Next Article in Journal
Using Multivariate Outliers from Smartphone Sensor Data to Detect Physical Barriers While Walking in Urban Areas
Next Article in Special Issue
Performing Realistic Workout Activity Recognition on Consumer Smartphones
Previous Article in Journal
Nonlinear Dynamic Behavior of Porous and Imperfect Bernoulli-Euler Functionally Graded Nanobeams Resting on Winkler Elastic Foundation
Previous Article in Special Issue
ExerTrack—Towards Smart Surfaces to Track Exercises
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Connections between Older Greek Adults’ Implicit Attributes and Their Perceptions of Online Technologies

by
Diogenis Alexandrakis
1,*,
Konstantinos Chorianopoulos
1 and
Nikolaos Tselios
2
1
Department of Informatics, Ionian University, 49100 Corfu, Greece
2
Department of Educational Sciences and Early Childhood Education, University of Patras, 26504 Patras, Greece
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 27 September 2020 / Revised: 16 October 2020 / Accepted: 21 October 2020 / Published: 25 October 2020
(This article belongs to the Collection Selected Papers from the PETRA Conference Series)

Abstract

:
Older Greek adults make use of web technologies much less than the majority of their peers in Europe. Based on the fact that psychosocial attributes can also affect technology usage, this exploratory quantitative research is an attempt to focus on the implicit factors related to older Greek adults’ perceived usability, learnability, and ease-of-use of web technologies. For this aim, a web 2.0 storytelling prototype has been demonstrated to 112 participants and an online questionnaire was applied for data collection. According to the results, distinct correlations emerged between older adults’ characteristics (chronological age, loneliness, future time perspective) and the perceived usability, learnability, and ease-of-use of the presented prototype. These outcomes contribute to the limited literature in the field by probing the connections between older people’s implicit attributes and their evaluative perceptions of online technologies.

1. Introduction

Human actions are mediated by tools, which can be external (e.g., a hammer or a computer) or internal (e.g., concepts and plans) [1]. Likewise, our experiences are neither independent of the tools we use nor of our perceptions. According to Heidegger [2], for example, technology is connected with the revelation of reality and truth. Nevertheless, the mediating nature of technology also bears drawbacks. Ihde [3] declared that technology usage is not neutral and depicted that there is also a concealed transformation of users’ experience and perceived reality for each enhancement the mediating technologies provide. Several internal factors can affect the experience, hence influence the way people value, accept, or reject technologies. As it becomes apparent from Plato’s cave allegory [4] and Indirect Realism [5], there is a continuous discussion going on about how our conceptions of the world are mediated by perception.
Human–Computer Interaction (HCI) is constantly evolving. Starting from the First Wave, based on the science of cognition and human factors, through the Second Wave and its focus on groups of users, it further advanced during the Third Wave to include issues of culture, emotions, experiences, and meaning-making [6,7]. During that course, philosophical foundations have been embedded in technology orientation and design, embracing users’ needs for personal development. Existentialism, for instance, based on the axiom that every human is responsible for himself/herself, thus having almost unlimited potentials for self-actualization, has been proposed as a visionary framework for understanding individuals and their relationship with technology [8].
Self-actualization and personal development are related to one’s recollections. Older adults’ personal memories help them build a sense of identity and continuity to the self [9]. Due to literature, identity construction and maintenance are further promoted by sharing memories with others, storytellers’ narratives can vary across different cultures, and people constantly revise their life stories, which makes identity formation a life-long procedure [10]. Notably, healthy individuals sometimes recollect memories of events that never happened. This malfunction of our memory system perhaps addresses our needs related to the self as well as to our social interactions [11]. From a technological perspective, it has been proposed that memory recording interventions should promote users’ creativity and active engagement instead of passively capturing segments of their activities for remembering [12]. Going a step further, digital platforms related to reminiscence and storytelling are apparently beneficial for older adults’ well-being (e.g., [13,14]).
During the last decades, the older adults’ population has been increasing worldwide. It has been estimated that in 2050, the number of people aged 65 years and older will have doubled compared to the current demographics. At that time, according to the World Health Organization [15], Japan, the Republic of Korea, Spain, and Greece will be the countries with the greatest proportions of older inhabitants within their adult populations. However, digital technology usage by older people still remains low compared to younger generations and, also, Internet use by older Greek adults is one of the lowest within the European Union [16]. This digital inequality within society excludes technology non-users from essential resources considering information, communication, and personal growth.
Technology is not culturally neutral [17,18] and older adults’ choice of adopting or rejecting a new technology is not always due to usability issues [19,20]. During the last years, studies about older adults and memories (e.g., [21,22]), loneliness (e.g., [23]), and Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (e.g., [24]) have been common in the HCI field. Hence, in this exploratory paper, which is an extension of our previous article [25], we try to gain further insights into the implicit factors that might correlate with older Greek adults’ conceptions upon learnability, usability, and ease-of-use of online technologies by focusing on a web 2.0 platform for reminiscing and storytelling. The research questions that guide this work are as follows:
  • RQ1. Which implicit factors correlate with older Greek adults’ perceived ease-of-use of web 2.0 storytelling technologies?
  • RQ2. Which implicit factors correlate with older Greek adults’ perceived learnability of web 2.0 storytelling technologies?
  • RQ3. Which implicit factors correlate with older Greek adults’ perceived usability of web 2.0 storytelling technologies?
The findings of this project contribute to the field of HCI by probing the relationships between older Greek adults’ psychosocial attributes and their perceptions that determine technology adoption. To the best of our knowledge, there is no similar research in literature.
The rest of the paper is organized as follows: Section 2 provides an overview of the related literature and the research hypotheses. Section 3 describes the methodology and the procedure that was followed. The results are presented in Section 4 and are discussed in Section 5. Limitations and future work are proposed in Section 6, followed by the conclusion section (Section 7).

2. Related Literature and Research Hypotheses

2.1. Implicit Factors Affecting People’s Behavior

Socioemotional Selectivity Theory is a theoretical framework that provides valuable insights into human motives and preferences [26]. Due to this theory, a person’s future time perspective (FTP), denoting the perceived time that has been left in life, has an effect on his/her choices. Individuals who feel that their future is open-ended tend to prioritize knowledge-based activities, whereas when they regard it as limited, they tend to prefer emotionally meaningful interactions. Under this scope, learning how to use an Internet tool can be identified as a knowledge-based behavior. Further in the literature, considering the connection between the subjective estimation of time and technology use, older adults’ evaluative perceptions of their age are related to their positive attitudes towards information and communication technologies [27] and technostress [28], which in turn affects user’s satisfaction. It is worth noting that satisfaction is a broad term that contains, among others, the ease-of-use dimension [29].
Feelings and emotions are an integral part of technology use [17]. Chen and Chan [30] indicated that psychosocial attributes, such as fear of death or loneliness, may have an intrinsic effect on older adults’ motives for technology usage. Loneliness is an issue that often troubles older populations. It has been generally described as an unpleasant state, which is caused by people’s asymmetry between their desirable social relationships and the relationships they believe that they have. There are cultural differences in older adults’ perceived loneliness. Southern Europeans, for example, tend to feel lonelier than the elderly in the northern European countries [31]. Although there are contradictory findings in the literature regarding the effect of loneliness on motivation, it has been argued that loneliness affects the person’s will to complete tasks [32] or learn new skills [33]. Furthermore, according to O’Luanaigh et al. [34], loneliness is associated with older people’s cognition deficits, considering visual memory and psychomotor processing. As it seems, emotions act as cognitive drivers under the context of initial technology use [35] and perceived loneliness, among other psychosocial and emotional variables, is related to different web 2.0 usage patterns [36]. Still, the mechanism through which loneliness affects cognitive abilities remains rather unknown [37].
Chronological age is also related to cognition deficits. Working memory effectiveness and processing speed, among others, are negatively affected by higher age [38]. In addition, there are visual, hearing, and haptic impairments related to older peoples’ physical states and manipulating digital technologies usually depends on users’ aforementioned abilities [39]. There are different usage patterns of digital applications [40] and other online tools [36,41] related to users’ age. It is noteworthy that age is not only a construction defined on the basis of time, but also a social construct that reflects the norms, the expectations, and the roles of each individual in the community that he/she lives in [42].

2.2. Users’ Evaluative Perceptions towards Technology

Perceived ease-of-use is a critical predictor of users’ technology acceptance. As a cognitive construct, it refers to the extent to which someone believes that using a particular tool will be free of effort [43]. Perceived ease-of-use is a core determinant in several models of technology acceptance and use, both for the general population (e.g., [44,45,46]) and the population of older adults (e.g., [47,48,49]). Nevertheless, it has been reported that the effectiveness of acceptance models may vary across different cultures [47,50,51]. Likewise, there are contradictory research outcomes regarding the effect of age on ease-of-use. For instance, Porter and Donthu [52] reported a negative relationship between these two structures, based on a USA sample, while Jimoh, Pate, Lin, and Schulman [53] found that older participants’ scores on perceived ease-of-use were higher than younger participants’ scores, in a study conducted in Africa.
Ease-of-learning may affect the acceptance or rejection of new technologies by older adults, as well [49,54]. In the current project, the term ‘learnability’, which has been used to denote ease-of-learning, is defined as the easiness of learning how to use technology and acquiring information from it [55]. According to the literature, despite cognitive declines, people’s learning abilities can remain efficient as they age. However, older adults need more time to complete training in computer-based systems than younger learners [56,57]. Notably, there is also a relationship between learnability and culture [58,59], as well as between life-long learning abilities and family loneliness [60].
Perceived barriers related to usability often impact technology adoption by older adults [36]. Usability is another concept that involves cognitive processes and derives from a web of qualities, such as complexity, learnability, and ease-of-use [61]. Usability is defined as the degree to which a system can be used in a certain context to achieve specific goals effectively, efficiently, and with satisfaction [62]. As for perceived usability, which is adapted in this study, it is a personal assumption of the usability of a system that can be formed independently of its use. Although actual usability is a fundamental concept in HCI, perceived usability seems to be more influential on users’ motivation to obtain a product [63]. Similarly to what has been observed in other studies, culture affects usability [64] and according to Bangor, Kortum, and Miller [65], there is a variety of factors that could further influence people’s usability ratings, such as the type of the interface being evaluated and the age of the users.

2.3. Research Hypotheses

Based on the information presented above, there appears to be an essential link connecting users’ implicit attributes with their evaluation of technology. Hence, since older Greek adults use the Internet much less, compared to the vast majority of their peers in Europe [16], in the current exploratory study we try to focus on the relationship between two sets of variables: (1) older Greek adults’ chronological age, loneliness, and FTP on the one hand and (2) factors that affect online technology adoption and usage (perceived usability, learnability, and ease-of-use) on the other hand. As a result, in order to answer RQ1, the following hypotheses have been tested:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Older adults’ chronological age has a negative relationship with perceived ease-of-use.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Older adults’ loneliness has a negative relationship with perceived ease-of-use.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Older adults’ future time perspective has a positive relationship with perceived ease-of-use.
Regarding RQ2, the following hypotheses have been proposed:
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
Older adults’ chronological age has a negative relationship with perceived learnability.
Hypothesis 5 (H5).
Older adults’ loneliness has a negative relationship with perceived learnability.
Hypothesis 6 (H6).
Older adults’ future time perspective has a positive relationship with perceived learnability.
Regarding RQ3, the hypotheses that have been put forward are:
Hypothesis 7 (H7).
Older adults’ chronological age has a negative relationship with perceived usability.
Hypothesis 8 (H8).
Older adults’ loneliness has a negative relationship with perceived usability.
Hypothesis 9 (H9).
Older adults’ future time perspective has a positive relationship with perceived usability.
A visual representation of the research hypotheses is given in Figure 1.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Sampling and Data Collection Tool

The participants were recruited through snowball sampling. An online questionnaire was designed and distributed for data collection and covered four categories: (a) demographics, (b) usability, learnability, and ease-of-use (c) loneliness, and (d) future time perspective.
A popular tool for collecting users’ subjective assessment of product usability is the System Usability Scale (SUS), developed by Brooke [66]. It comprises of ten statements that sum up to a single number that represents the participant’s views upon a technology. In this study, the Greek translation of the SUS [67] was implemented for the usability category. Furthermore, based on Lewis [61], perceived ease-of-use was measured with a single item, SUS question 3, and learnability appeared in the questionnaire with SUS question 7. The Greek version of the UCLA Loneliness Scale [68] was embedded for the loneliness category. Regarding the FTP construct, Carstensen and Lang’s [69] scale was translated into Greek and further reviewed by three English literature scholars. The questionnaire was checked by an academic expert on quantitative research and was pre-tested on five older adults, which were not included in the final sample.

3.2. Procedure

The survey was online for 3 months, starting in April 2019. Each respondent who entered the survey was informed about the project through a written introduction. Then a step-by-step presentation of the platform took place. In every step, the demonstration included texts and static images describing the platform interface, as well as screen recordings of the platform manipulation. The participants could control the presentation by repeating specific parts of it or moving backward or forward until they were completely aware of how to use the platform. At the end of the presentation, they were prompted to answer the questionnaire. Similar methodologies combining the evaluation of a product using SUS after its demonstration, have been followed in other studies (e.g., [70,71]).
It has been previously described that sharing personal memories online can be a beneficial and meaningful activity for older adults. Other fields that are affected by culture are technology design and use [17,18] and they should address end-users’ needs. Thus, the web technology that was evaluated in the current study is a prototype of a digital storytelling platform that has been previously constructed using participatory design methods with five older Greek adults [21]. With this platform, users can write and edit their stories, as well as read and comment on others’ narratives (Figure 2).

3.3. Statistical Analysis

According to the Shapiro–Wilk test, participants’ age, their loneliness and FTP scores, along with their perceived ease-of-use and perceived learnability of the presented prototype, were not normally distributed (Table 1). Hence, the correlations between the variables were analyzed using a non-parametric test, Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient [72].

4. Results

4.1. Descriptive Statistics

The final sample comprised of 112 older adults (42% male, 58% female) whose age ranged from 60 to 80 years (mean: 66.96, SD: 4.98). Regarding the highest level of formal education received, data showed that 10 (9%) had rounded Primary Education, 6 (5%) and 15 (13%) had graduated from Lower and Upper Secondary Education respectively, 32 (29%) had a vocational training certificate, and 47 (42%) had at least one University degree. As for the hardware they had been using to access the Internet, 46 (41%) respondents mentioned using their desktop PC, 65 (58%) their laptop, 56 (50%) their tablet, and 89 (79%) their smartphone.
Their loneliness scores were between 21 and 64 (initial scale range: 20–80, mean: 36.96, SD: 8.77) and their scores on the FTP scale were between 10 and 67 (initial scale range: 10–70, mean: 37.43, SD: 14.20). Perceived ease-of-use (initial scale range: 1–5, mean: 3.17, SD: 1.35) and learnability (initial scale range: 1–5, mean: 3.25, SD: 1.15) values ranged from 1 to 5. As for the SUS scores, they ranged from 0 to 100 (initial scale range: 0–100, mean: 57.34, SD: 21.69).

4.2. Correlations between Variables

According to the results, there was a negative relationship between perceived ease-of-use and users’ chronological age (H1, r = −0.160, p < 0.05). There was also a statistically valid negative relationship with loneliness (H2, r = −0.210, p < 0.05) and a positive relationship with future time perspective (H3, r = 0.355, p < 0.001).
Regarding H2, participants’ perceived learnability negatively correlated with their chronological age (H4, r = −0.269, p < 0.01). Additionally, a positive relationship was found with future time perspective (H6, r = 0.216, p < 0.05), but not with loneliness (H5, r = −0.119, n.s.).
As for H3, perceived usability was negatively associated with loneliness (H8, r = −0.234, p < 0.01) and positively related to future time perspective (H9, r = 0.329, p < 0.001). However, no valid correlation was observed with chronological age (H7, r = −0.125, n.s.).
The findings are presented in detail in Table 2 and the statistically valid correlations can be seen in Figure 3.

5. Discussion

In general, the outcomes of this study focus on the relationships between two groups of variables: users’ chronological age and specific psychosocial perceptions on the one hand and perceived usability, learnability, and ease-of-use on the other hand. Analogous research prompts have been suggested within the multidisciplinary field of Gerontechnology [30] and the broader discipline of HCI [6,7].
The results obtained suggest that users’ chronological age does not affect their perceived usability, which contradicts Bangor et al. [65] findings. This outcome opposes the stereotype that new technologies and older people do not get along and is congruent with Hauk et al. [39] suggestions that age-effects are present only for technologies that do not respond to older people’s needs. Another possible explanation for the aforementioned contradiction could be related to the cultural differences between the research samples, which is a factor that often affects usability [64]. However, in contrast to the study of Jimoh et al. [53], in the current project chronological age is negatively related to perceived ease-of-use, confirming Porter and Donthu’s [52] findings. Similarly, the negative correlation found between age and learnability seems to come along with the outcomes of Baldi’s [56] and Kelley and Charness’s [57] studies.
Loneliness correlated with older users’ SUS scores, denoting that the lonelier the user is, the lower his/her score is on perceived usability. Though from a different perspective, this relationship is consistent and extends Cenfetelli’s [35] conclusions regarding the role of emotions on technology usage, as well as Díaz-Prieto and García-Sánchez’s [36] findings regarding older adults’ feelings of loneliness and web 2.0 usage patterns. Additionally, loneliness correlation with ease-of-use supports similar findings in the literature (e.g., [34,37]) regarding the link between loneliness and older adults’ cognition deficits. Nevertheless, in contrast to the connection of loneliness with older adults’ motivation to learn [33], as well as Berduzco-Torres et al. [60] reported a link between loneliness and learning, the relationship between older adults’ loneliness and learnability was not valid in the current project.
As for the FTP and its relationship with usability, ease-of-use, and learnability, the confirmation of the analogous hypotheses supports previous findings related to users’ subjective evaluation of time [27,28] and, most importantly, it seems to extend Socioemotional Selectivity Theory. In particular, the subjective estimation of time left in someone’s life is positively associated not only with his/her motives towards knowledge-based activities but also with the results of knowledge-based processes, such as the perceived usability, learnability, and ease-of-use of a technology. Despite the connection that often appears in the literature between FTP and chronological age [26], only FTP was related to the SUS scores in the current study.

6. Limitations and Future Work

This study has some limitations due to its exploratory nature. First, this project focused on a specific technology and the participants did not use the technology itself. Second, the sample was limited and non-parametric. Third, there was no feedback from older people who do not use web technologies. As a result, the findings cannot be generalized to the broader population, but can be used for further research: (i) carry out usability measurements on different web tools and with older users that have previously used those technologies, (ii) gather a broader sample under probability sampling, and (iii) collect qualitative and quantitative feedback from non-users. An experiment that could lead to safe conclusions upon causality is also suggested.

7. Conclusions

The discussions taking place in the field of HCI have implied an intersection between technology, philosophy, personal development, and emotions [2,3,6,7,8,17]. Furthermore, the needs and preferences of distinct groups of users, such as the elderly population, increasingly gain significant attention from designers and scholars (e.g., [21,22]).
During the last decades, the older adults’ population has constantly been growing [15]. However, technology adoption still lacks compared to younger generations and, notably, older Greek adults are one of the groups that are the least connected to the Internet compared to their European peers [16]. Based on the literature, it appears that culture [47,50,51,64] and psychosocial attributes [30,32,34,37] can affect users’ perceptions upon several issues, ranging from feelings of loneliness to technology usability and acceptance. Thus, this project aimed at exploring the potential relationship between (i) older Greek adults’ age, feelings of loneliness, and future time perspective and (ii) their perceived usability, learnability, and ease-of-use of a web application, in order to gain further insights into the elements that might affect technology adoption and usage. For this purpose, a digital storytelling prototype was demonstrated to 112 participants and an online structured questionnaire was implemented for data collection. According to the results, participants’ chronological age negatively correlated with learnability and ease-of-use, their feelings of loneliness negatively correlated with their perceived usability and ease-of-use, whereas future time perspective positively correlated with usability, learnability, and ease-of-use.
These findings could help technology designers, adult educators, and caregivers better understand the implicit interactions between older adults and web 2.0 tools, in order to promote the privileges derived from the acceptance and use of these innovations.

Author Contributions

D.A., K.C. and N.T. made equal contributions to this article. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

No funding was received for this study.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Maria Dalakiari, Zacharias Alexandrakis, Antonis Archontakis, Konstantinos Tziafetas, Manousos Kamilakis, Myrto-Maria Ragga, and Eirini Lioumba for their valuable help in this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Nardi, B.A. Context and Consciousness: Activity Theory and Human-Computer Interaction; MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  2. Heidegger, M. The question concerning technology. Technol. Values Essent. Read. 1954, 99, 113. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ihde, D. Technology and the Lifeworld: From Garden to Earth; Indiana University Press: Bloomington, IN, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ferrari, G.R.F. (Ed.) Plato: The Republic; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  5. Brown, H.I. Direct realism, indirect realism, and epistemology. Philos. Phenomenol. Res. 1992, 52, 341–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bødker, S. When second wave HCI meets third wave challenges. In Proceedings of the 4th Nordic Conference on Human-Computer Interaction: Changing Roles, Oslo, Norway, 14–18 October 2006; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bødker, S. Third-wave HCI, 10 years later-participation and sharing. Interactions 2015, 22, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Kaptelinin, V. Technology and the givens of existence: Toward an existential inquiry framework in HCI research. In Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Montreal, QC, Canada, 21–26 April 2018; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2018; p. 270. [Google Scholar]
  9. Singer, J.; Rexhaj, B.; Baddeley, J. Older, wiser, and happier? Comparing older adults’ and college students’ self-defining memories. Memory 2007, 15, 886–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. McLean, K.C. The emergence of narrative identity. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2008, 2, 1685–1702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Newman, E.J.; Lindsay, D.S. False memories: What the hell are they for? Appl. Cogn. Psychol. Off. J. Soc. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2009, 23, 1105–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Thiry, E.; Lindley, S.; Banks, R.; Regan, T. Authoring personal histories: Exploring the timeline as a framework for meaning making. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Paris, France, 27 April–2 May 2013; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 1619–1628. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hausknecht, S.; Vanchu-Orosco, M.; Kaufman, D. Digitising the wisdom of our elders: Connectedness through digital storytelling. Ageing Soc. 2019, 39, 2714–2734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Morganti, L.; Riva, G.; Bonfiglio, S.; Gaggioli, A. Building collective memories on the web: The Nostalgia Bits project. Int. J. Web Based Communities 2013, 9, 83–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. United Nations. World Population Ageing 2019: Highlights. 2019. Available online: https://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/publications/pdf/ageing/WorldPopulationAgeing2019-Highlights.pdf (accessed on 17 September 2020).
  16. Eurostat. Ageing Europe—Looking at the Lives of Older People in the EU. 2017. Available online: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/en/web/products-statistical-books/-/KS-02-19-681 (accessed on 16 September 2020).
  17. Rousi, R.; Leikas, J.; Saariluoma, P. Emotions in Technology Design: From Experience to Ethics; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  18. Tedre, M.; Sutinen, E.; Kähkönen, E.; Kommers, P. Ethnocomputing: ICT in cultural and social context. Commun. ACM 2006, 49, 126–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Knowles, B.; Hanson, V.L. The Wisdom of Older Technology (Non-) Users. Commun. ACM 2018, 61, 72–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Waycott, J.; Vetere, F.; Pedell, S.; Morgans, A.; Ozanne, E.; Kulik, L. Not for me: Older adults choosing not to participate in a social isolation intervention. In Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 745–757. [Google Scholar]
  21. Alexandrakis, D.; Chorianopoulos, K.; Tselios, N. Insights on Older Adults’ Attitudes and Behavior Through the Participatory Design of an Online Storytelling Platform. IFIP Conference on Human-Computer Interaction; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 465–474. [Google Scholar]
  22. Lindley, S.E. Before I forget: From personal memory to family history. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2012, 27, 13–36. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dang, Y.; Munteanu, C.; Epp, C.D. Engaging Seniors through Automatically-Generated Photo Digests from their Families’ Social Media. In Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services, Taipei, Taiwan, 1–4 October 2019; pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  24. Yang, M. Health information literacy of the older adults and their intention to share health rumors: An analysis from the perspective of socioemotional selectivity theory. In International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 97–108. [Google Scholar]
  25. Alexandrakis, D.; Chorianopoulos, K.; Tselios, N. Implicit factors related to Greek older adults’ perceived usability of online technologies: An exploratory study. In Proceedings of the 13th ACM International Conference on PErvasive Technologies Related to Assistive Environments, Corfu, Greece, 29 June–2 July 2020; pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lang, F.; Carstensen, L. Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychol. Aging 2002, 17, 125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Alexandrakis, D. Factors related to computer and internet use during the Third Age: Results from an empirical research in Greece. Gerontechnology 2019, 18, 47–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Setyadi, H.J.; Widagdo, P.P.; Susanto, T.D. Cognitive age and chronological age of the technostress that effect on satisfaction, performance, and intention of continue the use of information technology in the university. In Proceedings of the 2017 3rd International Conference on Science in Information Technology (ICSITech), Bandung, Indonesia, 25–26 October 2017; IEEE: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 330–335. [Google Scholar]
  29. Doll, W.J.; Torkzadeh, G. The measurement of end-user computing satisfaction. MIS Q. 1988, 12, 259–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Chen, K.; Chan, A.H. A review of technology acceptance by older adults. Gerontechnology 2011, 10, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Dykstra, P.A. Older adult loneliness: Myths and realities. Eur. J. Ageing 2009, 6, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Perlman, D.; Peplau, L.A. Toward a social psychology of loneliness. Pers. Relationsh. 1981, 3, 31–56. [Google Scholar]
  33. Cai, J.; Kosaka, M. Learner-engaged curriculum co-development in Older Adult Education: Lessons learned from the universities for older adults in China. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2019, 98, 36–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. O’Luanaigh, C.; O’Connell, H.; Chin, A.V.; Hamilton, F.; Coen, R.; Walsh, C.; Walsh, J.; Caokley, D.; Cunningham, C.; Lawlor, B. Loneliness and cognition in older people: The Dublin Healthy Ageing study. Aging Ment. Health 2012, 16, 347–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cenfetelli, R.T. Getting in touch with our feelings towards technology. In Academy of Management Proceedings; Academy of Management Briarcliff: Manor, NY, USA, 2004; pp. F1–F6. [Google Scholar]
  36. Díaz-Prieto, C.; García-Sánchez, J.N. Psychological profiles of older adult Web 2.0 tool users. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 64, 673–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kidambi, N.; Lee, E.E. Insight into Potential Mechanisms linking Loneliness and Cognitive Decline: Commentary on “Health factors as potential mediator the longitudinal effect of loneliness on general cognitive ability”. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Rozas, A.X.P.; Juncos-Rabadán, O.; González, M.S.R. Processing speed, inhibitory control, and working memory: Three important factors to account for age-related cognitive decline. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 2008, 66, 115–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Hauk, N.; Hüffmeier, J.; Krumm, S. Ready to be a silver surfer? A meta-analysis on the relationship between chronological age and technology acceptance. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 84, 304–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Gordon, M.L.; Gatys, L.; Guestrin, C.; Bigham, J.P.; Trister, A.; Patel, K. App usage predicts cognitive ability in older adults. In Proceedings of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Scotland, UK, 4–9 May 2019; pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  41. Fairweather, P.G. How older and younger adults differ in their approach to problem solving on a complex website. In Proceedings of the 10th International ACM SIGACCESS Conference on Computers and Accessibility, Halifax, NS, Canada, 13–15 October 2008; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 67–72. [Google Scholar]
  42. Dubus, N.M. Self-perception of when old age begins for Cambodian elders living in the United States. J. Cross Cult. Gerontol. 2014, 29, 185–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Davis, F.D. Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of information technology. MIS Q. 1989, 13, 319–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Hansen, J.M.; Saridakis, G.; Benson, V. Risk, trust, and the interaction of perceived ease of use and behavioral control in predicting consumers’ use of social media for transactions. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 80, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Venkatesh, V.; Bala, H. Technology acceptance model 3 and a research agenda on interventions. Decis. Sci. 2008, 39, 273–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Venkatesh, V.; Davis, F.D. A theoretical extension of the technology acceptance model: Four longitudinal field studies. Manag. Sci. 2000, 46, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Chen, K.; Chan, A.H.S. Gerontechnology acceptance by elderly Hong Kong Chinese: A senior technology acceptance model (STAM). Ergonomics 2014, 57, 635–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ramírez-Correa, P.; Grandón, E.E.; Ramírez-Santana, M.; Belmar Órdenes, L. Explaining the use of social network sites as seen by older adults: The enjoyment component of a hedonic information system. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Renaud, K.; Van Biljon, J. Predicting technology acceptance and adoption by the elderly: A qualitative study. In Proceedings of the 2008 Annual Research Conference of the South African Institute of Computer Scientists and Information Technologists on IT Research in Developing Countries: Riding the Wave of Technology, Wilderness, South Africa, 6–8 October 2008; pp. 210–219. [Google Scholar]
  50. Dutot, V.; Bhatiasevi, V.; Bellallahom, N. Applying the technology acceptance model in a three-countries study of smartwatch adoption. J. High Technol. Manag. Res. 2019, 30, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Straub, D.; Keil, M.; Brenner, W. Testing the technology acceptance model across cultures: A three country study. Inf. Manag. 1997, 33, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Porter, C.E.; Donthu, N. Using the technology acceptance model to explain how attitudes determine Internet usage: The role of perceived access barriers and demographics. J. Bus. Res. 2006, 59, 999–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Jimoh, L.; Pate, M.A.; Lin, L.; Schulman, K.A. A model for the adoption of ICT by health workers in Africa. Int. J. Med. Inform. 2012, 81, 773–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Yu-Huei, C.; Ja-Shen, C.; Ming-Chao, W. Why Do Older Adults Use Wearable Devices: A Case Study Adopting the Senior Technology Acceptance Model (STAM). In Proceedings of the 2019 Portland International Conference on Management of Engineering and Technology (PICMET), Portland, OR, USA, 25–29 August 2019; IEEE: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  55. Caboral-Stevens, M.; Whetsell, M.V.; Evangelista, L.S.; Cypress, B.; Nickitas, D. USABILITY framework for older adults. Res. Gerontol. Nurs. 2015, 8, 300–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Baldi, R.A. Training older adults to use the computer: Issues related to the workplace, attitudes, and training. Educ. Gerontol. Int. Q. 1997, 23, 453–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kelley, C.L.; Charness, N. Issues in training older adults to use computers. Behav. Inf. Technol. 1995, 14, 107–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Howe, W.A.; Lisi, P.L. Becoming a Multicultural Educator: Developing Awareness, Gaining Skills, and Taking Action; Sage Publications: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  59. Morgan, T.J.; Suchow, J.W.; Griffiths, T.L. What the Baldwin Effect affects depends on the nature of plasticity. Cognition 2020, 197, 104165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Berduzco-Torres, N.; Medina, P.; Choquenaira-Callañaupa, B.; San-Martín, M.; Delgado Bolton, R.C.; Vivanco, L. Family loneliness: Its effects in the development of empathy, teamwork and lifelong learning abilities in medical students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Lewis, J.R. The system usability scale: Past, present, and future. Int. J. Hum.–Comput. Interact. 2018, 34, 577–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. International Organization for Standardization. Ergonomics of Human-System Interaction—Part 11: Usability: Definitions and Concepts. 2018. Available online: https://www.iso.org/standard/63500.html (accessed on 17 September 2020).
  63. Zheng, X.S.; Lin, J.J.; Zapf, S.; Knapheide, C. Visualizing user experience through “perceptual maps”: Concurrent assessment of perceived usability and subjective appearance in car infotainment systems. In International Conference on Digital Human Modeling; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2007; pp. 536–545. [Google Scholar]
  64. Wallace, S.; Yu, H.C. The effect of culture on usability: Comparing the perceptions and performance of Taiwanese and North American MP3 player users. J. Usability Stud. 2009, 4, 136–146. [Google Scholar]
  65. Bangor, A.; Kortum, P.T.; Miller, J.T. An empirical evaluation of the system usability scale. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2008, 24, 574–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Brooke, J. SUS: A “quick and dirty” usability scale. In Usability Evaluation in Industry; Jordan, P.W., Thomas, B., Weerdmeester, B.A., McClelland, I.L., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: London, UK, 1996; pp. 189–194. [Google Scholar]
  67. Katsanos, C.; Tselios, N.; Xenos, M. Perceived usability evaluation of learning management systems: A first step towards standardization of the System Usability Scale in Greek. In Proceedings of the 2012 16th Panhellenic Conference on Informatics, Piraeus, Greece, 5–7 October 2012; IEEE: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 302–307. [Google Scholar]
  68. Kafetsios, K.; Sideridis, G. Attachment, social support and well-being in young and older adults. J. Health Psychol. 2006, 11, 863–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Carstensen, L.; Lang, F. Future Time Perspective (FTP) Scale. Available online: https://lifespan.stanford.edu/projects/future-time-perspective-ftp-scale (accessed on 16 May 2019).
  70. Bonfim, G.; Medola, F.; Paschoarelli, L. Usability in the development of packaging design: Complementing the subjective evaluation. In Advances in Ergonomics Modeling, Usability & Special Populations; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 67–74. [Google Scholar]
  71. Nguyen, L.; Nie, Q.; Myers, D.; Gibson, A.; Kerssens, C.; Mudar, R.; Rogers, W. Social engagement through video chat for older individuals with and without cognitive impairment. Innov. Aging 2018, 2, 998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Cohen, L.; Manion, L.; Morrison, K. Research Methods in Education; Routledge: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Visual representation of the research hypotheses.
Figure 1. Visual representation of the research hypotheses.
Technologies 08 00057 g001
Figure 2. Initial page of the digital storytelling platform [21].
Figure 2. Initial page of the digital storytelling platform [21].
Technologies 08 00057 g002
Figure 3. Visual representation of the statistically valid relationships between the variables (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001).
Figure 3. Visual representation of the statistically valid relationships between the variables (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001).
Technologies 08 00057 g003
Table 1. Shapiro–Wilk normality test.
Table 1. Shapiro–Wilk normality test.
VariableTypeStatisticSig.
AgeScale0.9410.000
LonelinessOrdinal0.9740.030
FTPOrdinal0.9680.009
Perceived Ease-of-UseOrdinal0.8950.000
Perceived LearnabilityOrdinal0.9130.000
UsabilityOrdinal0.9800.098
Table 2. Variables’ correlations.
Table 2. Variables’ correlations.
HypothesisResearch QuestionVariablesCorrelation CoefficientSig. (1-Tailed)Accepted
H1RQ1Age and Ease-of-Use−0.1600.046Yes
H2RQ1Loneliness and Ease-of-Use−0.2100.013Yes
H3RQ1FTP and Ease-of-Use0.3550.000Yes
H4RQ2Age and Learnability−0.2690.002Yes
H5RQ2Loneliness and Learnability−0.1190.106No
H6RQ2FTP and Learnability0.2160.011Yes
H7RQ3Age and Usability−0.1250.094No
H8RQ3Loneliness and Usability−0.2340.007Yes
H9RQ3FTP and Usability0.3290.000Yes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Alexandrakis, D.; Chorianopoulos, K.; Tselios, N. Connections between Older Greek Adults’ Implicit Attributes and Their Perceptions of Online Technologies. Technologies 2020, 8, 57. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/technologies8040057

AMA Style

Alexandrakis D, Chorianopoulos K, Tselios N. Connections between Older Greek Adults’ Implicit Attributes and Their Perceptions of Online Technologies. Technologies. 2020; 8(4):57. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/technologies8040057

Chicago/Turabian Style

Alexandrakis, Diogenis, Konstantinos Chorianopoulos, and Nikolaos Tselios. 2020. "Connections between Older Greek Adults’ Implicit Attributes and Their Perceptions of Online Technologies" Technologies 8, no. 4: 57. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/technologies8040057

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop