Next Article in Journal
The Role of Alpha Oscillations among the Main Neuropsychiatric Disorders in the Adult and Developing Human Brain: Evidence from the Last 10 Years of Research
Next Article in Special Issue
Compromised Myelin and Axonal Molecular Organization Following Adult-Onset Sulfatide Depletion
Previous Article in Journal
Prospective Analysis of Functional and Structural Changes in Patients with Spinal Muscular Atrophy—A Pilot Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
The TRACK-MS Test Battery: A Very Brief Tool to Track Multiple Sclerosis-Related Cognitive Impairment
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Neural Signatures of Error Processing in Depressed Adolescents with Comorbid Non-Suicidal Self-Injury (NSSI)

1
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy III, University Hospital Ulm, 89075 Ulm, Germany
2
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Ulm, 89075 Ulm, Germany
3
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Medical University of Vienna, 1090 Vienna, Austria
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Submission received: 24 October 2022 / Revised: 24 November 2022 / Accepted: 6 December 2022 / Published: 8 December 2022

Abstract

:
Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI), as a highly prevalent psychiatric symptom in adolescents and young adults, is defined as the deliberate destruction of body tissue without suicidal intent. Impulsivity and dysfunctional response inhibition have been suggested to play a central role in adolescents’ vulnerability to self-harm. To investigate the potentially distinct neurobiology of NSSI, we used a well-established Go/No Go task in which activation of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) is interpreted as a neural correlate of processing failed response inhibition. Task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging data were obtained from 14 adolescents with a diagnosis of major depression and a history of NSSI (MD-NSSI), 13 depressed adolescents without NSSI (MD-only), and 14 healthy controls (HC). In line with hypotheses of dysfunctional response inhibition, we observed increased rates of commission errors in MD-NSSI along with significantly reduced error-related activations of the dACC and IFG. Intact response inhibition, as reflected by low commission error rates not different from HC, was observed in MD-only, along with increased activation of the error-processing network. Our findings support the hypothesis of a distinct neurobiological signature of NSSI. Further research on biomarkers of NSSI could focus on behavioral and neural correlates of failed response inhibition.

1. Introduction

Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) is defined as the deliberate destruction of one’s own body tissue without suicidal intent [1]. The most common methods are cutting, scratching, hitting or banging, carving, burning and scraping [2]. NSSI is particularly prevalent in mid-adolescence [3], with an estimated 23% lifetime prevalence among adolescents in community samples [4]. In adolescent psychiatric samples, prevalence rates of NSSI have found to be 50–60% for one incident or repetitive NSSI [5]. NSSI is observed by itself, but is seen frequently comorbid to further psychopathology and in the broad context of psychiatric disorders, including mood disorders [6,7,8]. NSSI has been categorized as impulsive behavior. Impulsivity and self-harm are also core diagnostic criterions of borderline personality disorder. However, in earlier adolescence, it has been shown that, in particular, mood disorders and NSSI often co-exist [9,10]. While NSSI is seen as an important predictor for the development of borderline personality disorder in early adulthood, only a minority of adolescent patients with self-injurious behavior fulfil the criteria for the diagnosis around the onset of NSSI behavior [11].
NSSI serves the primary function of diminishing negative feelings or thoughts in the majority of cases [12]. After engaging in NSSI, relief from these, and even pleasant feelings or thoughts, has been reported [8]. Low planning ability and mood-based impulsivity-related traits have been consistently associated with an increased risk of developing NSSI [13]. Thus, impulsivity might play a central role in adolescents’ vulnerability to self-harm [14] and has been identified as a risk factor [15,16]. Using several assessment methods, Fikke et al. [17] were able to show impaired inhibitory control in a subgroup of adolescents with NSSI of low frequency. Individual differences in impulsivity have been suggested to also relate to individual differences in cognitive control [18], which in turn has been linked to inhibition as one of its essential components [19]. Response inhibition is the cognitive process required to cancel an intended action and is a widely studied operationalization of inhibition [20].
Response inhibition and its neurobiology can be tested using the well-established combined Go/No Go Eriksen flanker paradigm [21]. Here, subjects are instructed to perform speeded responses on Go trials (e.g., pressing a button in response to the letters P, V), while not responding on No Go trials (e.g., to the letter R, U). The established index of inhibitory control measured hereby is the number of errors that a subject commits on No Go trials. A higher number of commission errors can be interpreted to relate to lower inhibitory control and is suggestive of greater levels of impulsivity. Besides the behavioral findings, the task has been successfully implemented in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) settings and thus permits investigation of the underlying neurobiology [22,23,24,25,26].
To date, several neuroimaging studies suggest that unsuccessful response inhibition indexed by commission errors in No Go trials activates prefrontal cortex regions, the supplementary motor area (SMA, e.g., [24,25]), and especially and consistently the right inferior frontal gyrus [19,27,28,29]. Functional and structural MRI [30,31,32,33] and EEG data [34] brought evidence that damage in this region leads to permanent impairments in subjects’ response inhibition.
Intending to shed light on the neurobiology of NSSI, we hypothesized that adolescents with NSSI would show altered neural reactivity of prefrontal cortex regions during a task examining response inhibition. In particular, the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex should play a significant role. To control for the effects of comorbid psychopathology, adolescents with at least five episodes of NSSI during the past year and a diagnosis of major depression were compared to depressed adolescents without a recent history of NSSI. We used the modified Erikson flanker Go/No Go paradigm without incentive manipulations, which has been shown to reliably elicit neural responses in brain areas processing failed response inhibition [22,23,24,25,26]. Based on previous electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies [24,35,36,37,38], we expected a blunted neural response in brain regions previously related to response inhibition and error processing in patients with comorbid NSSI. By contrast, depressed adolescents without NSSI were expected to show the previously reported [24] unimpaired activation of the error-processing network.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Twenty-eight adolescents fulfilling the diagnostic criteria for major depression (MD) were recruited for this study. Of these, 14 met the criteria for comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (‘MD-NSSI’ group; 11 female, 3 male). According to Section 3 of the DSM-5 [1], NSSI in this group was reported at least five times within the past year (mean number of NSSI events within the past year: 93.3, SD = 225.9). By contrast, the other 14 adolescents showed no NSSI events within the last 12 months (‘MD-only’ group; 11 female, 3 male) great enough to fulfil diagnostic criteria. Five participants of the MD-only group reported minor lifetime NSSI (two participants once, one participant four times, and two participants seven times, all events at least one year before the onset of the study). Additional symptom-based diagnoses according to DSM-IV at the time of the study were assessed using the German version of the semi-structured interview Kiddie-SADS-PL (Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children Present and Lifetime [39]), and revealed in both groups (MD-only/MD-NSSI) anxiety disorder (n = 2/n = 1), posttraumatic stress disorder (n = 4/n = 0), eating disorders (n = 2/n = 2), borderline personality disorder (n = 0/n = 1), and ADHD (n = 1/n = 2). Recruitment took place in the inpatient and outpatient units of the University Hospital for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy Ulm (Germany) and in a private medical practice for child and adolescent psychiatry in Ulm. Fifteen healthy adolescent controls (‘HC’ group; 12 female, 3 male) with no clinical diagnoses and no current or past psychiatric disorders were recruited as a control group. Dropouts included one female participant in the MD-only group and one female participant in the HC group, who both interrupted the fMRI procedure. Of all 41 participants, two were left-handed (HC: n = 0; MD-only: n = 1; MD-NSSI: n = 1). Two participants (MD-only: n = 1; HC: n = 1) reported regular cigarette smoking, which was forbidden at least two hours before fMRI scanning. None of the participants reported excessive use of alcohol, as assessed by the corresponding section of the Kiddie-SADS-PL. Other substance use disorders, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, current medical illnesses, or a history of epilepsy were exclusion criteria. All participants had reached puberty. All female participants were scanned between day one and ten of their menstrual cycles or after at least 14 days of continuous intake of oral contraception.
Both patient groups (MD-only and MD-NSSI) were matched for age, gender, IQ, and depression scores (see Table 1). Psychotropic medication was kept stable within patient groups for at least two weeks before fMRI scanning to ensure steady-state conditions and included SSRIs (n = 7), mirtazapine (n = 2), and methylphenidate (n = 1). Healthy controls were average-matched to patient groups regarding age, gender, and IQ. Nevertheless, the mean age of the HC group was one year less than that of the patients’ average. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Ulm University, Germany (EA 58/2012. Written informed consent was obtained from legal guardians and participants. All procedures were performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Psychometric Measurements

Past and current non-suicidal self-injurious behavior was assessed by the validated [40] semi-structured Self-Injurious Thoughts and Behaviors Interview (SITBI [41], German version by [40]). Present depressive symptoms were measured with the German version of the Beck Depression Inventory, second edition ([42]; German version by [43]), and the semi-structured interview Children’s Depression Rating Scale—Revised ([44]; German version by [45]). Intelligence quotients (IQs) were either estimated by the Prüfsystem für Schul und Bildungsberatung für 6.-13. Klassen (PSB) [46] or by using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV; Wechsler [47]). Parasuicidal behaviors were assessed with the semi-structured Self-Injurious Thoughts and Behaviors Interview—German (SITBI-G; [40]).

2.3. fMRI Procedure

The data presented here were collected as part of a larger experimental setup where participants first underwent a Cyberball task [48] and unpleasant electrical stimulation. Findings have already been reported [49].
To examine error processing during fMRI, a combined Go/No Go Eriksen flanker paradigm [21] was performed. The task has been demonstrated to be suitable to examine error monitoring and response inhibition in various electrophysiological and functional MRI studies [22,23,24,25,26]. As reported previously [23,24,25], five-letter strings with letters R, U, P, and V were shown. The action-relevant target was always mid-standing. During Go trials, subjects had to respond to the target letter R with their right index finger on a two-button box and to the target letter U with their right middle finger. In No Go trials, subjects were instructed to withhold their response whenever the letters P or V appeared. Combinations of target and flanker stimuli were either congruent (all five letters the same) or incongruent. In incongruent Go trials, targets flanked by visually similar No Go targets were shown (e.g., VVUVV). In incongruent No Go trials, visually similar Go targets flanked the central No Go target (e.g., UUVUU) (see Figure 1). After stimulus presentation, a response time window depending on individual reaction times was established. Feedback (German expressions for either correct of wrong) followed the response. Upon delayed responses, the feedback “faster” was displayed. To secure enough incongruent No Go errors, participants were instructed to emphasize speed over accuracy in Go trials. Incorrect button pushes and overly slow responses were recorded as separate types of errors. Overly slow responses were not further evaluated. Individual reaction time windows were determined during a training session before scanning. For each combination of the factors condition (Go, No Go) and type (congruent, incongruent), 66 trials were prepared (264 trials in total). The duration of one trial was 1.9 s, the mean inter-trial interval 3.01 s. The average stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) was 19.5 s for events of the same combination of condition by type (e.g., incongruent No Go). The entire fMRI task lasted around 22 min. A standard personal computer was used to register reaction times and the correctness of the subjects’ responses per trial.

2.4. Functional Data Acquisition

fMRI data were acquired using a 3.0 T MAGNETOM Allegra Scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). A T2*-sensitive gradient echo sequence was used for functional imaging, with an echo time (TE) of 33 ms, a flip angle of 90°, a field of view (FOV) of 230 mm, and a slice thickness of 2.5 mm, with an interslice gap of 0.5 mm. At a repetition time (TR) of 2000 ms, 35 transversal slices were recorded with an image size of 64 × 64 pixels during the Go/No Go task. Anatomical high-resolution T1-weighted images (1 × 1 × 1 mm voxels) were obtained (bandwidth (BW) = 130 Hz/Pixel, TR = 2500 ms, TI = 1.1 s, echo time (TE) = 4.57 ms, flip angle 12°) to co-register and normalize data into a standardized stereotactic space.

2.5. Data Analysis

2.5.1. Behavioral Data and Psychometric Measurements

Statistical analyses were calculated with JASP [50]. Excel (Microsoft Corporation (2022)) was used to create the figures presented. Between-group differences in non-fMRI data were assessed with one-factorial ANOVAs. Post-hoc analyses were Bonferroni-corrected. The significance level was set to p ≤ 0.05. One participant in the MD-only group did not commit any errors of commission and could therefore not be included in the fMRI analyses on the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent No Go trials’.

2.5.2. fMRI-Data

We used Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12, Wellcome Department, London, UK) under MATLAB R2020a (Math-Works, Version 9.8.0.1323502, Natrick, MA [Computer Software]) to pre-process imaging data from each session, including realignment, slice timing, and normalization into the standard MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) template, with a spatial resolution of 2 × 2 × 2 mm3. Spatial smoothing was performed using an 8 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. Low-frequency drifts were removed via high-pass filtering. Intrinsic autocorrelations were accounted for by an AR (1) model. On the first level, we modeled individual event types as trains of delta functions at stimulus onset and convolved them with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Regressors for individual fixed-effects analyses were obtained by modeling individual events for the combinations of factors condition (Go, No Go), type (congruent, incongruent), and within-time responses (correct, incorrect). This resulted in eight conditions of interest. Out-of-time responses and missed Go events were also modeled, but as conditions of no interest. The six realignment parameters were added to the design matrix. Of note, congruent Go and No Go trials did not yield errors consistently across subjects. Thus, the ‘congruent’ conditions, though modeled, were not included in second-level analyses.
For second-level group analyses, we computed a 3 × 4 ANOVA model with factors ‘group’ (HC, MD-only, MD-NSSI) and ‘condition’ (Go/incongruent/correct, Go/incongruent/incorrect; No Go/incongruent/correct, No Go/incongruent/incorrect). In line with previous analyses [23,24,25], hypothesis-driven group differences were calculated for the contrast of incorrect minus correct incongruent No Go trials. To exclude the effects of psychostimulant MPH medication, we furthermore recalculated the group comparisons after excluding the respective participant (n = 1). Results were considered significant at a threshold of p ≤ 0.005 at the voxel level, and p ≤ 0.05 at the cluster level, with a minimum of 183 contiguously significant voxels.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic Data and Psychometric Measurements

Depression scores in both patient groups were significantly higher relative to HC. An ANOVA for age revealed that HC were, on average, one year of life younger than the patient groups. However, this difference was significant only when HC subjects were compared to the MD-only group. According to the SITBI-G [40], one patient in the MD-only group and four patients in the MD-NSSI group reported suicide attempts, with a total number of one in MD-only and a mean number of 3.3 in MD-NSSI (SD = 3.9), respectively. Suicide plans were reported by the same patient in the MD-only group on one occasion within the past year. In the MD-NSSI group, six patients reported suicide plans at a mean rate of 3.3 (SD = 2.8) episodes within the last year. None of the participants in the HC group reported suicide attempts or plans. Suicidal thoughts were reported by participants from each group (HC: n = 2; MD-only: n = 12; MD-NSSI: n = 13) at mean rates from one episode within the past year (HC group), over 2.8 (SD = 3.0; MD-only), to 13.8 (SD = 31.1; MD-NSSI) episodes in the past year. The number of errors in Go trials did not differ significantly between groups. However, for No Go trials, a significant effect of group was found. Post-hoc tests confirmed a significantly greater number of commission errors in MD-NSSI compared to HC, while, in MD-only, the occurrence of commission errors was not significantly different from that in HC. Furthermore, reaction times showed a significant effect of group for incongruent Go trials. For both correct and incorrect Go trials, MD-only and MD-NSSI showed faster reaction times relative to HC. Table 1 summarizes demographical data and psychometric measurements. Table 2 summarizes task performance data obtained from the fMRI experiment.

3.2. fMRI Data

We first analyzed differential neural activations corresponding to errors (incorrect minus correct) in incongruent No Go trials separately for each group (HC, MD-only, MD-NSSI). In line with previous investigations [23,24,25], the following brain regions showed reliable differential neural activations for this contrast in each group: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), supplementary motor area (SMA), bilateral anterior insula, and bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). More details are provided in the Supplementary Materials (see Table S1).

3.2.1. Incorrect Minus Correct Incongruent No Go Trials

Investigating the effect of NSSI on neural error processing (incorrect minus correct incongruent No Go trials) by comparing the two patient groups revealed lower differential neural activations of dACC, left IFG (pars triangularis and pars opercularis), left supramarginal gyrus, and of the left inferior parietal cortex in MD-NSSI relative to MD-only (see Table 3 and Figure 2A,B). We found no significantly higher differential neural activations in MD-NSSI compared to MD-only.
Comparing error-related neural activation between each patient group and HC showed relatively increased left-lateralized differential activation in the MD-only group: left anterior insula, left postcentral gyrus, left inferior parietal cortex, left precuneus, and left cerebellum. Inverting the contrast (HC > MD-only group) did not yield any significant results. MD-NSSI patients showed reduced differential activation of the right middle frontal gyrus, right IFG (pars triangularis and pars opercularis), right supramarginal gyrus, and left inferior parietal cortex relative to HC. Significantly higher differential neural activation in MD-NSSI compared to HC was found for the left precuneus (see Table 3).
To confirm that our findings were not driven by confounding motor activity (button press upon incorrect No Go trial), we calculated the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent Go trials’ with a motor component present in each condition as a conjunction across all groups (HC, MD-only, MD-NSSI). This statistical map was then used as an inclusive mask thresholded at p ≤ 0.05, uncorrected for the re-calculation of the above between-group comparisons for failed response inhibition. All previous clusters could be confirmed at a minimum cluster size of 135 voxels. After excluding the participant receiving psychostimulant medication, all significant clusters from previous group comparisons were confirmed at a minimum cluster size of 95 voxels. Thus, both control calculations yielded similar results, with only smaller cluster sizes compared to the original analyses.
In summary, we revealed neural activations of brain regions reliably linked to neural error processing. MD-NSSI patients showed reduced neural error processing in both hemispheres relative to MD-only patients and healthy controls. The MD-only group showed enhanced neural error signaling when compared against the group of MD-NSSI and HC.

3.2.2. Incorrect Minus Correct Incongruent Go Trials

We found robust activation in the error-processing network comprising dACC and bilateral IFG in each of the three groups for the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent Go trials’ (see Table S2).
Investigating differences between the two patient groups in error-related brain activation for Go trials, we observed significantly lower neural activations of right and left IFG for the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent Go trials’ for MD-NSSI. This was similar to the effects in No Go trials. Furthermore, differential activation of SMA was significantly greater for MD-NSSI compared to MD-only. Relatively increased differential IFG activation in MD-only was observed when compared against HC (see Table 4).
Exploratory analyses investigating relations between imaging findings and behavioral data from the Go/No Go task during fMRI in the patients revealed opposite correlations with error rates for MD-only versus MD-NSSI, particularly in the dACC. Data are presented in the Supplementary Materials (see Figure S1).

4. Discussion

We investigated two clinical groups of adolescent MD patients with and without comorbid NSSI, and HC, using an established Erikson flanker Go/No Go paradigm during fMRI to examine the neural signaling of failed response inhibition. By implementing these two clinical samples in one study design, our investigation enabled us to disentangle specific neural patterns in brain regions relevant for failed response inhibition associated with NSSI. In contrast to the significantly enhanced reactivity in neural networks in all MD patients, we observed relatively blunted neural activation of these regions in the MD patients with comorbid NSSI. This might represent a correlate of impaired inhibitory control in the MD patients with NSSI, while neural reactivity to error processing in depressed adolescents without NSSI (MD-only) was increased not only compared to the other clinical group but also compared to healthy controls. Impaired inhibitory control (also often assessed by Go/No Go tasks) has also been reported from a systematic review and meta-analysis of adolescents and young adults with self-harm [51], which is in line with the findings presented in this study.
Behaviorally, the rate of commission errors for incongruent No Go trials did not significantly differ between MD-only and HC groups. Intact or even pronounced neural error processing, together with the equal or even smaller rate of commission errors in MD-only compared to HC, is in line with previous observations [24,52]. Hereby, the high accuracy is frequently accompanied by relatively slower reaction times and has been interpreted as a trade-off between accuracy and speed in depressed patients [53]. By contrast, the MD-NSSI group committed significantly more commission errors than HC and, as a trend, also more than MD-only. Furthermore, reaction times for commission errors were significantly increased compared to MD-only. Both phenomena are in line with impaired response inhibition, and potentially less cognitive control, in MD-NSSI patients. However, it has to be noted that a recent review pointed out that a robust pattern showing alterations in neural systems of cognitive control deficits in NSSI has not been established [54].
Neurofunctionally, we observed the expected differential activation in previously reported networks related to response inhibition and commission errors [27,55,56,57]. In particular, differential activation of the dACC, SMA, bilateral anterior insula, and bilateral inferior frontal gyrus was observed for each of the three groups (see Table S1). Comparing the three groups in our whole-brain analysis, we observed significantly decreased differential neural activation related to errors of commission (No Go errors) in MD-NSSI for the left IFG and midline dACC compared to MD-only. Decreased differential neural activation of the right IFG and middle frontal gyrus was evident for MD-NSSI patients when compared to HC. This effect of blunted differential neural activation in the MD-NSSI group, together with the increased number of errors of commission in MD-NSSI, can be interpreted as a correlate of impaired inhibitory control, as, e.g., discussed in Case et al. [58] and Schreiner et al. [59]. By contrast, MD-only patients presented increased differential neural activation of the dACC/IFG network for errors of commission when compared against MD-NSSI and even to HC.
In particular, the right-lateralized IFG has been linked to the specific executive function of response inhibition in neuroimaging studies with the Go/No Go paradigm, stop-signal tasks, or tasks involving cognitive set switching or interference suppression [19,29,60,61,62]. Graded neural activation in the IFG system was found to increase with increased demands for response control [63], allowing for the interpretation that lower activation might represent either a lower demand or perhaps also a lower ability for response control. Other neuroimaging studies showed co-activation of the right IFG during response inhibition, but also during other tasks, such as cognitive set switching [61] or interference suppression [62]. These findings indicate a broader role for this region in attentional and cognitive control [64]. Furthermore, differences in cognitive control may relate to differences in impulsivity [18], which may explain why our MD-NSSI patients showed an altered neural response for this region.
Regarding the dACC, previous studies consistently linked neural activations within this region with performance and error monitoring [56,65,66,67,68]. Moreover, according to [69], the dACC is involved in directing attention toward task-relevant stimuli and in maintaining associations between actions and their outcomes. Thus, activation of the dACC was suggested to serve to improve task performance through the modulation of control over the motor system and via allocating the capacities of different competing neural systems [70]. Several electrophysiological and fMRI studies [24,35,36,37,38] have already observed increased neural activation of this region during error processing in patients with major depression, as in the MD-only group investigated here. This may reflect a potential neural correlate of enhanced sensitivity to negative environmental cues or errors in major depression [71,72], which is more present in MD-only patients than in MD-NSSI patients (see Table 3).
Recent neuroimaging studies with fMRI using cognitive and emotional tasks [48,73,74,75] and a meta-analysis [76] provide further evidence for a distinct neurobiology regarding brain functioning in patients with NSSI. Involving a similar network as in the current study on error processing, altered neural reactivity during reward and emotion processing was observed for the rostral ACC and the left IFG extending into the insula. Further studies could try to elucidate whether altered reactivity within this network could serve as a more global biomarker for clinical interventions and in developing diagnoses.
Regarding the limitations of our study, in particular, the relatively small sample compromises the strength and the generalizability of our data, and, therefore, the present results await empirical replication within larger samples. Moreover, the use of psychostimulant medication in one patient in the MD-only group may have potentially altered the neural activations relevant for error processing. However, control for this potential confounder confirmed that the overall pattern of group differences did not change substantially. Similarly, control calculations to exclude confounders due to motor activity still yielded similar results, with only smaller cluster sizes compared to the original analyses. Conceptually, it is important to note that the type of impulsivity that is captured by the present Go/No Go paradigm is situated in the cognitive domain, with impulsivity operationalized as the inability to inhibit behavioral impulses, as an important component of executive functions [77]. Furthermore, impulsivity was measured in a non-emotionally laden situation, and it would be interesting to investigate response inhibition in a more emotionally laden experimental situation as the driving force behind its failure.
By examining neural responses to errors in depressed adolescents with and without NSSI, we aimed to improve our neurobiological understanding of NSSI and hope to stimulate improvements in diagnostic procedures and treatment outcomes or monitoring. We observed an attenuated neural response in brain regions previously related to error processing and response inhibition, along with poorer task performance in depressed patients with comorbid NSSI. This pattern may represent a possible correlate for the clinically observed impaired inhibitory control in NSSI. Considering that there are very few neuroimaging data on similar tasks in NSSI, the replication and validation of our findings is needed in larger samples before these potential biomarkers can be translated into clinical practice.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/article/10.3390/biomedicines10123188/s1, Figure S1: Scatterplot of numbers of commission errors in correlation to differential neural activities within midline dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC, peak voxel MNI coordinates: x/y/z = 10/22/38) presented with corresponding trend lines in depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 13) and in adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 13); Table S1: Significant neural activations (p ≤ 0.05; minimum cluster size of k = 183 contiguously significant voxels at p < 0.005, uncorrected) of the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent No Go trials’ separately for healthy adolescent controls (HC; n = 14), adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 12), and depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14); Table S2: Significant neural activations (p ≤ 0.05; minimum cluster size of k = 183 contiguously significant voxels at p < 0.005, uncorrected) of the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent Go trials’ separately for healthy adolescent controls (HC; n = 14), adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 13), and depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.A.; Data curation, R.C.B.; Formal analysis, K.M., S.H. and B.A.; Funding acquisition, P.L.P.; Investigation, B.A.; Methodology, G.G. and B.A.; Project administration, R.C.B. and B.A.; Resources, G.G.; Software, S.H.; Supervision, K.M. and B.A.; Validation, K.M. and H.G.; Writing—original draft, K.M. and S.H.; Writing—review and editing, P.L.P., G.G., H.G. and B.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ethics committee of Ulm University (protocol code EA 58/2012).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

P.P. has received research funding from the German Federal Ministry of Education and Health (BMBF), the German Federal Institute for Drugs and Medical Devices (BfARM), the Baden Wuerttemberg Foundation, the Volkswagen Foundation, the Landeszielsteuerungskommission, the Austrian research agency (FWF), the Hochuljubiläumsfonds, the Zukunftsfonds and the Nationalfonds. He has been involved in clinical research projects with Lundbeck and Servier. He has received a speaker’s honorarium from Shire, Gerrot Lannach, Infectopharm, GSK and Oral-B and is an advisor for Delta4 and Boehringer-Ingelheim. He declares no conflicts of interest with regard to the work presented in this manuscript. The other authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-V. 2013. Available online: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053 (accessed on 8 December 2013). [CrossRef]
  2. Klonsky, E.D.; Glenn, C.R. Assessing the functions of non-suicidal self-injury: Psychometric properties of the Inventory of Statements About Self-injury (ISAS). J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2009, 31, 215–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Plener, P.L.; Schumacher, T.S.; Munz, L.M.; Groschwitz, R.C. The longitudinal course of non-suicidal self-injury and deliberate self-harm: A systematic review of the literature. Borderline Personal. Disord. Emot. Dysregul. 2015, 2, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Gillies, D.; Christou, M.A.; Dixon, A.C.; Featherston, O.J.; Rapti, I.; Garcia-Anguita, A. Prevalence and Characteristics of Self-Harm in Adolescents: Meta-Analyses of Community-Based Studies 1990–2015. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2018, 57, 733–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kaess, M.; Parzer, P.; Mattern, M.; Plener, P.L.; Bifulco, A.; Resch, F. Adverse childhood experiences and their impact on frequency, severity, and the individual function of nonsuicidal self-injury in youth. Psychiatry Res. 2013, 206, 265–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Glenn, C.R.; Klonsky, E.D. Nonsuicidal Self-Injury Disorder: An Empirical Investigation in Adolescent Psychiatric Patients. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2013, 42, 496–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Nitkowski, D.; Petermann, F. Non-suicidal self-injury and comorbid mental disorders: A review. Fortschr. Neurol. Psychiatr. 2011, 79, 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Brown, R.C.; Plener, P.L. Non-suicidal Self-Injury in Adolescence. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2017, 19, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Klonsky, E.D.; Oltmanns, T.F.; Turkheimer, E. Deliberate self-harm in a nonclinical population: Prevalence and psychological correlates. Am. J. Psychiatry 2003, 160, 1501–1508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Jacobson, C.M.; Muehlenkamp, J.J.; Miller, A.L.; Turner, J.B. Psychiatric Impairment Among Adolescents Engaging in Different Types of Deliberate Self-Harm. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2008, 37, 363–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Buelens, T.; Costantini, G.; Luyckx, K.; Claes, L. Comorbidity Between Non-suicidal Self-Injury Disorder and Borderline Personality Disorder in Adolescents: A Graphical Network Approach. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 580922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Taylor, P.J.; Jomar, K.; Dhingra, K.; Forrester, R.; Shahmalak, U.; Dickson, J.M. A meta-analysis of the prevalence of different functions of non-suicidal self-injury. J. Affect Disord. 2018, 227, 759–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lockwood, J.; Daley, D.; Townsend, E.; Sayal, K. Impulsivity and self-harm in adolescence: A systematic review. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 26, 387–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Chandler, A.; Myers, F.; Platt, S. The Construction of Self-Injury in the Clinical Literature: A Sociological Exploration. Suicide Life-Threat. Behav. 2011, 41, 98–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Simeon, D.; Stanley, B.; Frances, A.; Mann, J.J.; Winchel, R.; Stanley, M. Self-mutilation in personality disorders: Psychological and biological correlates. Am. J. Psychiatry 1992, 149, 221–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Herpertz, S.; Steinmeyer, S.M.; Marx, D.; Oidtmann, A.; Sass, H. The significance of aggression and impulsivity for self-mutilative behavior. Pharmacopsychiatry 1995, 28 (Suppl. 2), 64–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Fikke, L.T.; Melinder, A.; Landrø, N.I. Executive functions are impaired in adolescents engaging in non-suicidal self-injury. Psychol. Med. 2011, 41, 601–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Dalley, J.W.; Everitt, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 2011, 69, 680–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Aron, A.R.; Robbins, T.W.; Poldrack, R.A. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004, 8, 170–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Wager, T.D.; Sylvester, C.-Y.C.; Lacey, S.C.; Nee, D.E.; Franklin, M.; Jonides, J. Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 2005, 27, 323–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Eriksen, B.A.; Eriksen, C.W. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 1974, 16, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Ruchsow, M.; Spitzer, M.; Grön, G.; Grothe, J.; Kiefer, M. Error processing and impulsiveness in normals: Evidence from event-related potentials. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005, 24, 317–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Sosic-Vasic, Z.; Ulrich, M.; Ruchsow, M.; Vasic, N.; Grön, G. The modulating effect of personality traits on neural error monitoring: Evidence from event-related fMRI. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e42930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Malejko, K.; Hafner, S.; Plener, P.L.; Bonenberger, M.; Groen, G.; Abler, B. Neural signature of error processing in major depression. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2021, 271, 1359–1368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Graf, H.; Abler, B.; Freudenmann, R.; Beschoner, P.; Schaeffeler, E.; Spitzer, M.; Schwab, M.; Grön, G. Neural correlates of error monitoring modulated by atomoxetine in healthy volunteers. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, 890–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ruchsow, M.; Walter, H.; Buchheim, A.; Martius, P.; Spitzer, M.; Kächele, H. Electrophysiological correlates of error processing in borderline personality disorder. Biol. Psychol. 2006, 72, 133–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Garavan, H.; Ross, T.J.; Murphy, K.; Roche, R.A.P.; Stein, E.A. Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction. Neuroimage 2002, 17, 1820–1829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Garavan, H.; Ross, T.J.; Stein, E.A. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1999, 96, 8301–8306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Menon, V.; Adleman, N.E.; White, C.D.; Glover, G.H.; Reiss, A.L. Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2001, 12, 131–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Castellanos, F.X.; Giedd, J.N.; Marsh, W.L.; Hamburger, S.D.; Vaituzis, A.C.; Dickstein, D.P. Quantitative Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1996, 53, 607–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Casey, B.J.; Castellanos, F.X.; Giedd, J.N.; Marsh, W.L.; Hamburger, S.D.; Schubert, A.B. Implication of Right Frontostriatal Circuitry in Response Inhibition and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 36, 374–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Vaidya, C.J.; Austin, G.; Kirkorian, G.; Ridlehuber, H.W.; Desmond, J.E.; Glover, G.H. Selective effects of methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A functional magnetic resonance study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1998, 95, 14494–14499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Sowell, E.R.; Thompson, P.M.; Welcome, S.E.; Henkenius, A.L.; Toga, A.W.; Peterson, B.S. Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 2003, 362, 1699–1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Overtoom, C.C.; Kenemans, J.L.; Verbaten, M.N.; Kemner, C.; van der Molen, M.W.; van Engeland, H. Inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A psychophysiological study of the stop task. Biol. Psychiatry 2002, 51, 668–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Alexopoulos, G.S.; Murphy, C.F.; Gunning-Dixon, F.M.; Kalayam, B.; Katz, R.; Kanellopoulos, D. Event-related potentials in an emotional go/no-go task and remission of geriatric depression. Neuroreport 2007, 18, 217–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Holmes, A.J.; Pizzagalli, D.A. Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Error Processing Dysfunctions in Major Depressive Disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008, 65, 179–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Chiu, P.H.; Deldin, P.J. Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in major depressive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2007, 164, 608–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Langenecker, S.A.; Kennedy, S.E.; Guidotti, L.M.; Briceno, E.M.; Own, L.S.; Hooven, T. Frontal and Limbic Activation During Inhibitory Control Predicts Treatment Response in Major Depressive Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 62, 1272–1280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Delmo, C.; Weiffenbach, O.; Gabriel, M.; Poustka, F. Kiddie-SADS-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL); Auflage Der Dtsch Forschungsversion Frankfurt Am Main Klin Für Psychiatr Und Psychother Des Kindes-Und Jugendalters Der Univ Frankfurt: Frankfurt, Germany, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  40. Fischer, G.; Ameis, N.; Parzer, P.; Plener, P.L.; Groschwitz, R.; Vonderlin, E. The German version of the self-injurious thoughts and behaviors interview (SITBI-G): A tool to assess non-suicidal self-injury and suicidal behavior disorder. BMC Psychiatry 2014, 14, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Nock, M.K.; Holmberg, E.B.; Photos, V.I.; Michel, B.D. Self-Injurious Thoughts and Behaviors Interview: Development, Reliability, and Validity in an Adolescent Sample. Psychol. Assess. 2007, 19, 309–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A.; Brown, G.K. Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio TX Psychol. Corp. 1996, 1, 82. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hautzinger, M.; Keller, F.; Kühner, C. Beck Depressions-Inventar (BDI-II); Harcourt Test Services Frankfurt: Frankfurt, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  44. Poznanski, E.O.; Mokros, H.B. Children’s Depression Rating Scale, Revised (CDRS-R); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  45. Keller, F.; Grieb, J.; Ernst, M.; Spröber, N.; Fegert, J.M.; Kölch, M. Children’s Depression Rating Scale-Revised (CDRS-R): Development of a German version and psychometric properties in a clinical sample. Z. Kinder. Jugendpsychiatr. Psychother. 2011, 39, 179–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Horn, W.; Lukesch, H.; Mayrhofer, S.; Kormann, A. Prüfsystem Für Schul-Und Bildungsberatung Für 6. Bis 13; Klassen–Revidierte Fassung; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  47. Petermann, F.; Petermann, U.J. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children®—Fourth Edition; Pearson: Frankfurt, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  48. Williams, K.D.; Cheung, C.K.; Choi, W. Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 79, 748–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Groschwitz, R.C.; Plener, P.L.; Groen, G.; Bonenberger, M.; Abler, B. Differential neural processing of social exclusion in adolescents with non-suicidal self-injury: An fMRI study. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 255, 43–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. JASP Team. JASP (Version 0.163), Computer software: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2022.
  51. McHugh, C.M.; Chun Lee, R.S.; Hermens, D.F.; Corderoy, A.; Large, M.; Hickie, I.B. Impulsivity in the self-harm and suicidal behavior of young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2019, 116, 51–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ladouceur, C.D.; Slifka, J.S.; Dahl, R.E.; Birmaher, B.; Axelson, D.A.; Ryan, N.D. Altered error-related brain activity in youth with major depression. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012, 2, 351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Tang, Y.; Zhang, X.; Simmonite, M.; Li, H.; Zhang, T.; Guo, Q. Hyperactivity within an extensive cortical distribution associated with excessive sensitivity in error processing in unmedicated depression: A combined event-related potential and sLORETA study. Undefined 2013, 90, 282–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kaess, M.; Hooley, J.M.; Klimes-Dougan, B.; Koenig, J.; Plener, P.L.; Reichl, C. Advancing a temporal framework for understanding the biology of nonsuicidal self- injury: An expert review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 130, 228–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Mathalon, D.H.; Whitfield, S.L.; Ford, J.M. Anatomy of an error: ERP and fMRI. Biol. Psychol. 2003, 64, 119–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Ullsperger, M.; Von Cramon, D.Y. Subprocesses of performance monitoring: A dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 1387–1401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Botvinick, M.; Nystrom, L.E.; Fissell, K.; Carter, C.S.; Cohen, J.D. Conflict monitoring versus selection for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature 1999, 402, 179–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Case, J.A.C.; Mattoni, M.; Olino, T.M. Examining the Neurobiology of Non-Suicidal Self-Injury in Children and Adolescents: The Role of Reward Responsivity. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 3561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Westlund Schreiner, M.; Klimes-Dougan, B.; Mueller, B.A.; Eberly, L.E.; Reigstad, K.M.; Carstedt, P.A. Multi-modal neuroimaging of adolescents with non-suicidal self-injury: Amygdala functional connectivity. J. Affect. Disord. 2017, 221, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Aron, A.R.; Fletcher, P.C.; Bullmore, E.T.; Sahakian, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 2003, 6, 115–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Konishi, S.; Nakajima, K.; Uchida, I.; Kikyo, H.; Kameyama, M.; Miyashita, Y. Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain 1999, 122, 981–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Bunge, S.A.; Dudukovic, N.M.; Thomason, M.E.; Vaidya, C.J.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron 2002, 33, 301–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Dodds, C.M.; Morein-Zamir, S.; Robbins, T.W. Dissociating Inhibition, Attention, and Response Control in the Frontoparietal Network Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Cereb. Cortex 2011, 21, 1155–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Sharp, D.J.; Bonnelle, V.; De Boissezon, X.; Beckmann, C.F.; James, S.G.; Patel, M.C. Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 6106–6111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Taylor, S.F.; Stern, E.R.; Gehring, W.J. Neural systems for error monitoring: Recent findings and theoretical perspectives. Neuroscientist 2007, 13, 160–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Gray, J.R.; Molfese, D.L.; Snyder, A. Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Response Conflict: Effects of Frequency, Inhibition and Errors. Cereb. Cortex 2001, 11, 825–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Carter, C.S.; Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Botvinick, M.M.; Noll, D.; Cohen, J.D. Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Error Detection, and the Online Monitoring of Performance. Science 1998, 280, 747–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Carter, C.S.; Macdonald, A.M.; Botvinick, M.; Ross, L.L.; Stenger, V.A.; Noll, D. Parsing executive processes: Strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 1944–1948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Dosenbach, N.U.F.; Visscher, K.M.; Palmer, E.D.; Miezin, F.M.; Wenger, K.K.; Kang, H.C. A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets. Neuron 2006, 50, 799–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Holroyd, C.B.; Coles, M.G.H. The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 2002, 109, 679–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Elliott, R.; Sahakian, B.J.; Michael, A.; Paykel, E.S.; Dolan, R.J. Abnormal neural response to feedback on planning and guessing tasks in patients with unipolar depression. Psychol. Med. 1998, 28, 559–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Steffens, D.C.; Wagner, H.R.; Levy, R.M.; Horn, K.A.; Krishnan, K.R.R. Performance feedback deficit in geriatric depression. Biol. Psychiatry 2001, 50, 358–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Malejko, K.; Neff, D.; Brown, R.C.; Plener, P.L.; Bonenberger, M.; Abler, B. Neural Signatures of Social Inclusion in Borderline Personality Disorder Versus Non-suicidal Self-injury. Brain Topogr. 2019, 32, 753–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Brown, R.C.; Plener, P.L.; Groen, G.; Neff, D.; Bonenberger, M.; Abler, B. Differential neural processing of social exclusion and inclusion in adolescents with non-suicidal self-injury and young adults with borderline personality disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2017, 8, 267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Plener, P.L.; Bubalo, N.; Fladung, A.K.; Ludolph, A.G.; Lulé, D. Prone to excitement: Adolescent females with non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) show altered cortical pattern to emotional and NSS-related material. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2012, 203, 146–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Lai, M.; Jiang, P.; Xu, J.; Luo, D.; Hao, X.; Li, J. Abnormal brain activity in nonsuicidal self-injury: A coordinate-based activation likelihood meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Psychoradiology 2021, 1, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Bakhshani, N.M. Impulsivity: A predisposition toward risky behaviors. Int. J. High Risk Behav. Addict. 2014, 3, e20428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Erikson flanker Go/No Go paradigm as used during fMRI. The figure shows a sequence of an incongruent No Go trial as an example, starting from fixation until feedback. The No Go target letter ‘P’ is highlighted by a red rectangle only for demonstrational purposes (not shown during the real fMRI experiment). Durations of each trial segment are listed in milliseconds (ms).
Figure 1. Erikson flanker Go/No Go paradigm as used during fMRI. The figure shows a sequence of an incongruent No Go trial as an example, starting from fixation until feedback. The No Go target letter ‘P’ is highlighted by a red rectangle only for demonstrational purposes (not shown during the real fMRI experiment). Durations of each trial segment are listed in milliseconds (ms).
Biomedicines 10 03188 g001
Figure 2. Neural activations of inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) for incorrect and correct incongruent No Go trials ((A). Left IFG; (B). Right IFG) in depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14), adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 12), and healthy controls (HC; n = 14). Bar charts depict fMRI peak voxel parameter estimates extracted from significant clusters located in left (MD-only minus MD-NSSI) and right (HC minus MD-NSSI) pars triangularis of IFG; error bars depict standard error of the mean. x, y, z: MNI coordinates provided in mm; BA = Brodmann area.
Figure 2. Neural activations of inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) for incorrect and correct incongruent No Go trials ((A). Left IFG; (B). Right IFG) in depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14), adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 12), and healthy controls (HC; n = 14). Bar charts depict fMRI peak voxel parameter estimates extracted from significant clusters located in left (MD-only minus MD-NSSI) and right (HC minus MD-NSSI) pars triangularis of IFG; error bars depict standard error of the mean. x, y, z: MNI coordinates provided in mm; BA = Brodmann area.
Biomedicines 10 03188 g002
Table 1. Socio-demographic and questionnaire data in healthy controls (HC), adolescents with major depression (MD-only), and depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI). Group differences were calculated with ANOVAs, followed by Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc comparisons at an alpha level of p ≤ 0.05.
Table 1. Socio-demographic and questionnaire data in healthy controls (HC), adolescents with major depression (MD-only), and depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI). Group differences were calculated with ANOVAs, followed by Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc comparisons at an alpha level of p ≤ 0.05.
ANOVAs with Corresponding Post-Hoc Comparisons
HCMD-OnlyMD-NSSI MD-NSSI vs.
MD-Only
HC vs. MD-OnlyHC vs. MD-NSSI
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)FpAverage DifferencepbonfAverage DifferencepbonfAverage Differencepbonf
Demographics
N141314--
Femalen = 11n = 10n = 11--
Malen = 3n = 3n = 3--
age [years]14.4 (1.8)16.2 (1.4)15.4 (1.9)3.3490.046−0.7970.721−1.7250.041−0.9290.494
Psychometry
IQ109.6 (10.6)101.8 (11.8)101.3 (13.2)2.0930.137------
CDRS-R score21.4 (2.6)56.4 (7.4)51.4 (13.6)59.29<0.001−4.9560.498−34.956<0.001−30.000<0.001
BDI-II score *2.9 (3.6)27.2 (10.7)21.7 (12.2)23.988<0.001−5.4520.462−24.31<0.001−18.857<0.001
Number of NSSI events last year0093.3 (225.9)--
Abbreviations: ANOVA: analysis of variance; SD: standard deviation; F: F-value; p: p-value; pbonf: Bonferroni-corrected p-value; n: sample size; []: corresponding unit; IQ: intelligence quotient; CDRS-R score: score on Children’s Depression Rating Scale—Revised; BDI-II score: score on Beck Depression Inventory—Second Version; *: BDI-II data from one subject in the MD-only group were missing (n = 12).
Table 2. Performance data in healthy controls (HC; n = 14), adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 13), and depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14). Group differences were calculated with ANOVAs, followed by Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc comparisons at an alpha level of p ≤ 0.05.
Table 2. Performance data in healthy controls (HC; n = 14), adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 13), and depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14). Group differences were calculated with ANOVAs, followed by Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc comparisons at an alpha level of p ≤ 0.05.
ANOVAs with Corresponding Post-Hoc Comparisons
HCMD-OnlyMD-NSSI MD-NSSI vs.
MD-Only
HC vs. MD-OnlyHC vs. MD-NSSI
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)FpAverage DifferencepbonfAverage DifferencepbonfAverage Differencepbonf
Incongruent No Go
Number of commission errors9.6 (6.5)11.6 (6.9)18.0 (11.6)3.5170.0406.3850.193−1.9731.000−8.3570.046
Reaction time [ms]448.5 (57.9)390.4 (53.8)457.3 (67.8)4.5620.017−66.8770.02458.1380.058−8.7391.000
Incongruent Go
Number of errors2.8 (2.0)3.5
(3.7)
4.3
(4.1)
0.6860.510------
Reaction time incorrect [ms]470.4 (39.4)393.5 (43.3)412.8 (47.3)9.883<0.00119.2590.91776.88<0.00157.6210.010
Number of correct responses21.1 (11.0)23.3 (11.0)20.4 (10.3)0.2630.771------
Reaction time correct [ms]468.0 (43.0)410.4 (36.0)427.0 (46.9)6.6850.00316.6140.94657.6430.00341.0290.044
Abbreviations: ANOVA: analysis of variance; SD: standard deviation; F: F-value; p: p-value; pbonf: Bonferroni-corrected p-value; n: sample size; []: corresponding unit; ms: milliseconds.
Table 3. Significant (p ≤ 0.05; minimum cluster size of 183 contiguously significant voxels at p < 0.005, uncorrected) between-group differences in differential neural activations from the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent No Go trials’ corresponding to neural error signaling. Depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury: MD-NSSI (n = 14); adolescents with major depression: MD-only (n = 12; no first-level contrast in 1 patient with zero No Go errors); healthy controls: HC (n = 14).
Table 3. Significant (p ≤ 0.05; minimum cluster size of 183 contiguously significant voxels at p < 0.005, uncorrected) between-group differences in differential neural activations from the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent No Go trials’ corresponding to neural error signaling. Depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury: MD-NSSI (n = 14); adolescents with major depression: MD-only (n = 12; no first-level contrast in 1 patient with zero No Go errors); healthy controls: HC (n = 14).
BAAnatomic LabelL/R/MCluster SizeZMNI
k (Vx) xyz
MD-NSSI < MD-only32dACCM1833.80102238
45IFG, pars triangularisL4273.62−484010
48IFG, pars opercularisL3484.34−38428
40supramarginal gyrusL4983.13−64−3430
2inferior parietal cortexL#3.53−42−3240
HC < MD-only48anterior insulaL12384.45−38−218
48postcentral gyrusL#3.88−48−618
3postcentral gyrusL#3.82−56−1640
40inferior parietal cortexL#3.23−40−3438
precuneusL4384.02−14−5050
37cerebellumL1896.63−20−42−26
MD-NSSI < HC45middle frontal gyrusR4493.62443424
48IFG, pars triangularisR#3.27382628
48IFG, pars opercularisR#3.24441630
48supramarginal gyrusR3663.6358−4226
40inferior parietal cortexL1914.00−42−4452
HC < MD-NSSI precuneusL9444.05−12−4652
n: sample size; BA: Brodman area; L/R/M: left/right/midline; Z: z-score of standard normal distribution; k: number of voxels (Vx); MNI: Montreal Neurological Institute (x-, y-, z-coordinates are provided in mm); mm: millimeter; dACC: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; #: maximum part of cluster above.
Table 4. Significant (p ≤ 0.05; minimum cluster size of 183 contiguously significant voxels at p < 0.005, uncorrected) between-group differences in differential neural activations from the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent Go trials’ corresponding to neural error signaling. Depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14); adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 13); healthy controls (HC; n = 14).
Table 4. Significant (p ≤ 0.05; minimum cluster size of 183 contiguously significant voxels at p < 0.005, uncorrected) between-group differences in differential neural activations from the contrast ‘incorrect minus correct incongruent Go trials’ corresponding to neural error signaling. Depressed adolescents with comorbid non-suicidal self-injury (MD-NSSI; n = 14); adolescents with major depression (MD-only; n = 13); healthy controls (HC; n = 14).
BAAnatomic LabelL/R/MCluster SizeZMNI
k (Vx) xYz
MD-NSSI < MD-only45IFG, pars triangularisL2324.12−482610
45IFG, pars triangularisR1993.1154324
48IFG, pars opercularisR#3.5350146
MD-only < MD-NSSI6SMAM2974.022−456
HC < MD-only45IFG, pars triangularisL4994.60−483012
48anterior insulaL#3.15−40146
n: sample size; BA: Brodman area; L/R/M: left/right/midline; Z: z-score of standard normal distribution; k: number of voxels (Vx); MNI: Montreal Neurological Institute (x-, y-, z-coordinates are provided in mm); IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; #: maximum part of cluster above; SMA: supplementary motor area.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Malejko, K.; Hafner, S.; Brown, R.C.; Plener, P.L.; Grön, G.; Graf, H.; Abler, B. Neural Signatures of Error Processing in Depressed Adolescents with Comorbid Non-Suicidal Self-Injury (NSSI). Biomedicines 2022, 10, 3188. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/biomedicines10123188

AMA Style

Malejko K, Hafner S, Brown RC, Plener PL, Grön G, Graf H, Abler B. Neural Signatures of Error Processing in Depressed Adolescents with Comorbid Non-Suicidal Self-Injury (NSSI). Biomedicines. 2022; 10(12):3188. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/biomedicines10123188

Chicago/Turabian Style

Malejko, Kathrin, Stefan Hafner, Rebecca C. Brown, Paul L. Plener, Georg Grön, Heiko Graf, and Birgit Abler. 2022. "Neural Signatures of Error Processing in Depressed Adolescents with Comorbid Non-Suicidal Self-Injury (NSSI)" Biomedicines 10, no. 12: 3188. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/biomedicines10123188

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop