Next Article in Journal
Effects of Japanese Special Moras Education Using Evernote
Previous Article in Journal
Competency-Based Education as Curriculum and Assessment for Integrative Learning
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Assisted Discovery Based Learning of the Electric Force with Scaffolding for Novice Students

by
Osvaldo Aquines Gutiérrez
1,*,
Ross K. Galloway
2,
Ayax Santos
1,
Humberto Martínez-Huerta
1 and
Héctor González
1
1
Department of Physics and Mathematics, Universidad de Monterrey, Avenida Morones Prieto 4500, San Pedro Garza García 66238, NL, Mexico
2
School of Physics & Astronomy, The University of Edinburgh, James Clerk Maxwell Building, King’s Buildings, Peter Guthrie Tait Road, Edinburgh EH9 3FD, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 24 February 2022 / Revised: 7 April 2022 / Accepted: 7 April 2022 / Published: 11 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Higher Education)

Abstract

:
Despite being one of the pillars of physics and a well-known subject, the electric force is sometimes not immediately evident to students. Discovery-based learning has proven to be very effective in science education; nevertheless, it can become stressful for students if they don’t have the necessary scaffolding and training to construct knowledge by themselves. In this work, examples of obstacles to students were the absence of the necessary background knowledge, distractions in multimedia environments, and that some students prefer regular instruction in order not to be left alone in the face of active learning dynamics. Therefore, we designed and implemented an assisted active learning sequence that includes moderated intervention from two different lecturers on an Electricity and Magnetism course at a private university in Mexico to compare the normalized learning changes with a standard directed lecture. The primary objective of the active learning sequence was to introduce students to the discovery of the electric force via a simulated experiment using a web-based physics simulation, Newtondreams. By comparing normalized learning changes between four groups, two control, and two experimental groups, we show that students in the experimental groups performed significantly better than the control groups ( c = 0.469 and 0.435 for the experimental groups, and c = 0.08 and 0.077 for the control groups). We performed a Wilcoxon Rank-Sum Test to examine the normalized gains between the groups. Observed p-values show that there is significant difference between experimental and control groups: CG1 vs. EG1 (p = 0.00109), CG2 vs. EG2 (p = 0.00079). On the contrary there are no observed significance on the effect of the instructor CG1 vs. CG2 (p = 1), EG1 vs. EG2 (p = 1). Then, that active learning is more effective than regular instruction. Studying the concentration factors we also found that active learning develop better comprehension that regular instruction lecture. At the end of the experiments, we performed student interviews that also showed they felt less stressed and more involved when using the assisted active learning sequence, making the learning experience more amenable when the instructor changes their role from presenter to a guide.

1. Introduction

In the last years, in the area of physics teaching, the results of various investigations provide evidence and knowledge that promote the use of innovative strategies based on active learning and student-centered, which improve the academic outcomes compared to those obtained from traditional teacher-centered instruction [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]. For example, Ref. [1] define active learning as “interactive engagement methods promote conceptual understanding through the interactive engagement of students in heads-on (always) and hands-on (usually) activities which yield immediate individual feedback to all students through discussion with peers and/or instructors”. Besides active learning, the students can be engaged with the course material through problem-solving, case studies, interactive lecture demonstrations, etc.
In an interactive demonstration [8,9,10,11], the student challenges their prior understanding of a concept, making a prediction of the outcome of the demonstration. After that, in small groups, they experience the demonstration using a simulation, collecting or working with data, taking a survey, or conducting an experiment with the intention to determine if their initial beliefs were confirmed or not. Finally, the students compare their beliefs with the results of the demonstrations and make a reflection on them. In such demonstrations the use of interactive simulations in the undergraduate physics classroom, becomes even more effective due to student engagement and knowledge ownership [8,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21]. Such discovery-based learning can be assisted or unassisted. Nevertheless, many authors maintain that it shouldn’t be applied totally unassisted [22]. Similarly, discovery-based learning is very effective in science education. Still, it can become stressful for students if they don’t have the necessary scaffolding and training to construct knowledge by themselves [23,24]. In this situation where students fall in a cognitive gap that prevents them from finishing the task individually, Vygotsky [25] stated, in his brain-based approach to learning, that there is a zone of proximal development in which, with appropriate assistance or guidance, they could achieve the task. Also, Brunner [26] believed that discovery-based learning could not be made a priori or without at least some knowledge base in the domain in question. More effectively, guided discovery takes the student through the path of experimentation avoiding knowledge gaps that could otherwise interfere in the learning process. Moreover, it is beneficial in developing the initial stages of executive function [27]. However, in practice, interruptions and distractions interfere with interactive learning and affect the learning environment considerably [28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35], making the development of new strategies necessary.

1.1. Background

Since many students did not have prior formal instruction on the topic (e.g., no electricity high school course), the strategy used constructivism based on their basic notions [36,37,38,39,40], they start with their own concepts and interpretative schemes of a broad spectrum of Physics topics. For instance, Ref. [37] said that, “a sense of mechanism is the knowledge that provides us with the capability to: (i) Assess the likelihood of various events based on generalizations about what does and does not happen. (ii) Make predictions and postdiction. That is, one can trace entailments forward or backward in time, explaining what will happen on the basis of what is the case, and explaining what must have been the case in order for the present circumstances to exist. (iii) Give causal descriptions and explanations. That is, one can look at a physical event and assign credit or blame for what happens to certain aspects of the circumstances and to general facts about the world”.
In the particular context, we had found that many of our students have not seen before (high school) the electric force concept in the classroom or do not remember it at all. This represented another challenge for the application of active learning. In our prior attempts, students stated that they felt lost and preferred direct instruction from the lecturer because of the lack of prior formal instruction in the topic. Previous to this work, we conducted a pilot survey to half of the students in the physic courses, thus, we randomly selected 61 students to analyze their electric force concept’s background. It is worth mentioning that the 61 random students in the pilot survey belonged to a previous generation; hence, they were not part of the sample for the main work. It showed that only 30% remembered Coulomb’s Law from their high school courses; the others (57%) commented not remembering any electricity topic. This test also noted that our sample of students’ background is heterogeneous; only 45% of the total came from a concentration of engineering; the others from areas such as business, health, programming, and business management (see Appendix A for more details on the survey).
This also reveals a local reality, in Mexico, that almost no student demonstrated high proficiency in science, reaching a proficiency level of 5 or 6 at the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2018 [41]. About 53% of students reached level 2 or higher in science. At level 2, students can recognize the correct explanation for familiar scientific phenomena and can use such knowledge to identify, in simple cases, whether a conclusion is valid based on the data provided. At level 5 or 6, students can creatively and autonomously apply their knowledge of science in a wide variety of situations, including unfamiliar situations.
In 2010, the educational system in Mexico experienced several changes, including introducing the teaching methodology based on competences; this methodology is still currently in use. While this system intends to promote social and learning skills rather than just transmit knowledge, its implementation has represented a major challenge for instructors [42], resulting in difficulty in developing the promoted skills [43]. In our first attempts to implement interactive learning in the classroom and physics courses, we found many obstacles, such as finding a suitable digital physics simulation that fulfilled the technical and academic requirements for the lesson. Besides, when students access the web, they also faced distractions from the virtual environments and the web itself, so that it is hard for them to remain focused on the task.
Distractions in virtual environments and websites such as instant messaging, streaming music and non-music audio, video streaming, games, mobile and computer-based applications, web surfing, etc., represent an additional challenge for some students, which may be associated with their ability to filter the distractors effectively (see for example [31]). This is why it is important to reduce distractions and interruptions to achieve the greatest possible deep engagement. In the application of virtual activities, it is, for example, very convenient to facilitate and reduce navigation between virtual tools, such as moving between different software or operating systems to run the virtual tools that can make learners face distractions [32]. For such reasons, it was decided to create and release our own web-based experiment repository: Newtondreams [44]. These simulations are ready to be used without any additional configuration nor software package installation, which helps to reduce distractions. Newtondreams covers three main digital simulator areas: Physics, Statistics, and Mathematics. A short discussion on the repository and the simulation used in this work is made in Section 1.2.
In this work, we designed and applied an assisted active learning method to help students develop knowledge without being overwhelmed due to knowledge gaps or by their lack of acquaintance with active learning. For such purposes, we designed and studied a learning sequence with limited instructor assistance using, as a cognitive tool, the digital simulation NewtonDreams [44]. We divided a sample of 140 second-year undergraduate engineering students at a private university in Mexico into control and experimental groups to get two types of lessons, one being a direct instruction class and the other, an assisted active learning sequence. We present our methodology in Section 2. We compare and discuss the effectiveness of both lesson types and present the obtained results in Section 3. Finally, in Section 4 we share our conclusions.

1.2. The Web Based Simulation: Newtondreams

Physics simulations have been observed to be highly engaging and educationally effective cognitive tools [45,46,47,48]. Their use has promoted a constructivist learning environment [49,50], where creativity is motivated by familiarising and encouraging the student to interact with physics phenomena. There are many initiatives in this direction, such as Open Source Physics [51], Conceptual Learning Of Science [52], and PhET Interactive Simulations [53], the most widely used. All of them attempt to be self-exploratory by students.
However, because we attempt to provide a quickly accessible cognitive tool even with a limited internet connection, the simulation used in this study belongs to the Newtondreams project [44], a web-based repository which can be accessed through the web-site www.newtondreams.com (accessed on 1 April 2022). Newtondreams aims to provide simulations for guided virtual experiments with a low computational cost and no restrictions of pre-installed software or technical specifications for available computing equipment. With such intentions, simulations are programmed directly in HTML/Javascript, so they would not require the downloading of extra libraries or packages and would then be operating system independent. The simulations can run on any device such as tablets, smartphones, PCs, laptops, and even smart TVs as long as they have a web browser. Currently, it is only available in Spanish. To run the simulation, students only need to load the simulation once into the browser; then, it can run even without an internet connection.
To help students focus on the main topic, simulations are depicted in a plain environment with a straightforward menu; each one has a main illustration of the physics concept to be studied and a pair of sliders where students can manipulate the main variables. This way students, can observe the relation between the involved parameters (e.g., what happens to the electric force as the magnitude of the charges increases).
In this study, students worked in groups. We requested at least one laptop per group to run the simulation as a team, though sometimes students desired to attend the activity individually on their smartphones or tablets. Complementary, because the lecturer interacts personally with the groups, the continuity of the session is supported, and distractions such as those present on the web are avoided.
The cognitive tool simulation used in this study is named Fuerza Electrica (Electric Force). It presents two charges that students can move in different positions in the x-y plane by dragging and sliding from each other, to change the electric force magnitude and direction. This allows students to visualize the effect of distance, magnitude, and the sign of the charge in the electric force and look at the superposition concept using 3 or more charges. It presents the exact values of the charges, forces, and their angle from the positive x-axis. Students can deduce or prove the precise relationship between variables and test their predictions. An example can be seen Figure 1.

2. Methodology

This work aims to compare the effectiveness between a direct instruction lecture and an assisted active learning sequence. So that, the research questions (RQ) are:
  • RQ1. Is an assisted active learning session more effective than regular instruction for novice students?
  • RQ2. Does guided virtual experimentation achieve a better comprehension than a textbook-based strategy?
For this purpose, we divided a sample of 140 second-year undergraduate engineering students enrolled in the Electricity and Magnetism course at a private university in Mexico into control and experimental groups as described in Table 1. Two lecturers with plenty of teaching experience in the electromagnetism course participated in the experiment; each one led a control and experimental group. Since it is the first-course session, the students take the session without prior preparation in both the experiment and control group.

2.1. The Control Groups

The control group received a 60 min regular instruction session which was lecturer-centered using the blackboard and a PowerPoint presentation as a visual aid. The content follows the Chapter 23 in [54]. The lecture was organized as follows, lecturers made an introduction of the topic, then explained Coulomb’s inverse-square law, then they solved textbook exercises.

2.2. The Experimental Groups

In the experimental groups, the 140 students were randomly assigned into teams of 3 to 4. To each team, the lecturer gave a single active learning sequence on 60 min long, which aimed to guide the student to discover the electric force concept by experimentation using a web-based physics simulation that provides virtual contact with the phenomena. Once students were divided into groups, they were ready to start the activity on their work-spaces, the lecturer presented the Newtondreams simulation and show them its basic functionality. The learning goal is to understand the force’s vector nature and the relationship between force, charge, and distance.
In the experimental groups, six tickets were given to the students at the beginning of the session at Part 1. Lecturers explained to students that tickets represented an opportunity to ask the teacher a straightforward question guiding the students with hints, trigger questions, analogies, etc., if they felt they needed, for instance, when they had specific questions, doubts, or if they felt stuck in the learning process. This also kept them from feeling anxious about the exercise. Furthermore, when students used tickets, lecturers were instructed to help them conclude and solve their doubts by themselves.
The active learning sequence consist of 3 main parts,
  • (∼15 min) The qualitative exploration; in this part, students are asked to make predictions of the behavior of the forces between electric charges; afterwards, they probe their predictions using the simulations. Finally, they describe the observed behavior with simple statements in their own words. They explore the relationship between force, charge, and distances and the vector nature of electric force. In this part, lecturers specified that it is an exploration activity, and there would be no penalties, especially if their predictions were incorrect. See for instance, the top panel of Figure 2 to see questions examples asked for the students predictions.
  • (∼20 min) The quantitative exploration. Students analyzed the numerical relationship between variables through trigger questions in the sequence.
  • (∼25 min) The synthesizes. Students identify the equations and models that describe the phenomena, the Coulombs law. In Figure 3, the are asked to build a table of F vs. r and then plot it with the aid of MS excel. Then they fit a curve to the data and find which formula fits better the observed data. They find that the best formula is r 2 . Further, in the sequence’s next item, they identify the inverse square relationship between F and r.
Figure 2. Student sample responses in the active sequence. Questions examples were asked for the students’ predictions in the qualitative exploration between the charges. (4) Question. What do you think that will happen to the force if the distance between them increases? Explain your reasoning. Students made predictions based on the fundamental notions and answered (Spanish) “The force decreases, since the further away Q1 is, the less they influence Q2”. Then, (5) they were asked to probe it with the simulation and answer a qualitative description. (5) Question. Test your prediction made in (4). Move Q2 away and then move it closer What can be said about the electric force and its relationship with the distance between charges? What happens when the charges are further away? What happens when they are closer?. Answer, “After testing it, we could affirm that the distance influences the magnitude of the force exerted; it increases if it is closer and decreases when it is further away”.
Figure 2. Student sample responses in the active sequence. Questions examples were asked for the students’ predictions in the qualitative exploration between the charges. (4) Question. What do you think that will happen to the force if the distance between them increases? Explain your reasoning. Students made predictions based on the fundamental notions and answered (Spanish) “The force decreases, since the further away Q1 is, the less they influence Q2”. Then, (5) they were asked to probe it with the simulation and answer a qualitative description. (5) Question. Test your prediction made in (4). Move Q2 away and then move it closer What can be said about the electric force and its relationship with the distance between charges? What happens when the charges are further away? What happens when they are closer?. Answer, “After testing it, we could affirm that the distance influences the magnitude of the force exerted; it increases if it is closer and decreases when it is further away”.
Education 12 00269 g002

2.3. Assesment Tools

At the beginning and the end of each session, all students answered a 15 min pre-test and post-test, and some students participated afterwards in a 25 min interview, which were another form of collecting data. Scores were calculated giving, one point per correct answer and zero points otherwise. These tests was based on questions 3–9 of the Conceptual Survey of Electricity and Magnetism (CSEM) [55]. In Section 3, we discuss the results. The relation between CSEM questions and concepts are shown in Table 2.

3. Discussion and Results

3.1. Comparing the Effectiveness between Sessions (RQ1)

To quantify and compare the efficiency of two teaching methods, it is common to use the normalized learning gain defined by Hake [56], given by
g = p o s t p r e 100 % p r e ,
where p o s t and p r e are the student’s post-test and pre-test scores out of 100%. Equation (1) represents the change in the average score over the maximum possible gain, “the fraction of concepts learned by a class that was not known at the beginning of the course” [57]. The normalized gain has been used to evaluate the teaching interventions in physics all the way back to [56]; since then, it has been widely used as a measure in PER and well-explored (see for instance [57,58]). It has also been used in some of the most seminal studies of reformed teaching practices and teaching interventions in physics, see for instance, Ref. [17] on Peer Instruction, and specifically in the case of electricity and magnetism using CSEM, Ref. [59].
Furthermore, because it has been argued that normalized gains can be biased at the extreme values of p r e scores [60], in this work, to quantify and compare the efficiency of both methods, we use the normalized learning change as defined by Marx and Cummins in [61],
c = p o s t p r e 100 % p r e , p o s t > p r e . drop , p o s t = p r e = 100 or 0 . 0 , p o s t = p r e . p o s t p r e p r e , p o s t < p r e .
Once again, p o s t and p r e are the student’s average post-test and pre-test scores out of 100%. In addition, we have also computed a correlation test between g, c, and the p r e -scores. The obtained p-values are p g , p r e = 0.0206 and p c , p r e = 0.3311 , for the normalized gain and change respectively. Since p c , p r e it is not significant, then c is less sensitive to p r e -test than g. Hence, we decided to compute the normalized change for each group case, as presented in Table 1. Figure 4 shows our results. The closer to one, the better the learning is. Using R software [62], we found that the normalized change medians are 0.18, 0.0, 0.5, and 0.5, for CG1, CG2, EG1, and EG2, respectively, they are indicated as solid lines in the figure. We also include the normalized change quarterlies. Observed average gains of experimental groups were significantly higher than the control groups, being c = 0.469 and c = 0.435 for the the lecturer 1 and 2, and c = 0.08 and c = 0.077 for the control groups.
We also performed a Wilcoxon Rank-Sum Test, using R software [62], to examine RQ1 by comparing normalized gains between the groups. Observed p-values show that there is significant difference between experimental and control groups: CG1 vs. EG1 (p = 0.00109), CG2 vs. EG2 (p = 0.00079). In general experimental groups show higher normalized gains. On the contrary there are no observed significance on the effect of the instructor CG1 vs. CG2 (p =1), EG1 vs. EG2 (p = 1). We show the descriptive statistics in Table 3 and the Wilcoxon Rank-Sum test in Table 4. These outcomes allowed to answer RQ1, supporting that active learning is more effective than regular instruction.

3.2. Comparing the Developed Comprehension (RQ2)

To measuring the comprehension between the regular lecture and the active learning session, we use the concentration factor, that is the students’ thinking schemes and how they change after each learning session. We estimate the concentration factor as defined by Bao and Redish [63],
C = m m 1 × i = 1 m n i 2 N 1 m .
where m stands for the number of different choices on one question, n is the total number of students who selected choice i, and N = i = 1 m n i is the total number of responses. Equation (3) is a measure of the students’ answers distributions. It is a normalized variable: if values are larger there are more concentrated answers, with one being a perfectly correlated response and zero a random answer. To have a graphical representation, Bao and Redish propose constructing an “S-C” plot, where S is the test score and C the concentration factor given by Equation (3). S and C can be divided into three different levels, High (H), Medium (M), and Low (L). When combining score and concentration factor, there are six different regions representing the level of concentration. Depending if the responses are concentrated on one choice, two choices or somewhat evenly distributed among three or more choices, these regions are classified into the following categories: one correct model (HH), one dominant incorrect model (LH), two possible incorrect models (LM), two popular models (MM) (correct and incorrect), and near random situation (LL) [63].
To compare the improvements after instruction, we made “S-C” plots for both experiment and control groups in Figure 5 and Figure 6, respectively, and list the change per question in the scores and concentration factors, Δ S and Δ C, in Table 5. We observe that the increases in S and C are larger for the experimental group. The most significant differences are in items 2 and 5, related to Newton’s Third Law, in which the control group even has negative values for Δ S and Δ C. We note these changes between S-C factors before and after the lessons with the blue arrows on both figures. The improvements in the S-C factors of the experimental groups is clear. Arrows are larger, and their direction is always towards higher scores and concentration factors. Moreover, contrary to the control group, all points after the lesson in the experimental group are in the MM and HM regions.
The HH (upper-right) is the most desirable region because it is dominated by accurate responses, while LL (lower-left) is the least desirable because a random error dominates it in responses. As we can observe in Figure 5, in most items, the average response in the experiment group migrates from LL towards HH after the active learning session, meaning that responses are no longer random but with definite and accurate reasoning behind them. On the contrary, in the control group (Figure 6), not all items migrate from LL towards HH. Some items even go backward after the session. This means that some concepts remain unclear in a direct instruction lecture, and thus, students still answer randomly. In general terms, comparing Figure 5 and Figure 6, we can observe by the length of the arrows that the increase of accuracy is larger in the experiment group. These outcomes allowed to answer RQ2, supporting that assisted active learning develop better comprehension that regular instruction lecture.

3.3. Observations on the Question Tickets

When facing doubt or being stuck in the learning process, we want students to argue between themselves before asking the lecturer, so each group received only six question tickets. If they decided to use one of the tickets, the instructor would assist them, giving hints or presenting the situation in different perspectives to help them conclude and solve their doubts by themselves. The questions tickets worked in the intended way. Students worked collaboratively to deduce what they were asked in the sequence because they did not want to spend a ticket. 89% of the teams spent one ticket on the item concerning Newton’s Third Law. Their initial prediction was that when there are two charges of different magnitude, the larger one exerts a greater force on the small one than the other way around. Students were shocked when they saw in the simulation that both forces were equal; some teams even claimed the simulation was wrong. For that reason, they spent one question ticket to clarify their doubts. 44% of teams spent one ticket when trying to deduce the multiplicative effect of charges. A few other tickets were spent in the superposition of forces when one of the charges was negative. It is worth mentioning that many question tickets were spent even when at least one of the students had deduced the correct answer, but the team could not reach an agreement on the answer. It seems that when the student population has very heterogeneous background knowledge, they have trouble reaching agreement. No team spent all of the question tickets.

3.4. Student Interviews

At the end of the experiment, we did interviews with some students to explore their opinion on the activity. This interview lasted 25 min. The objective was to characterize the students’ experience of the methodology. We conducted the interviews with 18 students from 73 of the experimental groups, including at least one member of each working group. To obtain open and direct opinions, we held informal group discussions where we asked eight specific questions. This group was selected using a randomized stratified sample, making sure that at least one student per group of the nine original groups was included. An external researcher led the group interviews to avoid any possible bias from the instructors. Here we list some of the representative student answers (between quotation marks) and our comments to five of the eight questions we asked.
  • How did you feel with this teaching-learning method? Did you like it or didn’t you like it? Do you prefer to work individually? What do you think?
    -
    “It gave us a clearer idea of what electricity really is; I had no idea how the forces were going to behave. This way is better than the teacher explaining everything; you are playing with the app with this method”.
    -
    “I liked it a lot because it takes you step by step on what you have to do and also you could get to the predictions on your own and verify the results”.
    -
    “I liked the simulation a lot because I’m very visual”.
  • How about working on teams?
    -
    “It’s ok to work in teams but in smaller groups, two or three persons, the dynamic works better”.
    -
    “It’s ok because someone else from your team can help you if you don’t know something”.
  • Do you think that it is better to learn this way or do you prefer the traditional method?
    -
    “Instead of replacing the classic method, both can be complemented. This helps to generate questions, and then with the teacher, you can understand them better”.
    -
    “This way is easier because you have a question, and you have to look for the answer and find why. It helps you to reason the problem”.
  • What advantages or disadvantages do you find in the working method?
    Disadvantages.
    -
    “Small groups, 3 or 2 persons”.
    -
    “Sometimes you really don’t know what to answer because you don’t know anything about the subject”.
    Advantages.
    -
    “The classic method of teaching for the theory, and the other for visualizing everything better”.
    -
    “Both methods complement, then when the teacher explains the subject, we already have an idea and are familiarized with the concepts, and we can understand it better”.
    -
    “It gives you previous knowledge about what you are going to see in class”.
    -
    “It gives you visual support”.
  • Do you feel more or less pressure with this method?
    All students agreed that they felt less pressure.
    -
    “It gives you a clearer vision about the concepts”.
    -
    “It helps you to interact with the concepts, because of the structure; first you make a prediction, then you can verify it in the program”.
    -
    “It doesn’t matter if you were right or wrong, it’s a prediction”.
    -
    “This method is more relax”.
  • What do you think of the question tickets?
    -
    “We didn’t have to worry about the number of questions because of the method. We had the possibility to answer our own questions with the app”.
    -
    “They do work because at the beginning with the predictions we wanted to ask everything to the teacher and when we realized that we had a certain number of questions available we said ‘Ok, let’s try on our own first and save them for later’”.
    -
    “Maybe fewer tickets, because we don’t really use it all”.
    -
    “They help to make people work harder”.
    -
    “We didn’t use them at all”.
  • Was there any exercise that surprised you with its result?
    Students agreed on number 7 (on Newton’s Third Law).
    -
    “We thought that there would be a difference on the force, but they’re the same”.
    -
    “We were expecting a difference between the forces”.
  • Did you feel you were discovering something about the subject and the concepts when answering the activity?
    -
    “Yes, because I didn’t know anything about electricity”.
    -
    “Yes, because when we made a prediction, and then we realized that it was ok, it was like -wow-”.
    -
    “Once you discover the relationship between charges, distances and forces, everything begins to fall into place; everything starts to make sense”.
  • From the items of the guided experiment, was there any result you didn’t expect?
    -
    “This one (pointing to the item corresponding to the two charges of different magnitudes, asking which one would have a higher force on the other one). We expected that the big charge would push harder than the small one”.
Newton’s Third Law seems not to be intuitive for students. In the concentration plots, we also observed that item 5 corresponding to this matter reported significantly higher increases in the experimental group than the control group. Other studies have also stated higher efficiency in illustrating Newton’s Third Law by using computer simulation [64].

4. Conclusions

We observe that the normalized learning changes indicate that the implemented assisted-discovery is significantly more effective than the regular instruction, being c = 0.469 and c = 0.435 for the the lecturer 1 and 2, and c = 0.08 and c = 0.077 for the control groups. This method shows to be advantageous when there are gaps in students’ background knowledge and when the course’s learning objectives are ambitious. If students are operating in their proximal development zone, discovery-based learning with limited intervention from the instructors helps them build their own knowledge and feel empowered.
Interactive learning using web-based simulations reduces stress in students because they feel free to explore and predict without being afraid of the consequences of not knowing the correct answer beforehand. Nevertheless, students also feel that it is appropriate to complement an active learning session with a regular instruction session to consolidate the concepts.
We observed that when the student population has very heterogeneous background knowledge, they have trouble reaching agreement. In this aspect, both the virtual experiment and the limited intervention from the instructors helped them reach a consensus. It would also be a good starting place to explore future directions: this methodology is designed for when the students’ background is very heterogeneous, and a good part of the students are afraid to participate and have little experience with active learning. Also, because the lecturer has to provide assistance, this method is limited to small class sizes (≲36). However, this method is particularly appropriate for novice students and does not rely on students with previous backgrounds or preparation. It helps to involve novice students since they feel less stressed for not knowing the concept beforehand. Because of that, we think it may be useful to other educational systems or schools with heterogeneous academic background.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, methodology, investigation, resources, writing—original draft preparation, writing—review and editing, visualization, O.A.G., R.K.G., A.S., H.M.-H. and H.G. Software and data curation O.A.G., A.S. and H.M.-H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
RQResearch question
CGControl group
EGExperimental group
CSEMConceptual Survey of Electricity and Magnetism
S-CScore-Concentration
L, M, HLow, medium, and high levels

Appendix A. The Pilot Survey

Lecturers conducted a pilot survey to 61 students to analyze their electric force concept’s background. It consisted of two direct questions: (A) What major did you study in high school?, and (B) What topics do you remember from electromagnetism? Results showed that 10% mentioned remembering only Coulomb’s Law, 13% circuits, and 20% slightly Coulomb’s Law and circuits; the rest (57%) commented not remembering any electricity topic. Furthermore, the quiz shows that the groups are heterogeneous; only 45% of the total came from a concentration of engineering; the others from areas such as business, health, programming, and business management. Thus, the student groups were very heterogeneous. Therefore, because of the heterogeneity of the student groups and the gap in electric force knowledge, we decided to take as a starting point in this work that there was no prior knowledge of such a concept.
Table A1. Pilot survey results. Percentage of students per program.
Table A1. Pilot survey results. Percentage of students per program.
Answers (A)Percentage (%)
Engineering44.3
Architecture and Engineering1.6
Humanities and Social Sciences8.2
Business/ Business management/BI course14.8
Programming1.6
Health care11.5
Chemical laboratory technician1.6
No specialization/Does not apply16.4
Table A2. Pilot survey results. Percentage of students who claim that they remember the topic.
Table A2. Pilot survey results. Percentage of students who claim that they remember the topic.
Answers (B)Percentage (%)
Coulomb’s Law8
Circuits13
Coulomb’s Law and Circuits20
Coulomb’s Law, but didn’t take any electricity course2
I do not remember (and analogous answers)57

References

  1. Fraser, J.M.; Timan, A.L.; Miller, K.; Dowd, J.E.; Tucker, L.; Mazur, E. Teaching and physics education research: Bridging the gap. Rep. Prog. Phys. 2014, 77, 032401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Bozzi, M.; Raffaghelli, J.E.; Zani, M. Peer Learning as a Key Component of an Integrated Teaching Method: Overcoming the Complexities of Physics Teaching in Large Size Classes. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Miller, K.; Callaghan, K.; McCarty, L.S.; Deslauriers, L. Increasing the effectiveness of active learning using deliberate practice: A homework transformation. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2021, 17, 010129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Deslauriers, L.; McCarty, L.S.; Miller, K.; Callaghan, K.; Kestin, G. Measuring actual learning versus feeling of learning in response to being actively engaged in the classroom. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 19251–19257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Fazio, C. Active learning methods and strategies to improve student conceptual understanding: Some considerations from physics education research. In Research and Innovation in Physics Education: Two Sides of the Same Coin; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 15–35. [Google Scholar]
  6. Hartikainen, S.; Rintala, H.; Pylväs, L.; Nokelainen, P. The concept of active learning and the measurement of learning outcomes: A review of research in engineering higher education. Educ. Sci. 2019, 9, 276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Nuñez, R.P.; Hernández, C.A.; Gamboa, A.A. Active learning and knowledge in physics: A reading from classroom work. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 2021, 1981, 012007. [Google Scholar]
  8. Sokoloff, D.R.; Thornton, R.K. Using Interactive Lecture Demonstrations to Create an Active Learning Environment. Phys. Teach. 1997, 35, 340–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Thornton, R.K. Effective Learning Environments for Computer Supported Instruction in the Physics Classroom and Laboratory. 2008. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/255619349_EFFECTIVE_LEARNING_ENVIRONMENTS_FOR_COMPUTER_SUPPORTED_INSTRUCTION_IN_THE_PHYSICS_CLASSROOM_AND_LABORATORY (accessed on 1 April 2022).
  10. Thornton, R.K.; Sokoloff, D.R. Assessing Student Learning of Newton’s Laws: The Force and Motion Conceptual Evaluation and Evaluation of Active Learning Laboratory and Lecture Curricula. Am. J. Phys. 1998, 66, 338–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Palmer, D. The POE in the primary school: An evaluation. Res. Sci. Educ. 1995, 25, 323–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Meltzer, L.; Thornton, R.K. Resource Letter ALIP-1: Active-Learning Instruction in Physics. Am. J. Phys. 2012, 80, 478–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Redish, E.F.; Steinberg, R. Teaching physics: Figuring out what works. Phys. Today 1999, 52, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Shieh, R.S. The impact of Technology-Enabled Active Learning (TEAL) implementation on student learning and teachers’ teaching in a high school context. Comput. Educ. 2012, 59, 206–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Pressley, M.; Wood, E.; Woloshyn, V.E.; Martin, V.; King, A.; Menke, D. Encouraging Mindful Use of Prior Knowledge: Attempting to Construct Explanatory Answers Facilitates Learning. Educ. Psychol. 1992, 27, 91–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Reif, F.; Larkin, J.H. Cognition in Scientific and Everyday Domains: Comparison and Learning Implications. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1991, 28, 733–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Crouch, C.H.; Mazur, E. Peer instruction: Ten years of experience and results. Am. J. Phys. 2001, 69, 970–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Laws, P.W. Millikan Lecture 1996: Promoting active learning based on Physics Education Research in introductory physics courses. Am. J. Phys. 1997, 65, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Beichner, R. The SCALE-UP Project: A Student-Centered Active Learning Environment for Undergraduate Programs. An Invited White Paper for the National Academy of Sciences. 2008. Available online: https://sites.nationalacademies.org/cs/groups/dbassesite/documents/webpage/dbasse_072628.pdf (accessed on 1 April 2022).
  20. Keiner, L.E.; Burns, T.E. Interactive Engagement: How Much is Enough? Phys. Teach. 2010, 48, 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. McDermott, L.C. Millikan Lecture 1990: What we teach and what is learned closing the gap. Am. J. Phys. 1991, 59, 301–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Alfieri, L.; Brooks, P.J.; Aldrich, N.J.; Tenenbaum, H.R. Does discovery based instruction enhance learning? J. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 103, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Pieaget, J. Psychology and Epistemology: Towards a Theory of Knowledge; Viking: New York, NY, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  24. Beed, P.; Hawkins, M.; Roller, C. Moving learners towards independence: The power of scaffolded instruction. Read. Teach. 1991, 44, 648–655. [Google Scholar]
  25. Vygotsky, L.S. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  26. Brunner, J.S. The act of discovery. Harv. Educ. Rev. 1961, 31, 31–32. [Google Scholar]
  27. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Hovdal, D.O.G.; Larsen, I.B.; Haugen, T.; Johansen, B.T. Understanding disruptive situations in physical education: Teaching style and didactic implications. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 2021, 27, 455–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hollis, R.B.; Was, C.A. Mind wandering, control failures, and social media distractions in online learning. Learn. Instr. 2016, 42, 104–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Awwad, F.; Ayesh, A.; Awwad, S. Are Laptops Distracting Educational Tools in Classrooms. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2013, 103, 154–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Ophir, E.; Nass, C.; Wagner, A.D. Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 15583–15587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Quintana, C.; Reiser, B.J.; Davis, E.A.; Krajcik, J.; Fretz, E.; GolanDuncan, R.; Kyza, E.; Edelson, D.; Soloway, E. A scaffolding design framework forsoftware to support science inquiry. J. Learn. Sci. 2004, 13, 337–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kirschner, P.A.; Bruyckere, P.D. The myths of the digital native and the multitasker. Teach. Teach. Educ. 2017, 67, 135–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Edelson, D.C.; Gordin, D.N.; Pea, R.D. Addressing the Challenges of Inquiry-Based Learning Through Technology and Curriculum Design. J. Learn. Sci. 1999, 8, 391–450. [Google Scholar]
  35. DeVore, S.; Marshman, E.; Singh, C. Challenge of engaging all students via self-paced interactive electronic learning tutorials for introductory physics. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2017, 13, 010127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Bransford, J.D.; Brown, A.L.; Cocking, R.R. How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School: Expanded Edition; The National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. DiSessa, A.A. Toward an Epistemology of Physics. Cogn. Instr. 1993, 10, 105–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hammer, D. More than misconceptions: Multiple perspectives on student knowledge and reasoning, and an appropriate role for education research. Am. J. Phys. 1996, 64, 1316–1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Hammer, D. Student resources for learning introductory physics. Am. J. Phys. 2000, 68, S52–S59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Redish, E.F. A Theoretical Framework for Physics Education Research: Modeling Student Thinking. In Proceedings of the Lectures Given at the International School of Physics “Enrico Fermi” Course CLVI, Varenna, Italy, 15–25 July 2003. [Google Scholar]
  41. Programa para la Evaluación Internacional de Alumnos (PISA) Resultados. Available online: https://www.oecd.org/pisa/publications/PISA2018_CN_MEX_Spanish.pdf (accessed on 1 April 2022).
  42. Chong-Muñoz, M.A.; Castañeda-Castro, R. Sistema educativo en México: El modelo de competencias, de la industria a la educación. Sincronía 2013, 63, 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  43. del Rey, A.; Sanchez-Parga, J. Crítica de la educación por competencias. Univ. Rev. Cienc. Soc. Hum. 2011, 15, 233–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Newtondreams Simulaciones Interactivas Multiplataforma. Universidad de Monterrey, Departamento de Física y Matemáticas. 2019. Available online: https://www.newtondreams.com/ (accessed on 1 April 2022.).
  45. Jonassen, D.H.; Kommers, P.A.M.; Mayes, J.T. Cognitive Tools for Learning; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  46. Adams, W.K.; Reid, S.; Lemaster, R.; Mckagan, S.B.; Perkins, K.K.; Dubson, M.; Wieman, C.E. A Study of Educational Simulations Part 1—Engagement and Learning. J. Interact. Learn. Res. 2008, 19, 397–419. [Google Scholar]
  47. Moore, E.B.; Herzog, T.A.; Perkins, K.K. Interactive simulations as implicit support for guided-inquiry. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2013, 14, 257–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Finkelstein, N.; Adams, W.; Keller, C.; Kohl, P.; Perkins, K.K.; Podolefsky, N.; Reid, S.; LeMaster, R. When learning about the real world is better done virtually: A study of substituting computer simulations for laboratory equipment. Phys. Rev. Spec. Top.-Phys. Educ. Res. 2005, 1, 010103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Jonassen, D.H.; Rohrer-Murphy, L. Activity theory as a framework for designing constructivist learning environment. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 1999, 47, 61–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Punithavathi, P.; Geetha, S. Disruptive smart mobile pedagogies for engineering education. Procedia Comput. Sci. 2020, 172, 784–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Christian, W.; Esquembre, F. Modeling physics with easy java simulations. Phys. Teach. 2007, 45, 475–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Haertel, H. COLOS: Conceptual Learning of Science. In Design and Production of Multimedia and Simulation-Based Learning Material; de Jong, T., Sarti, L., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  53. PhET Interactive Simulations. University of Colorado at Boulder. 2019. Available online: http://phet.colorado.edu (accessed on 1 April 2022).
  54. Serway, R.A.; Jewett, J.W. Physics for Scientists and Engineers, 9th ed.; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  55. Maloney, D.P.; O’Kuma, T.L.; Hieggelke, T.L.; Heuvelen, C.J. Surveying students’ conceptual knowledge of electricity and magnetism. Am. J. Phys. 2001, 69, S12–S23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Hake, R. Interactive-engagement versus traditional methods: A six-thousand-student survey of mechanics test data for introductory physics courses. Am. J. Phys. 1998, 66, 64–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Coletta, V.P.; Steinert, J.J. Why normalized gain should continue to be used in analyzing preinstruction and postinstruction scores on concept inventories. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2020, 16, 010108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Bao, L. Theoretical comparisons of average normalized gain calculations. Am. J. Phys. 2006, 74, 917–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kreutzer, K.; Boudreaux, A. Preliminary investigation of instructor effects on gender gap in introductory physics. Phys. Rev. Spec. Top. Phys. Educ. Res. 2012, 8, 010120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Nissen, J.M.; Talbot, R.M.; Thompson, A.N.; Dusen, B.V. Comparison of normalized gain and Cohen’s d for analyzing gains on concept inventories. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2018, 14, 010115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Marx, J.; Cummins, K. Normalized Change. Am. J. Phys. 2007, 75, 87–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 April 2022).
  63. Bao, L.; Redish, E. Concentration analysis: A quantitative assessment of student states. Am. J. Phys. 2001, 69, S45–S53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Sornkhatha, P.; Srisawasdi, N. Supporting Conceptual Development in Newton’s Laws of Motion Using an Interactive Computer-simulated Laboratory Environment. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2013, 93, 2010–2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. The Newtondream simulation of two charges interacting to illustrate the Electric Force [44].
Figure 1. The Newtondream simulation of two charges interacting to illustrate the Electric Force [44].
Education 12 00269 g001
Figure 3. Student sample responses in the active sequence. Students are guided to recognize the law of the inverse square. Question (16), Set Q1 = Q2 = 2 μ C, fill the following table of F12 as a function of r12. Then plot the table using MS Excel and adjust a function to the points and observe which expression fits the points better. Attach the plot to a word document. Question (17), Which of the following formulas best describes the behavior between the force and the charge?
Figure 3. Student sample responses in the active sequence. Students are guided to recognize the law of the inverse square. Question (16), Set Q1 = Q2 = 2 μ C, fill the following table of F12 as a function of r12. Then plot the table using MS Excel and adjust a function to the points and observe which expression fits the points better. Attach the plot to a word document. Question (17), Which of the following formulas best describes the behavior between the force and the charge?
Education 12 00269 g003
Figure 4. Observed normalized learning change quartiles. From left to right, (brown) the control groups GC1 and GC2, (cyan) the experimental groups GE1 and GE2. The solid lines stands for the medians of each group and dots are outliers.
Figure 4. Observed normalized learning change quartiles. From left to right, (brown) the control groups GC1 and GC2, (cyan) the experimental groups GE1 and GE2. The solid lines stands for the medians of each group and dots are outliers.
Education 12 00269 g004
Figure 5. Observed concentration factors (C) and test score (S) before (blue triangles) and after the lecture (red circles) for the experimental group. Δ S and Δ C are represented through the blue arrows per item.
Figure 5. Observed concentration factors (C) and test score (S) before (blue triangles) and after the lecture (red circles) for the experimental group. Δ S and Δ C are represented through the blue arrows per item.
Education 12 00269 g005
Figure 6. Observed concentration factors (C) and test score (S) before (blue triangles) and after the lecture (red circles) for the control group. Δ S and Δ C are represented through the blue arrows per item.
Figure 6. Observed concentration factors (C) and test score (S) before (blue triangles) and after the lecture (red circles) for the control group. Δ S and Δ C are represented through the blue arrows per item.
Education 12 00269 g006
Table 1. Description of control and experimental groups. Two lecturers participated in the experiment, and each one led a control and an experimental group.
Table 1. Description of control and experimental groups. Two lecturers participated in the experiment, and each one led a control and an experimental group.
Group TypeLecturerNumber of StudentsLabel
Control132CG1
Control235CG2
Experimental137EG1
Experimental236EG2
Table 2. Relation between CSEM questions and concepts.
Table 2. Relation between CSEM questions and concepts.
Item Number in
This Work
CSEM QuestionConcept
13Force-Charge
243rd Law
35Force-Distance
46Superposition
573rd Law
68Superposition
79Superposition
Table 3. Descriptive statistics on each normalized changes on each group.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics on each normalized changes on each group.
TagnMedianIqrMeanSd
CG1320.1830.3330.080.409
CG23500.310.0770.393
EG1370.50.550.4690.357
EG2360.50.60.4350.489
Table 4. The Wilcoxon Rank-Sum Test. Where (CG) stands for Control Group, (E) for Experimental Group, and (1, 2) is the Lecturer Id.
Table 4. The Wilcoxon Rank-Sum Test. Where (CG) stands for Control Group, (E) for Experimental Group, and (1, 2) is the Lecturer Id.
Group vs. Groupp-ValueEffsizeSignificant
CG1 vs. CG20.765-No
CG1 vs. EG10.0001820.451 (moderate)Yes
CG1 vs. EG20.0002910.440 (moderate)Yes
CG2 vs. EG10.00006050.473 (moderate)Yes
CG2 vs. EG20.0001320.454 (moderate)Yes
EC1 vs. EG20.838-No
Table 5. Displacements on score and concentration per item.
Table 5. Displacements on score and concentration per item.
ItemConceptControl Experiment
Δ S Δ C Δ S Δ C
1Force-Charge+0.15+0.17+0.23+0.15
23rd Law−0.06−0.06+0.25+0.24
3Force-Distance+0.12−0.13+0.62+0.19
4Superposition+0.21+0.12+0.22+0.22
53rd Law+0.09+0.02+0.64+0.34
6Superposition+0.01+0.08+0.08+0.10
7Superposition+0.17+0.06+0.22+0.18
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Aquines Gutiérrez, O.; Galloway, R.K.; Santos, A.; Martínez-Huerta, H.; González, H. Assisted Discovery Based Learning of the Electric Force with Scaffolding for Novice Students. Educ. Sci. 2022, 12, 269. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/educsci12040269

AMA Style

Aquines Gutiérrez O, Galloway RK, Santos A, Martínez-Huerta H, González H. Assisted Discovery Based Learning of the Electric Force with Scaffolding for Novice Students. Education Sciences. 2022; 12(4):269. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/educsci12040269

Chicago/Turabian Style

Aquines Gutiérrez, Osvaldo, Ross K. Galloway, Ayax Santos, Humberto Martínez-Huerta, and Héctor González. 2022. "Assisted Discovery Based Learning of the Electric Force with Scaffolding for Novice Students" Education Sciences 12, no. 4: 269. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/educsci12040269

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop