Next Article in Journal
Expectations of Personal Life Development and Decision-Making in People with Moderate Intellectual Disabilities
Next Article in Special Issue
Impacts of Cognitive Factors on Creativity Quality in Design: Identification from Performances in Recall, Association and Combination
Previous Article in Journal
Fear of Missing Out, Emotional Intelligence and Attachment in Older Adults in Argentina
Previous Article in Special Issue
Emotional Intelligence and Creative Self-Efficacy among Gifted Children: Mediating Effect of Self-Esteem and Moderating Effect of Gender
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Influence of Analytic Processing on Divergent and Convergent Thinking Tasks: The Role of Rational and Experiential Thinking Styles

1
College Student Mental Health Counseling Center, Xinjiang Medical University, Urumqi 830011, China
2
Zhou Enlai School of Government, Nankai University, Tianjin 300350, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Submission received: 24 October 2022 / Revised: 11 January 2023 / Accepted: 18 January 2023 / Published: 21 January 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Understanding Creativity and Stimulating Creativity)

Abstract

:
Scientific interest in the relationship between analytic processing and creativity has increased in recent years. However, there is conflicting evidence on whether analytic processing reduces or enhances creativity. We hypothesize that differences in creativity measurement paradigms (divergent or convergent thinking tasks) and the research orientation of analytic processing (dispositional or situational) may explain the conflicting findings. The present study aims to investigate how priming analytic processing affects individuals’ performance on divergent and convergent thinking tasks and the moderating role of thinking styles. In Study 1 (N = 155), participants were assigned to either an analytic processing group or a control group and performed convergent thinking (Remote Associates Task) and divergent thinking (Alternative Uses Test) tasks after priming. In Study 2 (N = 119), we conducted a priming paradigm of analytic processing that differed from Study 1, and a personal experiential-rational thinking style was introduced as a moderator. Results showed that priming analytic processing promoted convergent thinking performance but decreased fluency and flexibility scores on the divergent thinking task (Study 1). Notably, the effect of priming analytic processing on divergent thinking performance was significant only for participants with higher levels of rational thinking style (Study 2). These results suggest that thinking styles and dimensions of creativity should be considered in the relationship between analytic processing and creativity.

1. Introduction

Creativity, as an advanced expression of human intelligence, is often defined as the ability to develop novel and practical ideas or products (Sternberg and Lubart 1996). It is an important contributor to political, economic, and cultural development (Sternberg and Kaufman 2010). The study of factors that influence, cultivate, and stimulate creativity has become an enduring research focus in many disciplines, including psychology, education, management, and physiology. The role of cognitive factors has received much attention, and extensive research has been conducted on the predictive role of intelligence, executive functions, and creative self-efficacy on individual creativity (Frith et al. 2021; Batey et al. 2010; Wang et al. 2014).
Analytic processing is a crucial cognitive factor and refers to a form of information processing that is based on consciousness and rules, is driven by reason, consumes more cognitive resources, and is slower (Stanovich and West 2000). It is closely related to individual beliefs, cognition, and behavior (Epley and Gilovich 2006; Wen et al. 2020; Gervais and Norenzayan 2012). A growing number of researchers have studied the relationship between analytic processing and creativity, but their findings vary widely (Pennycook et al. 2015). For example, some studies have shown a significant negative correlation between analytic processing and creativity (Deshayes et al. 2021), suggesting that analytic processing may be detrimental to a person’s ability to generate original ideas (Nijstad et al. 2010). However, other studies have shown that rational thinking can significantly and positively predict individual performance on verbal and figural creativity tasks (Palmiero et al. 2020).
Considering that creativity involves both divergent thinking (generating multiple ideas or solutions) and convergent thinking (searching for the best single solution) (Leung et al. 2012), the conflicting results of previous studies may reflect the differential effects of analytic processing on the two components of creativity. Moreover, there are two important research directions in information processing: dispositional and situational. The former focuses on the temporary activation of analytic or intuitive processing through experimental manipulations, whereas the latter focuses on people’s thinking styles, i.e., individual differences in the extent to which people rely on each type of processing. Previous research on the relationship between creativity and analytic processing has typically focused on only one of these orientations, without considering how thinking styles (information processing tendencies) and situational analytic processing priming jointly influence creative task performance. Therefore, based on dual process theory, the present study sought to examine the effects of analytic processing on two types of creativity task performance (divergent/convergent) through experimental priming and the moderating role of individual’s inherent thinking styles to suggest a possible explanation for previous inconsistent findings and to provide theoretical support for the role of analytic processing in the cultivation of creativity.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Dual Process Theory

Dual process theory states that two independent information-processing systems are involved in human thinking and reasoning. System 1 (also known as intuitive processing or experiential system) is characterized by rapid processing speed, automatic responses, and parallel processing that requires few psychological resources. System 2 (also known as analytic processing or rational system) is characterized by slow, serial, and controlled processing that follows the rules of reasoning and requires more psychological resources (Stanovich and West 2000; Evans and Stanovich 2013; Norris and Epstein 2011). When System 2 (analytic processing) is activated and cognitive resources are available, System 1 (intuitive processing) input is often overridden (Evans and Stanovich 2013; Kahneman and Frederick 2002). Previous studies have shown that analytic processing can be temporarily boosted by experimental manipulations, and common priming paradigms include visual priming (Gervais and Norenzayan 2012), verbal fluency tasks with scrambled sentences (Gervais and Norenzayan 2012; Swami et al. 2014), writing exercise, etc. (Shenhav et al. 2012).
However, some researchers have also focused on individuals’ relatively stable preferences for these two types of processing (Phillips et al. 2016). Researchers such as Epstein extended the dual process theory to the domain of personality and cognitive style, and suggested that in the problem-solving and decision-making process, there are relatively stable individual differences in people’s reliance on each type of information processing, and people’s general preferences in information processing can be reflected by the concept of thinking style (Epstein et al. 1996). The experiential thinking style reflects dependence on intuitive processing (assigned to System 1 processing), while the rational thinking style reflects dependence on analytic processing (mapping onto System 2 processing) (Epstein et al. 1996; Sowden et al. 2015; Leikas et al. 2007). The point of view of Epstein et al. has been confirmed in many areas of research (Pacini and Epstein 1999; Ares et al. 2014). The present study intends to comprehensively examine the effects of analytic processing priming and individual thinking styles on creativity.

2.2. Divergent and Convergent Thinking

Creativity manifests itself in scientific invention, artistic creation, entrepreneurial innovation, and many other areas, and its construct is thought to include two measurable cognitive components: divergent and convergent thinking (Guilford 1967; W. Zhang et al. 2020; Runco and Jaeger 2012). Divergent thinking is an open-ended psychological process that emphasizes unconventionality, searching for variation, and exploring answers from different aspects, whereas convergent thinking emphasizes finding a single solution through analysis and deductive reasoning (Zmigrod et al. 2015). Generally, creativity can be achieved based on a combination of divergent and convergent thinking (Childs et al. 2022). Two prominent measures used in creativity research are the Alternative Uses Task (AUT, Guilford 1967) and the Remote Associates Task (RAT, Mednick 1962), which are most commonly used to assess divergent and convergent thinking, respectively (Cortes et al. 2019). In the AUT, participants are asked to find as many uses as possible for some common objects (e.g., brick, newspaper) so that they can consider and solve problems from different perspectives, and is thus diagnostic of divergent thinking. AUT performance was also found to be a significant predictor of creativity in the product design industry (Kwon et al. 2017). In contrast, RAT asks participants to find a word that can be combined with or is related to each of three seemingly unrelated words. In this task, there is only one correct answer for each item, and it requires fairly tight top-down constraints, making it diagnostic of convergent thinking (Chermahini and Hommel 2010). In addition, Lee et al. (2014) found that the RAT score correlated significantly and positively with scores on several tests of convergent thinking (e.g., fluid intelligence, working memory, academic achievement), indicating favorable external validity and that it correlated only weakly with tests of divergent thinking.
Although both divergent and convergent thinking are important components of creativity, some studies suggest that they are based on different cognitive processes (Hommel et al. 2011). Convergent thinking may rely on strong top-down cognitive control to focus attention on a problem and exclude extraneous information. Divergent thinking, on the other hand, can benefit from a low intensity of control from above to flexibly and quickly shift from one point of view to another (Colzato et al. 2012). Studies have found that several variables, such as meditation (Colzato et al. 2012), personality (Chamorro-Premuzic and Reichenbacher 2008), and risk-taking (Shen et al. 2018) have differential effects on performance in divergent and convergent thinking.

2.3. Analytic Processing, Thinking Styles, and Creativity

There are differing views in the scientific community about the relationship between analytic processing and creativity. Some researchers believe that analytic processing is not conducive to creativity and that a more intuitive, less analytical approach tends to enhance individual creativity performance (Wiley and Jarosz 2012; Aiello et al. 2012) because intuitive processing can generate a large number of new ideas by association, whereas analytic processing based on linear processing cannot (Sowden et al. 2015). Garfield et al. (2001) used groupware-based techniques to manipulate the cognitive process of creativity development and found that, compared to individuals who used analytic techniques, those who used intuitive problem-solving techniques presented more novel and paradigm-breaking ideas. In contrast, some researchers have argued that analytic processing plays a critical role in the creative process (Kaufmann and Vosburg 1997), and high-level creative problem-solving requires a rational and “rigorous information-processing model” (Rothenberg 1990). The Creativity Diamond Framework (Childs et al. 2022) considers analytic thinking as an important tool for fostering creativity. Barr et al. (2015) also confirmed a positive relationship between analytic processing and RAT creative task performance.
Due to the insufficient and contradictory results, it is necessary to further investigate the relationship between creativity and analytic processing. According to previous studies, the relationship between the two types of information processing and task performance may be influenced by the degree of compatibility between the information processing characteristics and the task (Phillips et al. 2016), and the effect of analytic processing priming on creativity may vary depending on the task type. Analytic processing is controlled, sequential, and based on logic and rules, and may be more consistent with convergent than divergent thinking tasks. Studies have shown that performance on the CRT task, which reflects individuals’ analytic processing ability, correlates with performance on single-solution tests (e.g., fluid intelligence, statistical numerical reasoning, working memory, and mechanical-spatial ability) (Sobkow et al. 2022; Otero et al. 2022), and these results also provide supportive evidence for a possible relationship between analytic processing and convergent thinking. Thus, priming analytic processing could lead individuals to invest more cognitive effort and engage in more careful and conscious processing (Swami et al. 2014), resulting in better performance on convergent thinking tasks that require a single answer. However, the focused attention, linear processing, and slow processing speed associated with analytic processing could be detrimental to creative performance on divergent tasks (Baird et al. 2012). Therefore, Hypothesis 1 is proposed in the present study: there are differences in the effects of priming analytic processing on different types of creativity, particularly enhancing divergent thinking performance and decreasing convergent thinking performance.
In addition, thinking style could also be an important moderating variable in the relationship between analytic processing and creativity. Numerous studies have shown that individuals’ traits, tendencies, and styles can influence their psychological and behavioral performance, along with temporary priming manipulations, such as trait power and state power (Schmidt-Barad and Uziel 2020), trait mindfulness and state mindfulness (Egan et al. 2018), trait attachment and attachment priming (Cuyvers et al. 2022), and chronic regulatory focus and situational regulatory focus, etc. (Krishna et al. 2021). Thinking styles reflect the relatively stable tendency of individuals to rely on the two processing modes, and they may interact with analytic processing priming. There is limited research examining the effects of thinking styles and contextual priming on individuals. Only Dane et al. (2011) showed that individuals exhibit higher levels of average and above-average creativity when asked to choose a problem-solving approach that differs from their inherent thinking style. Dane suggested that this may be because invoking the kind of processing that is not normally used helps individuals engage in breakthrough cognitive associations that lead to increased levels of creativity. However, in the Dane et al. study, only one divergent thinking task (idea generation task) was used, and manipulated information processing was divided into intuitive and analytic processing priming groups without specifying a control group, making it difficult to assess the specific effects of analytic processing separately. Therefore, the moderating effect of thinking style on the relationship between analytic processing priming and the two types of creativity needs further investigation.
Individuals’ long-term preference for a particular type of cognitive processing may have increased the accessibility of this processing mode, such that it is often in a preporatory state in information-processing scenarios (Fang et al. 2018). From this perspective, information processing is more likely to be activated quickly and effectively when the priming effect matches the individual’s inherent thinking style, resulting in a higher priming effect. Therefore, thinking style and information processing priming may have an “additive effect” on creativity. Based on the above analysis, this study proposes Hypothesis 2: individuals’ thinking styles (experiential vs. rational) will moderate the predictive effect of analytic processing activation on divergent and convergent thinking performance; specifically, the predictive effect of analytic processing activation on both types of creativity will be stronger for individuals with high rational thinking style and individuals with low experiential thinking style.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of analytic processing on performance on divergent and convergent thinking tasks and their boundary conditions in two experiments. Study 1 examined how analytic processing priming affects individual performance on two types of creative tasks (convergent and divergent) using a visual priming paradigm. In Study 2, we further examined how thinking styles moderate the influence of analytic processing priming on the two types of creativity.

3. Study 1

In this study, we examine how temporal priming of analytic processing influences divergent and convergent thinking performance. The visual priming paradigm was used to prime individual analytic processing to examine causal relationships between variables.

3.1. Participants and Procedure

The expected sample size was calculated using G*Power 3.1. Using d = 0.50 (a medium effect size), α = 0.05, and 1 − β = 0.80, it was calculated that at least 128 subjects were required. A total of 203 college students were recruited online to participate in the experiment, excluding those who did not complete the experiment and had previously participated in a similar experiment. The actual effective number of participants was 155, including 37 males and 118 females with a mean age of 22.11 years, SDage = 2.21.
A single-factor within-subjects design (analytic processing group vs. control group) was adopted, in which subjects were randomly assigned to an analytic processing group (n = 77) or a control group (n = 78), with the analytic processing group priming analytic processing and the control group performing unrelated priming. After the priming manipulation, subjects completed two creative tasks, divergent and convergent, and completed the manipulation check items. The order of the divergent and convergent thinking tasks was counterbalanced between subjects to prevent the order of the processing of the creativity tasks from affecting the results.

3.2. Materials

3.2.1. Analytic Processing Prime

According to Gervais and Norenzayan (2012), a visual priming paradigm was used to activate participants’ analytic processing. The experimental group was asked to look at a picture of Rodin’s The Thinker, which shows a man sitting on a rock and thinking. The control group was asked to view a picture of Myron’s Discobolus, which shows a man throwing a discus. The viewing time was 30 s for both groups. Previous studies have shown that the image of the Thinker can effectively implicitly activate analytic processing (Franks and Scherr 2017).

3.2.2. Divergent Thinking Task

The Alternative Uses Test (Abbreviated as AUT, Guilford 1967) was used to assess individual performance in divergent thinking. Subjects were asked to list as many uses for the “rope” as possible within three minutes. Creativity scores were measured using three indicators: Fluency, Flexibility, and Originality. Fluency was assessed by the total number of ideas generated by a subject, and flexibility by the number of categories included in the ideas. Originality was assessed by the degree of uniqueness of an idea. The score was 0 if the frequency of an opinion was above 3%, 1 point if it was between 1% and 3%, and 2 points if it was below 1%. Considering that originality scores may be highly correlated with fluency scores (Nusbaum and Silvia 2011), we used a corrective calculation for originality (originalitycorrected = originality/frequency) with reference to Shah and Gustafsson (2021).

3.2.3. Convergent Thinking Task

A validated Chinese version of the Remote Associates Test (RAT) (Xiao et al. 2016) was used to assess convergent thinking performance. Three words were assigned to each question. Subjects were asked to think of a word related to all three words (e.g., freshness, atmosphere, purification, and the answer word was air) within 3 min. The higher the score, the higher the level of convergent thinking. In this study, 14 of the 91 items from RAT were used, and this Chinese version was used among Chinese university students (Han et al. 2018; M. Zhang et al. 2020).

3.2.4. Manipulation Check

To assess the effectiveness of the prime of analytic processing, each subject had to complete a short five-question scale to evaluate the procesing mode used in the creativity task (Dane et al. 2011). A 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) was used with questions such as “I analyzed all available information in detail.” The higher the score, the more analytic processing was used in the task.

3.3. Results and Discussion

3.3.1. Manipulation Check

The results of the independent samples t-test showed that participants reported higher use of analytic processing in the analytic processing group (M = 2.80, SD = 0.44) than in the control group (M = 2.59, SD = 0.49), t(153) = 2.81, p = 0.006, Cohen’s d = 0.45, suggesting a valid experimental manipulation of analytic processing.

3.3.2. Hypothesis Testing

The descriptive statistical results of creativity task scores under different conditions are shown in Table 1. For the AUT performance, the results of the independent samples t-test showed that the analytic processing group scored significantly lower than the control group on fluency (t(153) = −2.51, p = 0.013, Cohen’s d = −0.41) and flexibility (t(153) = −2.46, p = 0.015, Cohen’s d = −0.40), but not on originality (t(153) = −0.74, p = 0.463, Cohen’s d = −0.12).
As for the RAT performance, results showed that the analytic processing group scored significantly higher than the control group on RAT scores (t(153) = 3.33, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.54).
To further exclude the possible effects of age and sex on the results, an ANCOVA was conducted with age and sex as covariates. The results showed a significant main effect of priming analytic processing on RAT scores (F(1, 151) = 10.94, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.07), fluency (F(1, 151) = 6.09, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.04), flexibility (F(1, 151) = 6.12, p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.04), but not on originality (F(1, 151) = 0.55, p = 0.459, ηp2 = 0.004).
Meanwhile, no significant effect was found for gender and age on the four variables (AUT-fluency, AUT-flexibility, AUT-originality, and RAT), Fs < 1.00, ps > 0.05.
The results were consistent with Hypothesis 1 that there were differences in the effects of analytic processing on performance on different types of creative tasks. Specifically, priming analytic processing significantly increased the RAT performance and decreased the fluency and flexibility scores of AUT compared to the control group.

4. Study 2

Based on the results of Study 1, Study 2 introduced the measurement of experiential and rational thinking styles to examine how the analytic processing prime and individual inherent thinking styles affect individual performance on the two types of creative tasks. In addition, to improve the robustness of the results of Study 1, the implicit verbal fluency task-priming paradigm was used to activate analytic processing.

4.1. Participants and Procedure

The sample size required for the study was calculated using G*Power3.1, and given the need for a moderating effect analysis, F tests (linear multiple regression: fixed model, R2 increase) were chosen, setting the effect size f2 = 0.10, α = 0.05, and 1 − β = 0.80, to calculate the minimum 100 subjects required. A total of 134 college students were recruited online to participate in the experiment, excluding those who did not complete the experiment seriously and those who had completed similar questions. The final valid sample size was 119. Of those, 58 subjects were included in the control group and 61 in the analytic processing group. There were 20 males and 99 females, with a mean age of 21.82 (SD = 2.06).
Identical to Study 1, a one-factor between-subjects experimental design was used, with analytic processing priming in the experimental group and irrelevant priming in the control group. First, subjects were required to fill out a questionnaire measuring experiential-rational thinking style, after which they were randomly assigned to either the experimental or control group, and after priming was completed, subjects completed two creativity tasks of divergent and convergent as well as manipulation check items.

4.2. Materials

4.2.1. Analytic Processing Prime

Following Gervais and Norenzayan’s (2012) implicit priming paradigm, word selection and sentence formation tasks were used to activate analytic processing. The task consisted of ten groups of five words each. Subjects had to exclude one word and rearrange the remaining four words to form a complete sentence (e.g., five words: love, my, run, I, parents; target sentence: I love my parents). In each word group, the excluded word was the target word used to manipulate processing. In the analytic processing group, five excluded words were related to analytic reasoning (e.g., “think,” “rational”), whereas in the control group, all 10 excluded words were conventional words unrelated to analytic reasoning. The effectiveness of this priming paradigm for activating analytic processing has been verified in a previous study (Geng et al. 2020), that is, analytic processing can be activated concomitantly through the initiation of trait construct.

4.2.2. Thinking Style Measurement

The Rational-Experiential Inventory for Adolescents (REI-A, Marks et al. 2008) was used to measure individual thinking style, which includes two dimensions of experiential and rational thinking styles, with 10 questions for each dimension (e.g., I try to avoid situations that require thinking in depth about something), through a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (completely inconsistent) to 5 (completely consistent). Cronbach’s α for the experiential and rational thinking style subscales in this study were 0.79 and 0.86, respectively.

4.2.3. Divergent Thinking Task, Convergent Thinking Task, and Manipulation Check

The divergent thinking task, convergent thinking task, and manipulation check items applied in study 2 were identical to Study 1.

4.3. Results and Discussion

4.3.1. Manipulation Check

First, an independent sample t-test was conducted to examine whether there was a significant difference between the analytic processing group and the control group in the manipulation test. The results showed that the analytic processing group (M = 2.85, SD = 0.51) reported a higher use of analytic processing than the control group (M = 2.65, SD = 0.42), t(121) = 2.29, p = 0.024, Cohen’s d = 0.42; thus, the manipulation was effective.

4.3.2. Hypothesis Test

To investigate the combined influence of individual inherent thinking styles and analytic processing priming on convergent and divergent thinking, a multiple regression equation was constructed (see Table 2). First, the priming condition was coded as a virtual variable (0-control group, 1-analytic processing group), and all continuous variables included in the regression equation were standardized. Simultaneously, the standardized score for the rational and experiential thinking styles is multiplied by the primed condition to form an interactive term. The score of the convergent thinking task (RAT) and three indicators of the divergent thinking task (fluency, flexibility, and originality) were taken as dependent variables, the priming conditions and rational and experiential thinking styles were added as predictive variables in Model 1, and two interaction items were added to Model 2.
For convergent thinking performance, Model 1 results showed that the priming condition positively predicted RAT scores after controlling for experiential thinking style and rational thinking style scores (B = 0.43, t = 2.35, p = 0.021); that is, priming analytic processing facilitated individual RAT performance compared to the control group. In Model 2, neither interaction terms were a significant predictor of RAT performance (p > 0.05).
For the fluency score of AUT, Model 1 results showed that the predictive effect of the priming condition on fluency score was significant (B = −0.37, t = −2.00, p = 0.048), and the predictive effects of both experiential and rational thinking styles were not significant (ps > 0.05). In Model 2, the interaction term between analytic processing priming and rational thinking style negatively predicted fluency score (B = −0.42, t = −2.36, p = 0.020), suggesting that rational thinking style significantly and negatively moderated the predictive effect of the priming condition on AUT-fluency score.
For the flexibility score, Model 1 results showed that the predictive effects of the priming condition on the flexibility score reached marginal significance (B = −0.34, t = −1.83, p = 0.070), and the predictive effects of both experiential and rational thinking styles were not significant (ps > 0.05). In Model 2, the interaction term between analytic processing priming and rational thinking style negatively predicted flexibility score (B = −0.53, t = −2.93, p = 0.004), suggesting that rational thinking style significantly negatively moderated the predictive effect of the priming condition on AUT-flexibility score.
For the originality score, Model 1 results showed that the predictive effects of the priming condition on the originality score was not significant (B = −0.28, t = −1.50, p = 0.136). In Model 2, the interaction terms of analytic processing priming with experiential and rational thinking styles were not significant predictors of AUT-originality score (ps > 0.05).
Further, a simple slope analysis was conducted to clarify the moderating role of the rational thinking style. Among individuals with a high rational thinking style (M + SD), analytic processing priming had a significant negative predictive effect on AUT-fluency (B = −0.79, t = −3.11, p = 0.002), whereas, among individuals with a low rational thinking style (MSD), the predictive effect of priming was not significant (B = 0.06, t = 0.24, p = 0.810) (see Figure 1). Similar results were found for flexibility score, that among individuals with a high rational thinking style (M + SD), analytic processing priming had a significant negative predictive effect on AUT-flexibility (B = −0.87, t = −3.38, p = 0.001), whereas among individuals with a low rational thinking style (MSD), the predictive effect of priming was not significant (B = 0.19, t = 0.75, p = 0.456) (see Figure 2).

5. General Discussion

Through two experiments, this study examined how priming analytic processing affects people’s performance on both divergent and convergent thinking tasks and how individuals’ inherent thinking styles moderate this effect.
This study found significant differences in the effects of priming analytic processing on different types of creativity. For convergent thinking, subjects in the analytic processing-primed group performed better on the RAT task than the control group, which is consistent with the findings of Barr et al. (2015). Convergent thinking task requires the generation of relevant answers based on well-defined criteria, relies on top-down cognitive processing (Jiang et al. 2022; Hommel et al. 2011), and requires individuals to invest a high level of cognitive resources to exercise cognitive control and prevent irrelevant ideas from entering the working memory (Radel et al. 2015). This process is closely linked to conscious, mental resource-draining, and logically reasoned analytic processing. Thus, priming analytic processing may help individuals stay focused during convergent thinking tasks, perform careful search and logical analysis, and eliminate distractions to find a single, correct solution.
For divergent thinking, the results showed that the analytic processing priming group performed lower fluency and flexibility scores than the control group, in line with Deshayes et al. (2021). The divergent thinking task process allows people to generate as many responses (ideas, solutions) as possible under relatively weak constraints, and under this premise, lower levels of cognitive control may facilitate individuals to quickly “jump” from one idea to another (Hommel et al. 2011) and help allow more unfiltered information to enter the processing stage, thereby facilitating the generation of more divergent ideas (Radel et al. 2015). Consequently, automatic, associative, and rapid intuitive processing may yield more benefits when performing divergent thinking tasks than slow, highly controlled analytic processing (Norris and Epstein 2011). Based on the dual processing theory perspective, when analytic processing is activated and cognitive resources are abundant, intuitive processing is often overridden (Strack and Deutsch 2004); therefore, activating analytic processing may limit individuals’ intuitive processing in divergent thinking tasks and reduce the number and categories of the generated ideas. In addition, Beaty et al. (2019) suggested that during creative cognition, Default Network (DN, supporting spontaneous thought) might get involved in idea generation and the Executive control network (ECN, supporting cognitive control) in guiding, constraining, and modifying DN processes. Razumnikova and Yashanina (2017) found that rational thinking and irrational thinking were associated with the frequency-regional organization of cortical alpha range biopotentials in convergent and divergent tasks, respectively. These results also provide cognitive-neural evidence to support the differential effects of analytic processing on divergent and convergent thinking task performance found in this study. However, the numerical difference in the originality score between the analytic processing group and the control group did not reach significance, which might be due to the task duration. Within 3 min, some particularly novel ideas may have not been fully explored, resulting in the non-significant difference in originality between the two groups. Alternatively, analytic processing might make participants evaluate the generated ideas based on the task constraints (Palmiero et al. 2020), and eliminate the more “common” answers during the idea generation process, thus preserving originality at the expense of fluency and flexibility.
In the present study, it was found that there was a boundary condition for the impairment of fluency and flexibility of AUT task by analytic processing priming; that is, the effect was only present in individuals with a high propensity for rational thinking, indicating that contextual analytic processing priming and rational thinking style, rather than experiential thinking style, synergistically influence divergent thinking performance, thus, Hypothesis 2 was partially tested. The findings also partially support Dane et al.’s (2011) finding that priming of analytic problem solving is detrimental to the creative performance of individuals with high rational thinking styles. This may be because priming is a concomitant activation of knowledge structures such as characteristic concepts or habits (Bargh et al. 1996), and individuals with high rational thinking have a long-term, stable preference for using analytic processing compared to individuals with low rational thinking style (Norris and Epstein 2011), therefore, their analytic processing systems may be more accessible and more likely to be activated quickly and effectively at the time of priming, and thus, be more susceptible to the effects of analytic processing priming, showing a decrease in divergent creativity. For convergent thinking task scores, thinking styles did not interact with analytic processing priming, possibly because the convergent thinking task itself has higher demands on analytic processing. According to previous research, the effect of information processing priming on task performance may be stronger when task features are higher in matching compatibility with information processing type (Phillips et al. 2016; Rusou et al. 2013); thus, even individuals with low rational thinking may be susceptible to analytic processing priming and show enhanced task performance in convergent thinking tasks. It is worth noting that no moderating effect of experiential thinking style was found in the present study, which is also consistent with Dane et al. (2011), possibly because the experiential thinking style and rational thinking style are independent of each other (Marks et al. 2008). Therefore, the level of an individual’s experiential thinking style does not reflect his or her preference for the use of analytic processing, nor does it interact directly with analytic processing priming.
This study advances theoretical and empirical research in the following ways. First, although dual process theory has accumulated a substantial theoretical and empirical foundation in both situational and dispositional research orientations (Gervais and Norenzayan 2012; Epstein et al. 1996), few studies have combined the priming of analytic processing with thinking styles to examine its effects on individuals. Within the framework of dual process theory, the present study systematically discusses the role of a stable thinking style and states activation of analytic processing on two creativity task performances, to some extent, adds to the study of creativity from the perspective of dual process theory. Second, by adopting AUT and RAT as creativity measures, this study indicates that analytic processing may have different effects on the two important components of creativity, divergent and convergent thinking, providing a probable perspective to explain the differences in the results of previous studies. Additionally, the research findings offer implications for the cultivation of creativity in education, organization, and other fields. Relevant practitioners should pay attention to the differentiated impact of analytic processing on different types of creativity. The results suggest that the priming or intervention of analytic processing could serve as a possible strategy for enhancing creativity, but it may play a greater role in creative tasks or phases that are dominated by convergent rather than divergent thinking. In addition, the potential influence of individual differences should be taken into account in analytic-processing-related-creativity promotion programs, and more effective interventions should be developed in conjunction with individual thinking styles.
This study has several limitations. First, we adopted two widely used creative tasks in the present study, however, there are numerous measures of divergent and convergent thinking, such as insight problems, Evaluation of Creative Potential (EPoC), etc. (Barbot et al. 2016). Therefore, it is necessary to examine the robustness of the results by applying multiple measures of creativity in future research. At the same time, there is a discrepancy between the cognitive creativity task under experimental conditions and the real innovation process, so in the follow-up studies, empirical sampling and other approaches could be considered to investigate the role of intuitive and analytic processing in the real innovation process. Second, the subjects in this study were all college students, which may limit the generalizability of the results. In the future, relationships between the variables in different groups will be discussed. Third, this study did not investigate the mechanism of analytic processing that influences creativity. In future research, neuroscience or other experimental methods could be used to further investigate the underlying mechanism of its effect on creative cognitive process.

6. Conclusions

Overall, through two experiments, the present study examined the effects of analytic processing priming on divergent and convergent thinking task performance and the moderating effects of individual thinking styles from the perspective of dual process theory. The results indicated that priming analytic processing enhanced individuals’ performance on the convergent thinking task (RAT) and reduced individuals’ fluency and flexibility scores on the divergent thinking task (AUT). Furthermore, the negative effects of analytic processing priming on AUT fluency and flexibility were significant only in individuals with a high rational thinking style.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.H., Y.-X.C., X.Z. and H.-K.Z.; Methodology, J.H. and H.-K.Z.; Formal analysis, Y.-X.C.; Investigation, J.H. and Y.-X.C.; Data curation, J.H. and Y.-X.C.; Writing—original draft, J.H.; Writing—review & editing, Y.-X.C.; Supervision, X.Z. and H.-K.Z.; Funding acquisition, X.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Social Science Foundation of China (19FSHB010) and the Teaching Reform Project of Teaching Supervisory Committee of Psychology in Higher Education Schools of Chinese Ministry of Education (20221039).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by Nankai University Institutional Review Board the approval number is NKUIRB2021082.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aiello, Daniel A., Andrew F. Jarosz, Patrick J. Cushen, and Jennifer Wiley. 2012. Firing the executive: When an analytic approach to problem solving helps and hurts. The Journal of Problem Solving 4: 116–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ares, Gaston, Franco Mawad, Ana Gimenez, and Alejandro Maiche. 2014. Influence of rational and intuitive Thinking styles on food choice: Preliminary evidence from an eye-tracking study with yogurt labels. Food Quality and Preference 31: 28–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Baird, Benjamin, Jonathan Smallwood, Michael D. Mrazek, Julia W. Y. Kam, Michael S. Franklin, and Jonathan W. Schooler. 2012. Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science 23: 1117–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Barbot, Baptiste, Maud Besançon, and Todd Lubart. 2016. The generality-specificity of creativity: Exploring the structure of creative potential with EPoC. Learning and Individual Differences 52: 178–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Bargh, J. A., M. Chen, and L. Burrows. 1996. Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 230–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Barr, Nathaniel, Gordon Pennycook, Jennifer A. Stolz, and Jonathan A. Fugelsang. 2015. Reasoned connections: A dual-process perspective on creative thought. Thinking & Reasoning 21: 61–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Batey, Mark, Adrian Furnham, and Xeniya Safiullina. 2010. Intelligence, general knowledge and personality as predictors of creativity. Learning and Individual Differences 20: 532–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Beaty, Roger E., Paul Seli, and Daniel L. Schacter. 2019. Network neuroscience of creative cognition: Mapping cognitive mechanisms and individual differences in the creative brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 27: 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas, and Lisa Reichenbacher. 2008. Effects of personality and threat of evaluation on divergent and convergent thinking. Journal of Research in Personality 42: 1095–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chermahini, Soghra, and Bernhard Hommel. 2010. The (b) link between creativity and dopamine: Spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition 115: 458–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Childs, Peter, Ji Han, Liuqing Chen, Pingfei Jiang, Pan Wang, Dongmyung Park, Yuan Yin, Elena Dieckmann, and Ignacio Vilanova. 2022. The creativity diamond—A framework to aid creativity. Journal of Intelligence 10: 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Colzato, Lorenza S., Ayca Ozturk, and Bernhard Hommel. 2012. Meditate to create: The impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Cortes, Robert A., Adam B. Weinberger, Richard J. Daker, and Adam E. Green. 2019. Re-examining prominent measures of divergent and convergent creativity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 27: 90–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cuyvers, Bien, Martine W. F. T. Verhees, Marinus H. Van Ijzendoorn, Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, Angela C. M. Rowe, Eva Ceulemans, and Guy Bosmans. 2022. The effect of attachment priming on state attachment security in middle childhood: The moderating roles of trait attachment and state attachment volatility. Journal of Early Adolescence 43: 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Dane, Erik, Markus Baer, Michael G. Pratt, and Greg R. Oldham. 2011. Rational versus intuitive problem solving: How thinking “Off the Beaten Path” can stimulate creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 5: 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Deshayes, Claire, Veronique Paban, Marie-Helene Ferrer, Beatrice Alescio-Lautier, and Caroline Chambon. 2021. A comprehensive approach to study the resting-state brain network related to creative potential. Brain Structure & Function 226: 1743–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Egan, Ryan P., Kaylin E. Hill, and Dan Foti. 2018. Differential effects of state and trait mindfulness on the late positive potential. Emotion 18: 1128–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Epley, Nicholas, and Thomas Gilovich. 2006. The anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic: Why the adjustments are insufficient. Psychological Science 17: 311–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Epstein, Seymour, Rosemary Pacini, Veronika Denes-Raj, and Harriet Heier. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 390–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Evans, Jonathan St B. T., and Keith E. Stanovich. 2013. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8: 223–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fang, Ping, Yan Ma, and Lei Wang. 2018. From fixed to flexible: To reconsider the impact of affect on cognition. Journal of Psychological Science 41: 285–91. [Google Scholar]
  22. Franks, Andrew, and Kyle Scherr. 2017. Analytic thinking reduces anti-atheist bias in voting intentions. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 27: 129–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Frith, Emily, Michael J. Kane, Matthew S. Welhaf, Alexander P. Christensen, Paul J. Silvia, and Roger E. Beaty. 2021. Keeping creativity under control: Contributions of attention control and fluid Intelligence to divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal 33: 138–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Garfield, Monica J., Nolan J. Taylor, Alan R. Dennis, and John W. Satzinger. 2001. Research report: Modifying paradigms—Individual differences, creativity techniques, and exposure to ideas in group idea generation. Information Systems Research 12: 322–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Geng, Xiaowei, Dan Liu, and Yanhua Niu. 2020. Analytic thinking reduces impact bias in affective forecast. Acta Psychologica Sinica 52: 1168–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gervais, Will M., and Ara Norenzayan. 2012. Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science 336: 493–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Guilford, Joy P. 1967. Creativity: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. The Journal of Creative Behavior 1: 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Han, Wei, Mi Zhang, Xue Feng, Guihua Gong, Kaiping Peng, and Dan Zhang. 2018. Genetic influences on creativity: An exploration of convergent and divergent thinking. PeerJ 6: e5403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Hommel, Bernhard, Lorenza S. Colzato, Rico Fischer, and Ingrid K. Christoffels. 2011. Bilingualism and creativity: Benefits in convergent thinking come with losses in divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Jiang, Yi, Yan Huang, Yuanzhi Wu, and Lei Wang. 2022. Drinking tea improves the convergent creativity performance. Food Quality and Preference 95: 104360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kahneman, Daniel, and Shane Frederick. 2002. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgement. In Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Edited by Thomas Gilovich, Dale Griffin and Daniel Kahneman. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 49–81. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kaufmann, Geir, and Suzanne K. Vosburg. 1997. ‘Paradoxical’ mood effects on creative problem-solving. Cognition & Emotion 11: 151–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Krishna, Anand, Sophia Ried, and Marie Meixner. 2021. State-trait interactions in regulatory focus determine impulse buying behavior. PLoS ONE 16: e0253634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Kwon, Jieun, Luke Bromback, and Barry Kudrowitz. 2017. Divergent thinking ability+ interest= creative ideas: Exploring the relationships between cognitive creativity assessments and product design idea generation. Presented at the International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference, Cleveland, OH, USA, August 6–9; New York: American Society of Mechanical Engineers. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lee, Christine, Anne Corinne Huggins, and David Therriault. 2014. A measure of creativity or intelligence? Examining internal and external structure validity evidence of the Remote Associates Test. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 8: 446–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Leikas, Sointu, Marjaana Lindeman, Katariina Roininen, and Liisa Lahteenmaki. 2007. Food risk perceptions, gender, and individual differences in avoidance and approach motivation, intuitive and analytic thinking styles, and anxiety. Appetite 48: 232–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Leung, Angela, Suntae Kim, Evan Polman, Lay See Ong, Lin Qiu, Jack A. Goncalo, and Jeffrey Sanchez-Burks. 2012. Embodied metaphors and creative “acts”. Psychological Science 23: 502–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Marks, Anthony D. G., Donald W. Hine, Rebecca L. Blore, and Wendy J. Phillips. 2008. Assessing individual differences in adolescents’ preference for rational and experiential cognition. Personality and Individual Differences 44: 42–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mednick, Sarnoff. 1962. The associative basis of creative problem solving process. Psychological Review. 69: 200–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Nijstad, Bernard A., Carsten K. W. De Dreu, Eric F. Rietzschel, and Matthijs Baas. 2010. The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology 21: 34–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Norris, Paul, and Seymour Epstein. 2011. An experiential thinking style: Its facets and relations with objective and subjective criterion measures. Journal of Personality 79: 1043–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Nusbaum, Emily C., and Paul J. Silvia. 2011. Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence 39: 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Otero, Inmaculada, Jesús F. Salgado, and Silvia Moscoso. 2022. Cognitive reflection, cognitive intelligence, and cognitive abilities: A meta-analysis. Intelligence 90: 101614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Pacini, Rosemary, and Seymour Epstein. 1999. The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 972–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Palmiero, Massimiliano, Raffaella Nori, Laura Piccardi, and Simonetta D’Amico. 2020. Divergent thinking: The role of decision-making styles. Creativity Research Journal 32: 323–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Pennycook, Gordon, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, and Derek J. Koehler. 2015. Everyday consequences of analytic thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science 24: 425–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Phillips, Wendy J., Jennifer M. Fletcher, Anthony D. G. Marks, and Donald W. Hine. 2016. Thinking styles and decision making: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 142: 260–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Radel, Rerni, Karen Davranche, Marion Fournier, and Arne Dietrich. 2015. The role of (dis)inhibition in creativity: Decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition 134: 110–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Razumnikova, Olga M., and Anna A. Yashanina. 2017. Roles of rational-irrational cognitive styles in the alpha rhythm reactivity during convergent and divergent thinking. Rossiiskii fiziologicheskii zhurnal imeni I.M. Sechenova 103: 349–58. [Google Scholar]
  50. Rothenberg, Albert. 1990. Creativity and Madness: New Findings and Old Stereotypes. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press. [Google Scholar]
  51. Runco, Mark A., and Garrett J. Jaeger. 2012. The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal 24: 92–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Rusou, Zohar, Dan Zakay, and Marius Usher. 2013. Pitting intuitive and analytical thinking against each other: The case of transitivity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 608–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Schmidt-Barad, Tomer, and Liad Uziel. 2020. When (state and trait) powers collide: Effects of power-incongruence and self-control on prosocial behavior. Personality and Individual Differences 162: 110009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Shah, Bhagyashree, and Erik Gustafsson. 2021. Exploring the effects of age, gender, and school setting on children’s creative thinking skills. The Journal of Creative Behavior 55: 546–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Shen, Wangbing, Bernhard Hommel, Yuan Yuan, Liu Chang, and Wei Zhang. 2018. Risk-taking and creativity: Convergent, but not divergent thinking is better in low-risk takers. Creativity Research Journal 30: 224–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Shenhav, Amitai, David G. Rand, and Joshua D. Greene. 2012. Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in god. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General 141: 423–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Sobkow, Agata, Angelika Olszewska, and Miroslav Sirota. 2022. The factor structure of cognitive reflection, numeracy, and fluid intelligence: The evidence from the Polish adaptation of the Verbal CRT. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. preprint. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Sowden, Paul T., Andrew Pringle, and Liane Gabora. 2015. The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning 21: 40–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23: 645–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Sternberg, Robert J., and James C. Kaufman. 2010. Constraints on creativity: Obvious and not so obvious. In The Cambridge handbook of Creativity. Edited by James C. Kaufman and Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 467–482. [Google Scholar]
  61. Sternberg, Robert J., and Todd I. Lubart. 1996. Investing in creativity. American Psychologist 51: 677–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Strack, Fritz, and Roland Deutsch. 2004. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology 8: 220–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Swami, Viren, Martin Voracek, Stefan Stieger, Ulrich S. Tran, and Adrian Furnham. 2014. Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition 133: 572–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Wang, Chung-Jen, Huei-Ting Tsai, and Ming-Tien Tsai. 2014. Linking transformational leadership and employee creativity in the hospitality industry: The influences of creative role identity, creative self-efficacy, and job complexity. Tourism Management 40: 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wen, Wen, Jian Li, George Kyriacou Georgiou, Cheng Huang, and Lu Wang. 2020. Reducing the halo effect by stimulating analytic thinking. Social Psychology 51: 334–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wiley, Jennifer, and Andrew F. Jarosz. 2012. Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21: 258–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Xiao, Wei, Xiang Yao, and Yongtao Qiu. 2016. Constructing Chinese Remote Associates Test (RAT) with application of item response theory. Acta Scientiarum Naturalium Universitatis Pekinensis 52: 354–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Zhang, Mi, Fei Wang, and Dan Zhang. 2020. Individual differences in trait creativity moderate the state-level mood-creativity relationship. PLoS ONE 15: e0236987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Zhang, Weitao, Zsuzsika Sjoerds, and Bernhard Hommel. 2020. Metacontrol of human creativity: The neurocognitive mechanisms of convergent and divergent thinking. NeuroImage 210: 116572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Zmigrod, Sharon, Lorenza S. Colzato, and Bernhard Hommel. 2015. Stimulating creativity: Modulation of convergent and divergent thinking by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Creativity Research Journal 27: 353–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The interaction between rational thinking style and analytic processing prime on fluency.
Figure 1. The interaction between rational thinking style and analytic processing prime on fluency.
Jintelligence 11 00023 g001
Figure 2. The interaction between rational thinking style and analytic processing prime on flexibility.
Figure 2. The interaction between rational thinking style and analytic processing prime on flexibility.
Jintelligence 11 00023 g002
Table 1. Creativity task scores under different conditions (M ± SD).
Table 1. Creativity task scores under different conditions (M ± SD).
AUTRAT
FluencyFlexibilityOriginality
control group (n = 78)8.10 ± 1.885.32 ± 1.540.18 ± 0.226.47 ± 2.41
analytic processing group (n = 77)7.29 ± 2.174.74 ± 1.390.15 ± 0.177.82 ± 2.61
Table 2. Moderating effects of experiential and rational thinking styles.
Table 2. Moderating effects of experiential and rational thinking styles.
PredictorRATAUT-FluencyAUT-FlexibilityAUT-Originality
Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2
Condition0.43 *0.43 *−0.37 *−0.36 *−0.34 +−0.34 +−0.28−0.28
Experiential thinking style−0.12−0.050.120.150.010.03−0.10−0.05
Rational thinking style0.03−0.01−0.100.10−0.020.24+0.100.25+
Condition * experiential thinking style −0.15 −0.07 −0.03 −0.02
Condition * rational thinking style 0.10 −0.42 * −0.53 ** −0.31 +
R20.070.080.070.110.030.100.030.06
F2.83 *1.872.72 *2.85 *1.222.52 *1.251.41
ΔR2 0.01 0.05 + 0.07 * 0.03
Note: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, + p < 0.1, Non-standardized coefficient B was reported in this table. The primed condition was a dummy-coded predictor, where 0 = control group and 1 = analytic processing group.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hongdizi, J.; Cui, Y.-X.; Zhou, X.; Zhai, H.-K. Influence of Analytic Processing on Divergent and Convergent Thinking Tasks: The Role of Rational and Experiential Thinking Styles. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 23. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jintelligence11020023

AMA Style

Hongdizi J, Cui Y-X, Zhou X, Zhai H-K. Influence of Analytic Processing on Divergent and Convergent Thinking Tasks: The Role of Rational and Experiential Thinking Styles. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(2):23. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jintelligence11020023

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hongdizi, Jianati, Yu-Xin Cui, Xiang Zhou, and Hong-Kun Zhai. 2023. "Influence of Analytic Processing on Divergent and Convergent Thinking Tasks: The Role of Rational and Experiential Thinking Styles" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 2: 23. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jintelligence11020023

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop