Next Article in Journal
Influence of Analytic Processing on Divergent and Convergent Thinking Tasks: The Role of Rational and Experiential Thinking Styles
Next Article in Special Issue
Does ICT Usage Have a Positive or Negative Effect on Taiwanese Older Adults’ Emotional Experiences? The Moderating Role of Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction
Previous Article in Journal
Navigating the Spectrum of Conventionality: Toward a New Model of Creative Thinking
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Fear of Missing Out, Emotional Intelligence and Attachment in Older Adults in Argentina

by
Marian Durao
1,2,
Edgardo Etchezahar
1,2,3,
Miguel Ángel Albalá Genol
1,* and
Mariela Muller
1,2
1
Faculty of Education, International University of Valencia, 46002 Valencia, Spain
2
Faculty of Psychology, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires 1053, Argentina
3
National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires 1428, Argentina
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 3 November 2022 / Revised: 6 January 2023 / Accepted: 18 January 2023 / Published: 21 January 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Emotion and Aging)

Abstract

:
In recent years, the rise of social networks has changed relationships and lifestyles around the world. This has led to the emergence of the Fear of Missing Out (FoMO), which consists of the need to constantly check social media and the anguish that comes from feeling a lack of rewarding experiences. The impact derived from the use of technologies in a digital environment has been widely studied in young people but not so much in older adults. The main aim of this study was to analyze FoMO levels in older adults and their relationships with sociodemographic and formative factors. Another aim of this study was to analyze whether the dimensions of emotional intelligence, the different forms of attachment and psychological symptomatology affect the FoMO levels of older adults. A total of 690 older adults from Argentina aged between 60 and 90 years (M = 69.01; SD = 5.48) participated, 54.5% of whom were women, responding using a geolocated online questionnaire. The main results confirmed that older people show FoMO levels similar to other general samples. In addition, results show several predictor variables with respect to FoMO: emotional attention, insecure attachment, depression and difficulties in interpersonal relationships. The implications of the results observed in older adults are discussed.

1. Introduction

In recent years, the exponential growth in the use of technology has significantly facilitated the reduction of social and emotional distance between people, particularly since the emergence of COVID-19. In this situation, older adults (people over 65 years of age) were affected (Haase et al. 2021) because of the risk of suffering mental health problems derived from social isolation (Oddone and Pochintesta 2021; OMS 2021).
Despite the obvious benefits of social media and the digital world, they are not without their challenges in the area of mental health (Scheinfeld and Voorhees 2022). One of the negative aspects of the use of social media is the so-called Fear of Missing Out (FoMO): the desire to be permanently informed of what is happening to others on social networking sites, which leads to an excessive use of social networks, associated with the belief that others are experiencing more pleasurable situations than one’s own (Przybylski et al. 2013). FoMO leads to various cognitive and emotional aspects that affect mental health and social relationships (Hayran and Anik 2021). Recent studies in the Argentine population (Cohen et al. 2020; Cervigni et al. 2022) reported a significant increase in symptoms related to various psychological disorders (e.g., a 43% increase in anxiety, a 29% increase in depression, among others) and social isolation in all age groups.
So far, the study of FoMO has been conducted mainly in a young population. However, older adults have significantly increased their use of technologies and their various functionalities (Fernández and Guglialmelli 2021; Liu et al. 2016; Zhang et al. 2021), necessitating their study, specifically in the older population.

1.1. Emotional Intelligence as a Possible Protective Factor of FoMO

Emotional intelligence (hereafter EQ) comprises a set of intrapersonal and interpersonal competencies and skills, which influence the way in which individuals relate to their community (Ruvalcaba et al. 2014). According to Goleman (1995, p. 68), it constitutes “a meta-skill that determines the degree of ability to master our other faculties” (Goleman 1995, p. 68). According to this model, emotional intelligence includes (Mayorga-Lascano 2019): attention, i.e., the degree to which people consider that they are aware of their own emotions and feelings; emotional clarity, the way in which people believe they perceive their own emotions; and emotion repair, i.e., the belief in one’s own possibility to regulate one’s emotional states.
There is evidence that EQ is a predictor of both physical and mental health (Schutte et al. 2007; Petrides et al. 2016). In this sense, it is a protective factor against addictions or violent behaviors (González Yubero et al. 2021; Orozco Solis 2021), and it is also related to well-being in older adults, favoring critical thinking, promoting inclusion and modulating FoMO (Bandera-Pastor et al. 2022). Likewise, the relationship between cell phone use and EQ, or between FoMO and EQ, has been documented in numerous studies (Mosa Qutishat 2020). In recent years, various studies have shown the relationship between emotional intelligence and FoMO, which is often considered a protective factor. However, these studies have been conducted mainly in the adolescent and university population or in the general population (Soriano-Sánchez 2022).
Regarding the link between emotional intelligence and FoMO, in young people, it has been shown that it favors academic success and a beneficial use of social media (Iqbal et al. 2021). Emotional intelligence, besides being negatively related to FoMO, acts as a mediator with respect to depression (Kartol and Gündoğan 2020). Thus, there are numerous studies (Casale et al. 2018 ; Hamissi et al. 2013; Van Deursen et al. 2015) that have found negative relationships between EQ and FoMO in young and adult populations, as well as with respect to other variables associated with the development of FoMO (internet and cell phone addiction and abuse, social anxiety, self-regulation, among others). From an applied perspective, Chen et al. (2022) explored the effect of psychological interventions on FoMO that improve emotional intelligence, finding improvements and a reduction of FoMO.
Given the cross-cutting impact of FoMO and emotional intelligence on the entire population, it is necessary to develop studies that specifically consider the population of older adults, thus evaluating the possible differences with respect to the populations already studied. Thus, to guarantee an adequate study of this variable, it is necessary to consider a population with a heterogeneous geographic distribution and a population which is not only represented by large cities (Cole 2009). Therefore, heterogeneity and greater representativeness in relation to the study are favored, taking into account the population from various locations in the country (Argentina).

1.2. Forms of Attachment and Their Implications with Regards to FoMO

In recent years, assorted studies (Blackwell et al. 2017) have shown evidence of the role of distinct attachment profiles in the development of FoMO and other related behaviors. According to attachment theory (Ainsworth 1963; Bowlby 1969), the bond established between a child at an early age and its caregivers lays the foundation for the emotional attachment style of the adult individual. The effects of childhood attachment relationships extend into adulthood, and influence the emotions, relational style and defensive behaviors of individuals (Bowlby 1984) throughout their lives (Soriano-Sánchez 2022). The aforementioned leads to the classification of attachment most commonly used in the psychological field (Bowlby 1984; Main and Solomon 1986) into: secure, avoidant, anxious and ambivalent.
First, secure attachment is shown in adulthood through the ability to have healthy relationships with other people, where an environment of intimacy and closeness can be developed, with adequate autonomy, self-efficacy and self-esteem. Secondly, avoidant attachment is shown as a rejection towards commitment, in which the person may feel a certain fear or refusal to manage relationships that have a degree of intimacy, generating discomfort in them and preferring independence. Thirdly, in the anxious attachment style, the person has a constant need for contact in their relationships, feeling a great discomfort derived from the lack of reciprocity. Finally, ambivalent attachment appears as a great fear of abandonment or loneliness, which generates situations of dependence due to the low personal autonomy developed and the need for constant approval.
Apparently, subjects with an insecure attachment style (anxious, avoidant or ambivalent) would have behaviors that tend to replace the affection of those close to them (i.e., family and friends) through the search for gratification in social media (D’Arienzo et al. 2019; Holte and Ferraro 2020). Specifically, in relation to FoMO and its link with attachment styles, both are significantly related. According to previous studies, FoMO is found to be related to the anxious attachment style rather than the avoidant attachment style. Meanwhile, Blackwell et al. (2017) noted among his findings that anxious and ambivalent attachment styles predict the abusive use of modern technologies; in other words, people often seek to soothe their discomfort through the use of social media. Furthermore, those who show avoidant attachment, although they consider themselves self-sufficient and frequently avoid intimacy, also want to use social media as a way to keep people in their lives but at a distance (Aranda et al. 2022; Nitzburg and Farber 2013). Likewise, people with the ambivalent attachment style are at a greater risk of developing addictive behaviors (Valizadeh et al. 2017). High FoMO levels lead to detachment in adult relationships (Hogan 2015). Therefore, it is necessary to continue to study in depth the degree and type of relationship between the different attachment styles and the development of FoMO and other related variables.

1.3. Psychological Symptoms in FoMO

In recent years, older adults increased their use of technology and social media, sometimes promoted by family and community support (Galica et al. 2021) to mitigate the consequences of social disconnection and its consequences on mental health deterioration, especially during the COVID-19 pandemic (Merchant and Lurie 2020). Many older adults knew how to use technology to connect with others prior to the onset of the pandemic, and many others adopted new technologies from the onset of the pandemic and pointed out problems related to dependence on family and friends due to lack of knowledge (Haase et al. 2021).
However, there is compelling evidence that social connectedness facilitated by the use of technology improves the mental, social and physical well-being of older adults (Baker et al. 2018; Siegel and Dorner 2017). Still, at present, many studies report new problems generated by the excessive use of these technologies (Akbari et al. 2021). Some groups may be more oriented to develop FoMO, such as those characterized by excessive use of social media, regardless of their stage in life. Thus, FoMO may increase with age in some populations (Akbari et al. 2021).
The mechanism underlying FoMO is shaped by cognitive and anxiety-related aspects (Przybylski et al. 2013), which are reflected in subjective discomfort, interpersonal difficulties, depression, problematic use of social media, attention and concentration difficulties, and these aspects also hinder the life experience (Przybylski et al. 2013; Hayran and Anik 2021). The desire to be permanently informed of what is happening to others on social media can generate problems in family interactions (Scheinfeld and Voorhees 2022) and higher levels of loneliness and envy in adults (Barry and Wong 2020), generating social comparisons based on the information they see on social media, which triggers the belief that others are doing something fun and are happier, with a consequent decrease in self-esteem (Buglass et al. 2017).
Specifically, the appearance and development of FoMO is related to a discomfort frequently linked to psychological symptoms (depression, anxiety, loneliness, self-esteem problems, among others) that affect people’s well-being; the appearance and development of FoMO is also related to the abusive use of social media and smartphones (Sánchez-Carbonell et al. 2008). Several studies have empirically corroborated the predictive effect of anxious-depressive, psychotic and interpersonal sensitivity symptoms on FoMO and problematic smartphone use (Gul et al. 2022; Tugtekin et al. 2020), although the sample of participants is usually limited to young people. Thus, the need to investigate in older adults the relationship between FoMO and psychological symptomatology, as well as its effects, is evident.
In accordance with the above, this work had two main objectives: First, to analyze FoMO levels in a sample of older adults and their relationships with gender, age, social class and educational level. Second, to study whether the dimensions of emotional intelligence, different forms of attachment and psychological symptoms affect the levels of FoMO in older adults. Based on the objectives of the study, two hypotheses were raised. On the one hand, according to hypothesis 1, it is expected that FoMO will be found to be present in older people with levels similar to those of other groups of the general population. In this way, aging is not a process that gives rise to these new problems, but rather there will be other psychosocial and socio-emotional variables that need to be analysed. Therefore, it is necessary to study these novel variables in the older population as well as in samples of young people. On the other hand, according to hypothesis 2, the variables studied will show relationships in different directions in relation to FoMO; emotional intelligence will be related in a negative sense, as well as the type of secure attachment. In contrast, it is expected to find positive relationships between FoMO and insecure attachment styles, as well as with the psychological symptoms explored. These relationships with psychosocial variables will therefore be relevant to explain the levels of FOMO in older people.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A geolocalized online questionnaire was administered, with stratified sampling based on the geographical regions of Argentina (see Table 1). The complete and valid protocols totaled 690 cases (with a sampling error of ±2.5% and a confidence level of 95%) from urban environments. The average age of the participants was 69.01 years (SD = 5.48), with an age range between 60 and 80 years. We performed a post hoc power analysis (Power = Φ (−z1 − α/2 + |μ0 − μ1| xn√n/σ), taking into account about two million people between 60 and 80 who reside in Argentina according to the INDEC (2018), and the sample size was adequate (Φ (26,265.858) = 1 = 100% power. Of the participants, 54.5% (n = 376) identified as female, while 45.5% (n = 314) identified as male. With respect to educational level, 4.6% of the sample had only primary education, 26.2% had secondary education, 31.2% had tertiary education and 38% had completed their university studies.

2.2. Measures

Self-report measures were employed using a battery of assessment instruments consisting of:
Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) Scale. To assess the construct, we proceeded to perform the adaptation and validation of the original version of the scale (Przybylski et al. 2013), composed of 10 items that determine dimension 1, FOM-NI (e.g., “I fear my friends have more rewarding experiences than me”) and dimension 2, FOM-SO (e.g., “It bothers me when I miss an opportunity to meet up with friends”). Each item was rated on a Likert scale with five anchors, ranging from 1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree (the same response format was used for the other scales used in this study). The higher the score in both dimensions, the higher the levels of FoMO.
Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS-12). A reduced 12-item version of the 12-item Trait Meta-Mood Scale proposed by Salguero et al. (2009) was used, which was derived from the TMMS-24 (Salovey et al. 1995; Fernández-Berrocal et al. 1998). This scale consists of 12 items, which determine three dimensions. The first dimension, emotional attention, represents the ability to monitor one’s own emotions and feelings (e.g., “I pay a lot of attention to the way I feel”); the second dimension, clarity of emotions, refers to the ability to understand and label one’s emotions (e.g., “I can usually define my feelings”); and finally, the third dimension, emotional repair, represents the ability to regulate negative emotional states and prolong the positive ones (e.g., “Although I sometimes feel sad, I usually have an optimistic outlook”). A Likert-type response format ranging from 1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree was used in this study.
Relationship Questionnaire (RQ). A four-item scale designed to measure attachment style in adults was used. This questionnaire is an adapted version of the original, which extends the three measures of attachment (Shaver and Hazan 1987) to four. The version adapted to Spanish, proposed by Alonso-Arbiol et al. (2002) consists of a total of four items, each of which represents an attachment style: secure attachment (“It is easy for me to become emotionally close to others. I am comfortable depending on them and having them depend on me. I don’t worry about being alone or having others not accept me.”); avoidant attachment (“I am comfortable without close emotional relationships. It is very important to me to feel independent and self-sufficient, and I prefer not to depend on others or have others depend on me.”); anxious attachment (“I want to be completely emotionally intimate with others, but I often find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like. I am uncomfortable being without close relationships, but I sometimes worry that others don’t value me as much as I value them.”); and ambivalent attachment (“ I am uncomfortable getting close to others. I want emotionally close relationships, but I find it difficult to trust others completely, or to depend on them. I worry that I will be hurt if I allow myself to become too close to others.”). Each item is rated on a Likert scale with five anchors, ranging from 1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree.
Online Global Symptoms Scale (Escala de Síntomas Globales Online or ESGO): The local version of the ESGO was used to evaluate the construct (Durao et al. 2019), composed in its definitive version of 20 items that account for four dimensions that make up the global evaluation of the symptomatology presented by the individuals. For our study, three of the four dimensions were used; psychoticism was discarded, since it does not have a theoretical link with the study variables. Each of the dimensions is made up of a series of five items: depression (e.g., “I often feel that my problems are unsolvable”; “I don’t enjoy myself as much as I used to”), anxiety (e.g., “At times I feel intense fear and think I’m going to die”; “I have sensations in my body that others can’t understand”), interpersonal relations (e.g., “I tend to be on the lookout for negative judgment”; “I sometimes feel inferior to my peers or friends”). Each item is rated on a Likert scale with five anchors, ranging from 1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree.
Socio-demographic data questionnaire: Information on the gender, age, self-perceived socio-economic level, household typology (lives alone/does not live alone) and highest level of education was collected from the participants.

2.3. Procedure and Data Analysis

People who met the criteria of age (60 and over) and geographic region were invited to participate via social media, based on the quotas stipulated for the sample distribution. Participants were previously informed, at the start, about the purpose of the study, the institution responsible for it and they were provided with a contact e-mail address in case they required further information. Additionally, they were informed that the data collected in this study would only be used for academic-scientific purposes and would be protected in accordance with Argentine National Law 25,326 on the protection of personal data. The questionnaires were collected from March to May 2022. The statistical analyses that guided the development of this study were conducted using SPSS for Windows software version 19.0 (George and Mallery 2010) and EQS 6.1 (Bentler 2007). First, a descriptive analysis of the FoMO-NI and FoMO-SO (means and standard deviations) was conducted. Also, Pearson’s correlations have been estimated to analyze the relationship between the FoMO dimensions, educational level and participants’ age. Moreover, an analysis on the relationships between FoMO, emotional intelligence, attachment and psychological symptoms was conducted. Finally, we performed a multiple linear regression on emotional intelligence, attachment types and symptomatology to explain the variance in FoMO-NI and FoMO-SO.

3. Results

Firstly, when comparing the results of the general population (Durao et al. 2023) with the sample of older adults, no differences were observed in the FoMO means. Regarding FoMO-NI, in the general population, a mean of 2.21 (DT = 1.053) was observed and FoMO-SO had a mean of 3.14 (DT = 1.131). In our sample of older adults with whom we worked in this study, it was observed that for FoMO-NI, the mean was 2.12 (DT = 0.959) and for FoMO-SO the mean was 3.18 (DT = 1.112).
Next, the relationships between FoMO dimensions and participants’ age, educational level and social class were analyzed (Table 2).
As seen in Table 2, the two dimensions of FoMO are significantly related to each other, but no relationships are observed with other variables such as participants’ age, educational level and social class. Likewise, no differences were observed based on participants’ gender.
Subsequently, we proceeded to analyze the relationships between the dimensions of FoMO and emotional intelligence, the various expressions of attachment and psychological symptoms (Table 3).
Finally, a stepwise regression model was tested to analyze which variables make the greatest contribution to FoMO in older adults (Table 3). To this end, different models were calculated, some considering FoMO-NI as the dependent variable and others considering FoMO-SO as the dependent variable. As independent variables, we considered, first, the three dimensions of emotional intelligence, second, the different forms of attachment and, finally, three categories of psychological symptoms.
As seen in Table 4, for FoMO-NI and FoMO-SO, the first dimension, emotional intelligence, is fundamental, since it makes a significant contribution to both dimensions; however, the other two dimensions do not make any contribution to emotional intelligence. Regarding the different forms of attachment, for FoMO-NI, attachment 3 (AT-Anx) and 4 (AT-Amb) are significant, while for FoMO-SO, the contribution of attachment 1 (AT-Secure) is significant. Finally, symptomatology is relevant for FoMO-NI, particularly depression (Deppres.) and interpersonal relationships (Interp. Rel.), which is not the case for FoMO-SO since the change in R-squared is not significant for step 3 and the contribution of interpersonal relationships is very low.

4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to analyze the level of FoMO in older adults and its relationship with various sociodemographic, educational and psychosocial variables. According to the current literature, FoMO has been studied mainly in young people, with older people being an age group that is assumed to be unaffected by this problem, since they are digital migrants (Román-García et al. 2016). However, one of the main findings of the present study showed that there are no significant differences in FoMO levels between older adults and the general population. This seems to indicate that FoMO is a phenomenon that affects all age segments and, therefore, it is necessary to study its possible consequences on the older population. In this sense, and despite the fact that current empirical studies are mostly focused on adolescents and young adults, several authors suggest that FoMO levels increase with age; it is, thus, present in all age groups (Casali and Torres 2021; Fernández and Guglialmelli 2021; Liu et al. 2016; Zhang et al. 2021).
The two dimensions of the FoMO construct in our study replicate the findings of Li et al. (2022): on one hand, the dimension called “Fear of missing novel information” (FOM-NI), and on the other, the dimension called “Fear of missing social opportunities” (FOM-SO). In our sample of older adults, we observed that the values for both dimensions were similar to those found in previous research with adults and young people in similar contexts (Durao et al. 2023). This fact could be derived from the increase in the daily use of technologies (with different functions not only linked to leisure) that has occurred across the population, regardless of their age (Haase et al. 2021), but it may suppose a different scope for the social functioning of the older population. While technology is considered, by younger people, as just another option that enriches social relationships, occasionally for the older population, it could be rather an imposition as the only way of interaction. Based on the first hypothesis, these results are consistent with those evidenced in the correlational analysis, since age did not show relationships with either of the two FoMO factors. On the one hand, FOM-NI seems to be related to the fear that other people are having more rewarding experiences because of the constant updating of information. In this sense, the COVID-19 pandemic may have caused the population of older adults to rely on the people in their environment (Banskota et al. 2020), needing help to connect and participate in virtual meetings and dealing with technical difficulties. On the other hand, FOM-SO is linked to difficulties related to the comprehension of the communicative codes of interaction with respect to the groups to which people belong in their lives. FoMO and the lack of understanding and interpretation of social media may become sources of stress and anxiety (Gil et al. 2015), and even an indicator of psychological and social distress (Vivaldi and Barra 2012).
Based on the second hypothesis, the relationships between FoMO and the analyzed constructs were explored and contrasted. With respect to emotional intelligence (EQ), the findings point to a relationship of FOM-NI with the attention dimension of EQ. In this regard, attention would be involved in the process of constantly checking various digital and technological media in order to have a greater sense of being up-to-date. Furthermore, based on the results, in all cases, attention is a predictor variable of FOM-NI and FOM-SO. Therefore, the ability to monitor one’s own emotions and feelings acts as a predictor of various FoMO-related behaviors in older adults. Attention may be a protective factor against negative symptoms and addictive tendencies or impulsive behaviors (González Yubero et al. 2021; Orozco Solis 2021) in addition to being a protective factor for FoMO and enhancing critical thinking (Bandera-Pastor et al. 2022; Sporzon and López-López 2021). However, high emotional attention values by themselves may generate hypervigilance if the other factors of emotional intelligence (emotional clarity and repair) are not developed. Additionally, we only found a predictive effect of FoMO with respect to the clarity of emotions dimension (EQ) in one of the contrasted models. It is necessary to consider the great relevance of contact and social interactions for the correct development of emotional intelligence (Ruvalcaba et al. 2014), which could be weakening the protective value against FoMO in the older population, as evidenced in studies with younger populations. Considering that improving EQ globally decreases FoMO (Chen et al. 2022), there remains a need to evaluate new intervention models to empirically assess whether improvement in the three dimensions (attention, clarity and repair) could function as a protector of FoMO.
Regarding the relationships between attachment styles and FoMO, the FOM-NI dimension is related to anxious and ambivalent attachment styles. These results are consistent with previous studies (Liu and Ma 2019). In this regard, this bonding is related to the need of people with such attachment styles to be in continuous social contact, combined with avoidant behaviors in the ambivalent type (Aranda et al. 2022; D’Arienzo et al. 2019). In this sense, social media provides a way of social interaction with a certain distance where, additionally, suppressing any type of virtual interaction may be simpler than in face-to-face contexts. In turn, the secure attachment style is related to FOM-SO. This finding could be pointing to possible indirectly positive aspects of FoMO related to the search for social contact and support, although it may manifest with behaviors and consequences that are harmful to the person’s well-being. In the older population, as has been suggested by various organizations and institutions, the use of social media has positive aspects such as facilitating or generating encounters and continuity in interpersonal relationships (Baker et al. 2018; Siegel and Dorner 2017). Thus, the secure attachment type is positioned as a predictor of FOM-SO and not of FOM-NI. However, anxious and ambivalent attachment types appear to be predictors of FOM-NI, which is consistent with several abusive and obsessive behaviors in the use of technologies with the goal of feeling that new information and possible situations are not out of control (Torres Mendoza and Serna Sarrato 2021). Therefore, the secure attachment style affects the presence of FOM-SO more, which can be related to the need to relate to people they are close to and their own social environment. In contrast, insecure attachment styles (anxious and ambivalent) predict FOM-NI, which does not have such a social component and is more related to the need and fear of missing out on novel and rewarding experiences that anyone may be experiencing. In conclusion, in our sample of older people, there is no single attachment style that affects the two dimensions of FoMO in a homogeneous way.
On the other hand, it has been shown that in older adults, FoMO is related to symptoms of depression, anxiety and difficulties in interpersonal relationships. In the case of FOM-NI, depression and difficulties in interpersonal relationships are two clear predictive symptoms. Additionally, only symptoms derived from interpersonal relationships have a predictive value for FOM-SO. In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, the older population has been affected by illness and has been prone to fear of contracting the virus (Girdhar et al. 2020). This has significantly impacted various indicators of their quality of life and interpersonal relationships (Hall 2021; Cervigni et al. 2022), as well as overall mental health (Mukhtar 2020). While the use of social media has facilitated contact with family and friends for this age group, it can also be inferred that the “Fear of Missing Out” is not only present in the face of what others may be experiencing but also associated with how other older adults do or do not manage social media and technology in general. The use of technology enabled and increased social communication in the older population, reducing feelings of isolation and loneliness (Banskota et al. 2020). This is consistent with what has been stated by several authors; although many individuals approach and remain in social media seeking to mitigate their social exclusion, the same social networks may generate discomfort, loneliness and low self-esteem (Merchant and Lurie 2020), as well as harmful effects and emotions linked to FoMO. Given the deficiencies in social interactions that older people have (Akbari et al. 2021), this could make it the psychological symptomatology with the greatest predictive power on FoMO. However, it is suggested to continue exploring depression and anxiety variables in future studies, as well as other possible ones with a greater presence during aging.
As has been shown in this paper, it is necessary to continue developing studies to evaluate both FoMO and the presence and impact of other related variables in older adults. Future studies could also delve deeper into differences with respect to gender, age, and even specifically with participants from different occupations. Additionally, it is suggested that in future studies, different variables that could affect people throughout life may be considered, such as unwanted loneliness, feelings of social exclusion, illnesses, among others. Therefore, it would be advisable to know the impact derived from the use of technologies in a digital environment, such as FoMO levels, on the risk of cognitive impairment. It should be noted that no differences were found in terms of household typology (lives alone/does not live alone) with respect to the rest of the variables studied. This aspect should be taken into account in the future. If we intend to improve the levels of psychological and social well-being in the future, it is essential to continue developing studies and strategies that allow us to evaluate all the new changes and phenomena, as well as the psychological impact they have on society, in order to be able to reverse them.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.D. and E.E.; methodology, E.E.; software, E.E.; validation, E.E. and M.D.; formal analysis, M.D., E.E. and M.Á.A.G.; investigation, M.D., E.E., M.Á.A.G. and M.M.; resources, M.D. and E.E.; data curation, E.E.; writing—original draft preparation, M.Á.A.G., M.D., E.E. and M.M.; writing—review and editing, M.D.; visualization, E.E.; supervision, M.Á.A.G. and E.E.; project administration, M.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the “CEISH—Committee of Evaluation and Monitoring for Research with Human” of the University of Buenos Aires (CEID202118, 17 September 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ainsworth, Mary Dinsmore Saslter. 1963. The development of infant-mother interaction among the Ganda. In Determinants of Infant Behavior. Edited by Bryan Foss. New York: Wiley, pp. 67–104. [Google Scholar]
  2. Akbari, Hassan, Liangzhe Yuan, Rui Qian, Wei-Hong Chuang, Shih-Fu Chang, Yin Cui, and Boqing Gong. 2021. Vatt: Transformers for multimodal self-supervised learning from raw video, audio and text. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 34: 24206–21. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alonso-Arbiol, Itziar, Phillip R. Shaver, and Sagrario Yárnoz. 2002. Insecure attachment, gender roles, and interpersonal dependency in the Basque Country. Personal Relationships 9: 479–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Aranda, María, Marta García-Domingo, Virginia Fuentes, and Rocío Linares. 2022. Inteligencia emocional y apego adulto: Efectos del uso problemático del smartphone. Anales de Psicología 38: 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Baker, Steven, Jeni Warburton, Jenny Waycott, Frances Batchelor, Thuong Hoang, Briony Dow, Elizabeth Ozanne, and Frank Vetere. 2018. Combatting social isolation and increasing social participation of older adults through the use of technology: A systematic review of existing evidence. Australasian Journal on Ageing 37: 184–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Bandera-Pastor, Laura, Quintana Orts Cirenia, and Rey Peña Lourdes. 2022. Afrontamiento resiliente y miedo a perderse algo en tiempos de pandemia por COVID-19: Un estudio piloto sobre el papel moderador de la regulación emocional. Health and Addictions 22: 253–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Banskota, Swechya, Healy Margaret, and Goldberg Elizabeth. 2020. 15 smartphone apps for older adults to use while in isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Western Journal of Emergency Medicine 21: 514–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Barry, Christopher, and Megan Wong. 2020. Fear of missing out (FoMO): A generational phenomenon or an individual difference? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 37: 2952–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bentler, Peter M. 2007. On tests and indices for evaluating structural models. Personality and Individual Differences 42: 825–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Blackwell, David, Leaman Carrie, Tramposch Rose, Osborne Ciera, and Liss Miriam. 2017. Extraversion, neuroticism, attachment style and fear of missing out as predictors of social media use and addiction. Personality and Individual Differences 116: 69–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bowlby, John. 1969. Attachment and Loss. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bowlby, John. 1984. Violence in the family as a disorder of the attachment and caregiving systems. The American Journal of Psychoanalysis 44: 9–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Buglass, Sarah L., Jens F. Binder, Lucy R. Betts, and Jean D. M. Underwood. 2017. Motivators of online vulnerability: The impact of social network site use and FOMO. Computers in Human Behavior 66: 248–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Casale, Silvia, Rugai Laura, and Fioravanti Giulia. 2018. Exploring the role of positive metacognitions in explaining the association between the fear of missing out and social media addiction. Addictive Behaviors 85: 83–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Casali, Ana, and Diego Torres. 2021. Impacto del COVID-19 en docentes universitarios argentinos: Cambio de directrices, dificultades y aumento del estrés. Revista Iberoamericana de Tecnología en Educación y Educación en Tecnología 28: 423–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Cervigni, Mauricio A., Tomás Caycho-Rodríguez, Ignacio Barés, Manuel Calandra, and Miguel Gallegos. 2022. Impacto psicológico de la pandemia por COVID-19 en adultos mayores de Argentina. Revista Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento (RACC) 14: 64–74. [Google Scholar]
  17. Chen, Shih-Chu, Yen-Han Chang, Jing-Han Huang, Ching-Wen Hsu, Chih-Hsuan Lin, and Pei-Yi (Patricia) Kuo. 2022. Exploring the Effect of Emotion Awareness Intervention on Reducing FoMO. In Extended Abstracts of the 2022 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI EA’22). New York: Association for Computing Machinery. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Cohen, Sandra, Manes Rossi Francesca, Caperchione Eugenio, and Brusca Isabel. 2020. Debate If not now then when COVID 19 as an accelerator for public sector accrual accounting in Europe. Public Money & Management 41: 10–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Cole, Elisabeth. 2009. Intersectionality and research in psychology. American Psychologist 64: 170–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. D’Arienzo, Maria Chiara, Valentina Boursier, and Mark D. Griffiths. 2019. Addiction to Social Media and Attachment Styles: A Systematic Literature Review. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 17: 1094–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Durao, Marian, Albalá Genol Miguel Angel, Etchezahar Edgardo, and Muller Mariela. 2023. Adaptation and Validation of the Fear Of Missing Out (FoMO) Scale to the Argentine Context. Manuscript pending of publication. [Google Scholar]
  22. Durao, Marian, Ungaretti Joaquín, Genise Gabriel, and Etchezahar Edgardo. 2019. Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Síntomas Globales Online (ESGO). Ciencias Psicológicas 13: 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Fernández-Berrocal, Pablo, Denis Frank Cunza-Aranzábal, Michael White, Dámaris Quinteros-Zúñiga, Jania Elizabeth Jaimes-Soncco, and Wilter C. Morales-García. 1998. Adaptación al castellano de la escala rasgo de metaconocimiento sobre estados emocionales de Salovey et al.: Datos preliminares. Libro de Actas del V Congreso de Evaluación Psicológica 1: 83–84. [Google Scholar]
  24. Fernández, Claudia, and Maria Maxima Guglialmelli. 2021. Los trabajadores de la Economía Social y Solidaria en la pandemia: Un análisis del impacto, las estrategias y las políticas públicas en Argentina. Paper presented at the V Congreso Regional de Estudios del Trabajo NEA. XV Congreso Nacional de la Asociación Argentina de Especialistas en Estudios del Trabajo (ASET), virtual, September 7–8; Available online: https://digital.cic.gba.gob.ar/items/00a70259-ed80-4b9e-89b4-6b4f6a8b4aff (accessed on 1 January 2022).
  25. Galica, Jacqueline, Ziwei Liu, Danielle Kain, Shaila Merchant, Christopher Booth, Rachel Koven, Michael Brundage, and Kristen R. Haase. 2021. Coping during COVID-19: A mixed methods study of older cancer survivors. Supportive Care in Cancer 29: 3389–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. George, Darren, and Paul Mallery. 2010. SPSS for Windows Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference 17.0 Update. Upper Saddle River and Boston: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gil, Frederic, Georgina del Valle, and Ursula Oberst. 2015. Nuevas tecnologías ¿Nuevas patologías? El smartphone y el fear of missing out. Aloma 33: 73–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Girdhar, Ritika, Vivek Srivastava, and Sujata Sethi. 2020. Managing mental health issues among elderly during COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Geriatric Care and Research 7: 32–52. [Google Scholar]
  29. Goleman, Daniel. 1995. Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books. [Google Scholar]
  30. González Yubero, Sara, Susana Lázaro Visa, and Raquel Palomera Martín. 2021. ¿Qué aporta la inteligencia emocional al estudio de los factores personales protectores del consumo de alcohol en la adolescencia? Psicologia Educativa 27: 27–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gul, Hesna, Sumeyra Firat, Mehmet Sertcelik, Ahmet Gul, Yusuf Gurel, and Birim G. Kilic. 2022. Effects of psychiatric symptoms, age, and gender on fear of missing out (FoMO) and problematic smartphone use: A path analysis with clinical-based adolescent sample. Indian Journal of Psychiatry 64: 289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Haase, Kristen R., Theodore Cosco, Lucy Kervin, Indira Riadi, and Megan E. O’Connell. 2021. Older adults’ experiences with using technology for socialization during the COVID-19 pandemic: Cross-sectional survey study. JMIR Aging 4: 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hall, Kevin. 2021. Biocommunicability. The Biopolitics of Pandemic Communication. In Pandemic Communication and Resilience. Risk, Systems and Decisions. Edited by David M. Berube. Cham: Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hamissi, Jalaleddin, Mohadese Babaie, Mehdi Hosseini, and Fatemeh Babaie. 2013. The relationship between emotional intelligence and technology addiction among university students. International Journal of Collaborative Research on Internal Medicine & Public Health 5: 310–19. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hayran, Ceren, and Lalin Anik. 2021. Well-being and fear of missing out (FOMO) on digital content in the time of COVID-19: A correlational analysis among university students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18: 1974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hogan, Thomas P. 2015. Pruebas Psicológicas: Una Introducción Práctica. México: Editorial El Manual Moderno. [Google Scholar]
  37. Holte, Alex J., and F. Richard Ferraro. 2020. Anxious, bored, and (maybe) missing out: Evaluation of anxiety attachment, boredom proneness, and fear of missing out (FoMO). Computers in Human Behavior 112: 106465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. INDEC. 2018. Estudio Nacional Sobre el Perfil de las Personas con Discapacidad: Resultados Provisorios 2018, 1st ed.; Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires: Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Censos—INDEC.
  39. Iqbal, Fahad, Kyle Lam, Viknesh Sounderajah, Jonathan M. Clarke, Hutan Ashrafian, and Ara Darzi. 2021. Characteristics and predictors of acute and chronic post-COVID syndrome: A systematic review and meta-analysis. EClinicalMedicine 36: 100899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kartol, Aslı, and Selim Gündoğan. 2020. The mediating role of emotional intelligence in the relationship between the Fear of Missing Out in social enviroments (FoMO) and depression. European Journal of Education Studies 6: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Li, Li, Zhimin Niu, Songli Mei, and Mark D. Griffiths. 2022. A network analysis approach to the relationship between fear of missing out (FoMO), smartphone addiction, and social networking site use among a sample of Chinese university students. Computers in Human Behavior 128: 107086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Liu, Chang, and Jiang-Liung Ma. 2019. Adult attachment orientations and social networking site addiction: The mediating effects of online social support and the fear of missing out. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 2629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Liu, Jiangmeng, Cong Li, Nick Carcioppolo, and Michael North. 2016. Do Our Facebook Friends Make Us Feel Worse? A Study of Social Comparison and Emotion. Human Communicattion Reearch 42: 619–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Main, Mary, and Judith Solomon. 1986. Discovery of an insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. In Affective Development in Infancy. Edited by T. Berry Brazelton and Michael W. Yogman. Norwood: Ablex Publishing, pp. 95–124. [Google Scholar]
  45. Mayorga-Lascano, Marlon. 2019. Relación entre la creatividad, la inteligencia emocional y el rendimiento académico de estudiantes universitarios. Veritas & Research 1: 13–21. [Google Scholar]
  46. Merchant, Raina, and Nicole Lurie. 2020. Social media and emergency preparedness in response to novel coronavirus. JAMA 323: 2011–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Mukhtar, Sonia. 2020. Psychological health during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic outbreak. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 66: 512–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Nitzburg, George, and Barry Farber. 2013. Putting up emotional (Facebook) walls? Attachment status and emerging adults’ experiences of social networking sites. Journal of Clinical Psychology 69: 1183–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Oddone, Julietta, and Paula Pochintesta. 2021. Las personas mayores durante la pandemia COVID-19: Políticas públicas y acceso a las tecnologías de la información y comunicación en Argentina. Anthropologica 39: 289–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. OMS/Organización Mundial de la Salud. 2021. Global Report on Ageism. WHO. Available online: https://www.who.int/es/teams/social-determinants-of-health/demographic-change-and-healthy-ageing/combatting-ageism/global-report-on-ageism (accessed on 15 December 2022).
  51. Orozco Solis, Mercedes Gabriela. 2021. Inteligencia emocional, empatía y buen trato como factores protectores frente a la agresión física en adolescentes. CES Psicología 14: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Petrides, Konstantinos, Stella Mavroveli Mikolajczak, María José Sanchez-Ruiz, Adrian Furnham, and Juan Carlos Pérez-González. 2016. Developments in trait emotional intelligence research. Emotion Review 8: 335–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Przybylski, Andrew, Kow Murayama, Cody DeHaan, and Valerie Gladwell. 2013. Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior 29: 1841–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Qutishat, Mohammed Ghalib. 2020. Academic adjustment, emotional intelligence, and fear of missing out among undergraduate students: A descriptive correlational study. Oman Medical Journal 35: 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Román-García, Sara, Ana Almansa-Martínez, and María del Rocio Cruz-Díaz. 2016. Adults and Elders and their use of ICTs: Media Competence of Digital Immigrants. Comunicar 49: 101–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Ruvalcaba, Norma, Julia Gallegos, Maryurena Lorenzo, and África Borges. 2014. Propiedades psicométricas del inventario de competencias socioemocionales para adolescentes (EQi-YV) en población mexicana. Revista Evaluar 14: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Salguero, José, Pablo Fernández-Berrocal, Nekane Balluerka, and Aitor Aritzeta. 2009. Measuring perceived emotional intelligence in the adolescent population: Psychometric properties of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal 38: 1197–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Salovey, Peter, Jhon Mayer, Susan Goldman, Carolyn Turvey, and Tibor Palfai. 1995. Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In Emotion, Disclosure, & Health. Edited by James W. Pennebaker. Washington: American Psychological Association, pp. 125–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sánchez-Carbonell, Xavier, Marta Beranuy, Montserrat Castellana, Ander Chamarro, and Ursula Oberst. 2008. Internet and cell phone addiction: Passing fad or disorder? Adicciones 20: 149–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Scheinfeld, Emily, and Heather Voorhees. 2022. How Social Media, FoMO, and Isolation influence our perceptions of others who “break the rules”. Social Media+ Society 8. [Google Scholar]
  61. Schutte, Nicola, Jhon Malouff, Einar Thorsteinsson, Navjot Bhullar, and Sally Rooke. 2007. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual Differences 42: 921–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Shaver, Phillip, and Cindy Hazan. 1987. Being lonely, falling in love. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 2: 105–24. [Google Scholar]
  63. Siegel, Christian, and Thomas Dorner. 2017. Information technologies for active and assisted living—Influences to the quality of life of an ageing society. International Journal of Medical Informatics 100: 32–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Soriano-Sánchez, José Gabriel. 2022. Factores psicológicos y consecuencias del síndrome fear of missing out: Una revisión sistemática. Revista de Psicología y Educación 17: 69–78. [Google Scholar]
  65. Sporzon, Giada, and María Del Carmen López-López. 2021. Evaluación de la inteligencia emocional y la conducta prosocial y su correlación en alumnado de Educación Primaria. Estudios Sobre Educación 40: 51–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Torres Mendoza, Isis, and Cristina Serna Sarrato. 2021. El desarrollo de los vínculos en la sociedad actual: Dependencia, confianza o control. In En I. Martínez Sánchez: Padres y Educación en la Era Digital. Cuenca: Ediciones de la Universidad Castilla-la Mancha, pp. 91–98. Available online: https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=7856237 (accessed on 17 January 2023). [CrossRef]
  67. Tugtekin, Ufuk, Esra Barut Tugtekin, Adile Kurt, and Kadir Demir. 2020. Associations between fear of missing out, problematic smartphone use, and social networking services fatigue among young adults. Social Media+ Society 6: 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Valizadeh Hänggi, Susan Jürgen Mérillat, and Lutz Jäncke. 2017. Age prediction on the basis of brain anatomical measures. Human Brain Mapping 38: 997–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Van Deursen, Alexander, Colin Bolle, Sabrina Hegner, and Piet Kommers. 2015. Modeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior: The role of smartphone usage types, emotional intelligence, social stress, self-regulation, age, and gender. Computers in Human Behavior 45: 411–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Vivaldi, Flavia, and Enrique Barra. 2012. Bienestar psicológico, apoyo social percibido y percepción de salud en adultos mayores. Terapia Psicológica 30: 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Zhang, Yali, Sen Li, and Guoliang Yu. 2021. The relationship between social media use and fear of missing out: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica Sinica 53: 273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Distribution of Cases per Region of Argentina.
Table 1. Distribution of Cases per Region of Argentina.
Geographical Regionf%SexAge
MaleFemale
City of Buenos Aires11917.340.3%59.7%60–80 (M = 70.34; SD = 6.35)
Buenos Aires province33748.946%54%60–80 (M = 68.73; SD = 5.22)
Region 1 (Cordoba, Santa Fe, Mendoza, San Luis, San Juan)1101646.4%53.6%60–80 (M = 69.59; SD = 5.29)
Region 2 (Salta, Jujuy, Catamarca, La Rioja, Santiago del Estero, Tucuman)273.951.9%48.1%60–77 (M = 68.11; SD = 4.63)
Region 3 (Misiones, Chaco, Formosa, Entre Rios, Corrientes) 456.544.4%55.6%60–79 (M = 67.40; SD = 5.39)
Patagonian Region (La Pampa, Rio Negro, Neuquen, Chubut, Santa Cruz, Tierra del Fuego)527.451%49%60–80 (M = 68.65; SD = 5.21)
TOTAL69010045.5%54.5%60–80 (M = 69.01; SD = 5.48)
Table 2. Relationships between FoMO dimensions, age, educational level and social class.
Table 2. Relationships between FoMO dimensions, age, educational level and social class.
12345
1. FoMO-NI.802.312 **.001−.091 *−.063
2. FoMO-SO .769−.039.083 *.021
3. Age -−.018.148 **
4. Educational level -.258 **
5. Social class -
Note: **. p < .001; *. p < .01.
Table 3. Relationships between FoMO, emotional intelligence, attachment and psychological symptoms.
Table 3. Relationships between FoMO, emotional intelligence, attachment and psychological symptoms.
MSDαω123456789101112
1. FoMO-NI2.360.41.76.78-.312 **.354 **−.094 *−.126 *−.016.006.364 **.419 **.441 **.401 **.515 **
2. FoMO-SO2.410.53.78.79 -.314 **.084.020.196 **−.041.142 **.101 *.056.115 *.132 **
3. EI-Attent.2.770.72.81.83 -.187 **−.024.063.054.289 **.286 **.400 **.436 **.361 **
4. EI-Clarity2.650.69.77.77 -.345 **.101 *.102 *−.088−.187 *−.108 *−.030−.170 *
5. EI-Repair2.910.55.79.80 -.091 *.124 **−.127 *−.162 *−.434 *−.242 *−.219 *
6. AT. Secure2.440.61-- -.140 **−.030−.055.000−.068−.052
7. AT-Avoid2.470.54-- -−.029.115 **.061−.018−.045
8. AT-Anx.2.700.79-- -.562 **.379 **.326 **.390 **
9. AT-Amb.2.810.90-- -.413 **.355 **.473 **
10. Deppres.2.540.53.78.80 -.607 **.536 **
11. Anxiety2.470.73.81.82 -.577 **
12. Interp. Rel.2.630.66.84.85 -
Note. α: Cronbach’s Alpha; ω: McDonald’s omega. ** p < .001; * p < .01.
Table 4. Regression model results for the FoMO dimensions.
Table 4. Regression model results for the FoMO dimensions.
StepPredictorβR2ΔR2
FoMO-NI1EI-Attent.0.342 ***0.1330.133
EI-Clarity−0.120
EI-Repair−0.077
2EI-Attent.0.209 ***0.2390.106 ***
EI-Clarity −0.036
EI-Repair−0.065
AT- Secure0.012
AT-Avoid.−0.021
AT-Anx.0.153 **
AT-Amb.0.250 ***
3EI-Attent..093 *0.3300.91 ***
EI-Clarity −.003
EI-Repair .024
AT- Secure .013
AT-Avoid.−.005
AT-Anx..110 *
AT-Amb..125 *
Deppres..139 *
Anxiety .019
Interp. Rel..291 *
FoMO-SO1EI-Attent.0.302 ***0.0950.095 ***
EI-Clarity 0.023
EI-Repair0.037
2EI-Attent..269 *0.1350.039 **
EI-Clarity .028
EI-Repair .038
AT-Secure.188 *
AT-Avoid.−.086
AT-Anx. .066
AT-Amb. .012
3EI-Attent..283 *0.1450.010
EI-Clarity .039
EI-Repair−.006
AT- Secure.196 *
AT-Avoid.−.075
AT-Anx. .076
AT-Amb. .018
Deppres.−.128
Anxiety−.011
Interp. Rel..075 *
Note: * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Durao, M.; Etchezahar, E.; Albalá Genol, M.Á.; Muller, M. Fear of Missing Out, Emotional Intelligence and Attachment in Older Adults in Argentina. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 22. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jintelligence11020022

AMA Style

Durao M, Etchezahar E, Albalá Genol MÁ, Muller M. Fear of Missing Out, Emotional Intelligence and Attachment in Older Adults in Argentina. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(2):22. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jintelligence11020022

Chicago/Turabian Style

Durao, Marian, Edgardo Etchezahar, Miguel Ángel Albalá Genol, and Mariela Muller. 2023. "Fear of Missing Out, Emotional Intelligence and Attachment in Older Adults in Argentina" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 2: 22. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jintelligence11020022

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop