. Med. 2009, 76, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hattiangadi, N.; Pillion, J.P.; Slomine, B.; Christensen, J.; Trovato, M.K.; Speedie, L.J. Characteristics of auditory agnosia in a child with severe traumatic brain injury: A case report. Brain Lang. 2005, 92, 12–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Loetscher, T.; Lincoln, N.B. Cognitive rehabilitation for attention deficits following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2013, 2013, CD002842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Murakami, T.; Hama, S.; Yamashita, H.; Onoda, K.; Hibino, S.; Sato, H.; Ogawa, S.; Yamawaki, S.; Kurisu, K. Neuroanatomic pathway associated with attentional deficits after stroke. Brain Res. 2014, 1544, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Scheibel, R.S.; Newsome, M.R.; Steinberg, J.L.; Pearson, D.A.; Rauch, R.A.; Mao, H.; Troyanskaya, M.; Sharma, R.G.; Levin, H.S. Altered brain activation during cognitive control in patients with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabilit. Neural Repair 2007, 21, 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Barker-Collo, S.; Feigin, V.; Lawes, C.; Parag, V.; Senior, H. Attention deficits after incident stroke in the acute period: Frequency across types of attention and relationships to patient characteristics and functional outcomes. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 2010, 17, 463–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Sinclair, K.L.; Ponsford, J.L.; Rajaratnam, S.M.W.; Anderson, C. Sustained attention following traumatic brain injury: Use of the Psychomotor Vigilance Task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2013, 35, 210–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Snow, J.C.; Mattingley, J.B. Goal-driven selective attention in patients with right hemisphere lesions: How intact is the ipsilesional field? Brain 2006, 129, 168–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Ziino, C.; Ponsford, J. Vigilance and fatigue following traumatic brain injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2006, 12, 100–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ben-David, B.M.; Nguyen, L.L.; Van Lieshout, P.H. Stroop effects in persons with traumatic brain injury: Selective attention, speed of processing, or color-naming? A meta-analysis. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 354–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mayer, A.R.; Yang, Z.; Yeo, R.A.; Pena, A.; Ling, J.M.; Mannell, M.V.; Stippler, M.; Mojtahed, K. A functional MRI study of multimodal selective attention following mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012, 6, 343–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Stapleton, T.; Ashburn, A.; Stack, E. A pilot study of attention deficits, balance control and falls in the subacute stage following stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 2001, 15, 437–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Azouvi, P.; Couillet, J.; Leclercq, M.; Martin, Y.; Asloun, S.; Rousseaux, M. Divided attention and mental effort after severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1260–1268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Blanchet, S.; Paradis-Giroux, A.-A.; Pépin, M.; McKerral, M. Impact of divided attention during verbal learning in young adults following mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2009, 23, 111–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Chung, C.S.Y.; Pollock, A.; Campbell, T.; Durward, B.R.; Hagen, S. Cognitive rehabilitation for executive dysfunction in adults with stroke or other adult non-progressive acquired brain damage. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2013, 2013, CD008391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Mathias, J.L.; Wheaton, P. Changes in attention and information-processing speed following severe traumatic brain injury: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 2007, 21, 212–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Paré, N.; Rabin, L.A.; Fogel, J.; Pépin, M. Mild traumatic brain injury and its sequelae: Characterisation of divided attention deficits. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2009, 19, 110–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Poulin, V.; Korner-Bitensky, N.; Dawson, D.R.; Bherer, L. Efficacy of executive function interventions after stroke: A systematic review. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 2012, 19, 158–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kaipio, M.L.; Cheour, M.; Ceponiene, R.; Ohman, J.; Alku, P.; Näätänen, R. Increased distractibility in closed head injury as revealed by event-related potentials. Neuroreport 2000, 11, 1463–1468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Marsh, J.E.; Vachon, F.; Jones, D.M. When does between-sequence phonological similarity promote irrelevant sound disruption? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2008, 34, 243–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Rinne, T.; Särkkä, A.; Degerman, A.; Schröger, E.; Alho, K. Two separate mechanisms underlie auditory change detection and involuntary control of attention. Brain Res. 2006, 1077, 135–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Sarno, S.; Erasmus, L.P.; Frey, M.; Lippert, G.; Lipp, B. Electrophysiological correlates of active and passive attentional states after severe traumatic brain injury. Funct. Neurol. 2006, 21, 21–29. [Google Scholar]
  29. Botvinick, M.M.; Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Carter, C.S.; Cohen, J.D. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001, 108, 624–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ponsford, J.; Olver, J.H.; Curran, C. A profile of outcome: 2 Years after traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 1995, 9, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Sarno, S.; Erasmus, I.P.; Lipp, B.; Schlaegel, W. Multisensory integration after traumatic brain injury: A reaction time study between pairings of vision, touch, and audition. Brain Inj. 2003, 17, 413–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kaipio, M.L.; Cheour, M.; Öhman, J.; Salonen, O.; Näätänen, R. Mismatch negativity abnormality in traumatic brain injury without macroscopic lesions on conventional MRI. NeuroReport 2013, 24, 440–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Kewman, D.G.; Yanus, B.; Kirsch, N. Assessment of distractibility in auditory comprehension after traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 1988, 2, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Lew, H.; Lee, E.; Pan, S.; Date, E. Electrophysiologic abnormalities of auditory and visual information processing in patients with traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2004, 83, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Schaefer, M.; Tewes, U.; Münte, T.F.; Johannes, S. Lateralized irrelevant speech alters visuospatial selective attention mechanisms. Biol. Psychol. 2006, 72, 51–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Solbakk, A.K.; Reinvang, I.; Andersson, S. Assessment of P3a and P3b after moderate to severe brain injury. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 2002, 33, 102–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Wilson, M.J.; Harkrider, A.W.; King, K.A. Effects of complexity of visual distracters on attention and information processing speed reflected in auditory p300. Ear Hear. 2012, 33, 480–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Escera, C.; Alho, K.; Schröger, E.; Winkler, I. Involuntary attention and distractibility as evaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiol. Neuro-Otol. 2000, 5, 151–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Schweizer, T.A.; Kan, K.; Hung, Y.; Tam, F.; Naglie, G.; Graham, S. Brain activity during driving with distraction: An immersive fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th ed.; Pearson Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  41. Gronwall, D.M.A. Paced auditory serial-addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1977, 44, 367–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Calabrò, R.S.; Naro, A.; Filoni, S.; Pullia, M.; Billeri, L.; Tomasello, P. Walking to your right music: A randomized controlled trial on the novel use of treadmill plus music in Parkinson’s disease. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 2019, 16, 68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Speranza, L.; Pulcrano, S.; Perrone-Capano, C.; di Porzio, U.; Volpicelli, F. Music affects functional brain connectivity and is effective in the treatment of neurological disorders. Rev. Neurosci. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Snyder, J.S.; Alain, C. Toward a neurophysiological theory of auditory stream segregation. Psychol. Bull. 2007, 133, 780–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Bigand, E.; McAdams, S.; Forêt, S. Divided attention in music. Int. J. Psychol. 2000, 35, 270–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Crawley, E.J.; Acker-Mills, B.E.; Pastore, R.E.; Weil, S. Change detection in multi-voice music: The role of musical structure, musical training, and task demands. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2002, 28, 367–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Davison, L.L.; Banks, W.P. Selective attention in two-part counterpoint. Music Percept. 2003, 21, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gallun, F.J.; Mason, C.R.; Kidd, G., Jr. Task-dependent costs in processing two simultaneous auditory stimuli. Percept. Psychophys. 2007, 69, 757–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Shinn-Cunningham, B.; Ihlefeld, A. Selective and divided attention: Extracting information from simultaneous sound sources. In Proceedings of the ICAD’04: Tenth Meeting of the International Conference on Auditory Display, Sydney, Australia, 6–9 July 2004. [Google Scholar]
  50. Slovarp, L.; Azuma, T.; Lapointe, L. The effect of traumatic brain injury on sustained attention and working memory. Brain Injury. 2012, 26, 48–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Janata, P. Brain networks that track musical structure. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2005, 1060, 111–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Janata, P.; Tillmann, B.; Bharucha, J.J. Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2002, 2, 121–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Trainor, L.J.; McDonald, K.L.; Alain, C. Automatic and controlled processing of melodic contour and interval information measured by electrical brain activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 430–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bengtsson, S.L.; Ullen, F.; Ehrsson, H.H.; Hashimoto, T.; Kito, T.; Naito, E.; Forssberg, H.; Sadato, N. Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices. Cortex 2009, 45, 62–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Macaluso, E.; Frith, C.D.; Driver, J. Directing attention to locations and to sensory modalities: Multiple levels of selective processing revealed with PET. Cereb. Cortex 2002, 12, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Mirsky, A.F.; Anthony, B.J.; Duncan, C.C.; Aheam, M.B.; Kellam, S.G. Analysis of the elements of attention: A neurophysiological approach. Neuropsychol. Rev. 1991, 2, 109–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ponsford, J. Rehabilitation of attention following traumatic brain injury. In Cognitive Neurorehabilitation, 2nd ed.; Stuss, D.T., Winocur, G., Roberston, I.H., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 507–521. [Google Scholar]
  58. Serences, J.T.; Yantis, S. Spatially selective representations of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional priority in human occipital, parietal, and frontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 284–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Talsma, D.; Kok, A. Nonspatial intermodal selective attention is mediated by sensory brain areas: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 2001, 38, 736–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Jeong, E.; Lesiuk, T. Development and preliminary evaluation of a music-based attention assessment for patients with traumatic brain injury. J. Music Ther. 2011, 48, 551–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Jeong, E. Psychometric validation of a music-based attention assessment: Revised for patients with traumatic brain injury. J. Music Ther. 2013, 50, 66–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Jeong, E.; Ryu, H. Melodic contour identification reflects the cognitive threshold of aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 8, 134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Jeong, E.; Ryu, H.; Jo, G.; Kim, J. Cognitive load changes during music listening and its implication in earcon design in public environments: An fNIRS study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  64. Jeong, E.; Ryu, H.; Shin, J.-H.; Kwon, G.H.; Jo, G.; Lee, J.-Y. High oxygen exchange to music indicates auditory distractibility in acquired brain injury: An fNIRS study with a vector-based phase analysis. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 16737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Jo, G.; Kim, Y.-M.; Jun, D.W.; Jeong, E. Pitch processing can indicate cognitive alterations in chronic liver disease: An fNIRS study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 535775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Franzen, M.D. Neuropsychological assessment in traumatic brain injury. Crit. Care Nurs. Q. 2000, 23, 58–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Léon-Carrión, J.; von Wild, K.R.H.; Zitnay, G.A. Neuropsychological assessment of persons with acquired brain injury. In Brain Injury Treatment: Theories and Practices; Léon-Carrión, J., Taaffe, P.J., Martín, J.M., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2006; pp. 401–440. [Google Scholar]
  68. Delis, D.C.; Kaplan, E.; Kramer, J.H. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  69. Keifer, E.; Tranel, D. A neuropsychological investigation of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2013, 35, 1048–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Conners, C.K.; Staff, M.H.S. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II: Computer Program for Windows Technical Guide and Softwqre Manual; Multi-Health Systems, Inc.: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  71. Stewart, L.; Von Kriegstein, K.; Warren, J.D.; Griffiths, T.D. Music and the brain: Disorders of musical listening. Brain 2006, 12 Pt 10, 2533–2553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Zatorre, R.J.; Evans, A.C.; Meyer, E. Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch. J. Neurosci. 1994, 14, 1908–1919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Legrain, V.; Van Damme, S.; Eccleston, C.; Davis, K.D.; Seminowicz, D.A.; Crombez, G. A neurocognitive model of attention to pain: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Pain 2009, 144, 230–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Buschman, T.J.; Miller, E.K. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 2007, 315, 1860–1862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Fritz, J.B.; Elhilali, M.; David, S.V.; Shamma, S.A. Auditory attention—Focusing the searchlight on sound. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007, 17, 437–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Uhlig, M.; Fairhurst, M.T.; Keller, P.E. The importance of integration and top-down salience when listening to complex multi-part musical stimuli. Neuroimage 2013, 77, 52–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Draper, K.; Ponsford, J. Cognitive functioning ten years following traumatic brain injury and rehabilitation. Neuropsychology 2008, 22, 618–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Willmott, C.; Ponsford, J.; Hocking, C.; Schönberger, M. Factors contributing to attentional impairments after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 2009, 23, 424–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Stulemeijer, M.; Andriessen, T.M.J.C.; Brauer, J.M.P.; Vos, P.E.; Van Der Werf, S. Cognitive performance after Mild Traumatic Brain Injury: The impact of poor effort on test results and its relation to distress, personality and litigation. Brain Inj. 2007, 21, 309–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Coronado, R.A.; Gay, C.W.; Bialosky, J.E.; Carnaby, G.D.; Bishop, M.D.; George, S.Z. Changes in pain sensitivity following spinal manipulation: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 2012, 22, 752–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Coronado, V.G.; McGuire, L.C.; Sarmiento, K.; Bell, J.; Lionbarger, M.R.; Jones, C.D.; Geller, A.I.; Khoury, N.; Xu, L. Trends in traumatic brain injury in the U.S. and the public health response: 1995–2009. J. Saf. Res. 2012, 43, 299–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Faul, M.; Coronado, V. Epidemiology of traumatic brain injury. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2015, 127, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  83. Stratton, S.J. Population research: Convenience sampling strategies. Prehospital Disaster Med. 2021, 36, 373–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Demographic characteristics.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics.
n%
GenderMale3075
Female1025
EthnicityCaucasian512.5
African American1230
Hispanic2375
Data since brain injuryLess than a year1025
1 to 2 years1230
More than 2 years1845
Table 2. Structure of the 45-item MAA.
Table 2. Structure of the 45-item MAA.
SubtestsItem#StimuliContextsTasks
Sustained
Attention
Basic4A set of five tones A single auditory stream To identify the direction(s) of the melodic contour
Advanced13Two to three sets of five tones
Selective
Attention
6One to three set(s) of five tones and environmental soundsTwo auditory streams: One conveying task-relevant information and the other delivering distracting sounds To identify the direction(s) of the melodic contour in the presence of distraction
Divided
Attention
Basic16One to two set(s) of five tones Two auditory streams: both conveying task-relevant information To identify the direction(s) of the melodic contour in the presence of more competing sounds
Advanced6Three sets of five tones To identify two direction(s) of melodic contours
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of the MAA and neurocognitive batteries.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of the MAA and neurocognitive batteries.
MSDMinimumMaximum
MAASustained Attention-Basic3.351.2104
Sustained Attention-Advanced8.184.09014
Selective Attention3.401.8806
Divided Attention-Basic5.483.99016
Divided Attention-Advanced0.080.2701
Total20.489.76241
DSTForward7.743.49217
Backward7.872.30315
Sequencing6.472.68214
Total5.322.16111
CWITColor naming6.244.26113
Word reading6.494.51114
Inhibition7.084.94115
Inhibition/Switching6.244.83115
CCPTOmissions82.0278.0840.86321.03
Commissions48.7211.1833.3873.90
Hit RT56.0516.7516.7986.44
Hit RT SE57.0017.6133.1594.71
Variability55.6214.6730.1189.78
Detectability47.6011.1621.4366.40
Hit RT ISI Change51.0112.4724.4890.18
Hit SE ISI Change50.179.6535.9272.54
Note: n = 38 (MAA), n = 38 (CWIT, DST), and n = 22 (CCPT); RT, reaction time; SE, standardized error; ISI, inter-stimulus interval.
Table 4. Correlations between the MAA and neurocognitive batteries.
Table 4. Correlations between the MAA and neurocognitive batteries.
MAASustained
Attention-Basic
Sustained
Attention-
Advanced
Selective
Attention
Divided
Attention-Basic
Divided
Attention-
Advanced
Total
Subtests
DSTForward0.170.310.39 *0.36 *0.39 *0.40 *
Backward0.030.54 ***0.52 **0.41 *0.45 **0.51 **
Sequencing0.170.35 *0.46 **0.32 *0.39 *0.40 *
CWITColor naming−0.000.50 **0.50 **0.41 *0.150.48 **
Word reading0.010.51 **0.47 **0.39 *0.150.47 **
Inhibition−0.150.46 **0.43 **0.43 **0.240.44 **
Inhibition/Switching−0.070.36 *0.36 *0.44 **0.38 *0.40 *
CCPTHit RT SE0.08−0.36−0.43 *−0.20−0.38 *−0.39 *
Variability0.02−0.44 *−0.54 **−0.34−0.49 **−0.48 **
Detectability0.010.170.25−0.010.170.23
Hit RT ISI Change0.130.240.250.45 *0.320.21
Hit SE ISI Change0.210.320.38 *0.51 **0.42 *0.28
Note: n = 38 (CWIT, DST), n = 22 (CCPT); * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Correlations between the demographic variables, MAA, and neurocognitive batteries.
Table 5. Correlations between the demographic variables, MAA, and neurocognitive batteries.
AgeDateEducationMusic EducationMAADST
Age-
Date0.13-
Education0.160.14-
Music education0.290.290.50 **-
MAA−0.640.650.37 *0.16-
DST 0.230.030.32 *0.130.40 *-
Note: n = 38 for this analysis, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 6. Comparison of the MR models.
Table 6. Comparison of the MR models.
ModelVariablesR2R2 ChangeFβ
1MAA performance0.470.2310.13 **0.47 **
2MAA performance0.520.056.31 **0.39 *
Education 0.23
3MAA performance0.540.024.50 **0.38 *
Education 0.31
Music education −0.16
Note: n = 38 for this analysis, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jeong, E.; Ireland, S.J. Criterion-Related Validation of a Music-Based Attention Assessment for Individuals with Traumatic Brain Injury. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 16285. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192316285

AMA Style

Jeong E, Ireland SJ. Criterion-Related Validation of a Music-Based Attention Assessment for Individuals with Traumatic Brain Injury. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(23):16285. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192316285

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jeong, Eunju, and Susan J. Ireland. 2022. "Criterion-Related Validation of a Music-Based Attention Assessment for Individuals with Traumatic Brain Injury" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 23: 16285. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph192316285

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop