Next Article in Journal
Asymmetric Power Relations in Gynaecological Consultations for Cervical Cancer Prevention: Biomedical and Gender Issues
Previous Article in Journal
Perspectives and Attitudes of Patients with COVID-19 toward a Telerehabilitation Programme: A Qualitative Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Conspiracy Theories, Psychological Distress, and Sympathy for Violent Radicalization in Young Adults during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study

1
Division of Social and Cultural Psychiatry, McGill University, CLSC Parc-Extension, 7085 Hutchison, Montréal, QC H3N 1Y9, Canada
2
Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics, and Occupational Health, McGill University, 1020 Pine Ave. W, Montreal, QC H3A 1A2, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(15), 7846; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18157846
Submission received: 4 June 2021 / Revised: 19 July 2021 / Accepted: 21 July 2021 / Published: 24 July 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Mental Health)

Abstract

:
The COVID-19 pandemic has spread uncertainty, promoted psychological distress, and fueled interpersonal conflict. The concomitant upsurge in endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories is worrisome because they are associated with both non-adherence to public health guidelines and intention to commit violence. This study investigates associations between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, support for violent radicalization (VR) and psychological distress among young adults in Canada. We hypothesized that (a) endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories is positively associated with support for VR, and (b) psychological distress modifies the relationship between COVID-19 conspiracy theories and support for VR. A total of 6003 participants aged 18–35 years old residing in four major Canadian cities completed an online survey between 16 October 2020 and 17 November 2020, that included questions about endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, support for VR, psychological distress, and socio-economic status. Endorsement of conspiracy theories was associated with support for VR in multivariate regression (β = 0.88, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.80–0.96). There is a significant interaction effect between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress (β = 0.49, 95% CI 0.40–0.57). The magnitude of the association was stronger in individuals reporting high psychological distress (β = 1.36, 95% CI 1.26–1.46) compared to those reporting low psychological distress (β = 0.47, 95% CI 0.35–0.59). The association between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and VR represents a public health challenge requiring immediate attention. The interaction with psychological distress suggests that policy efforts should combine communication and psychological strategies to mitigate the legitimation of violence.

1. Introduction

Countering violent radicalization (VR), defined as an “individual or collective process whereby normal practices of dialogue, compromise and tolerance between groups/individuals with diverging interests are abandoned and one or more groups/individuals engage in violent actions to reach a specific goal,” [1] is a public health priority. Ideologically motivated violence is a matter of public health because acts of terror, hate incidents, and hate crimes have serious physical and mental health consequences not only for those most proximally affected by violence, but also impacting the wellbeing of the broader population [2]. In the past, efforts to counter VR were primarily concerned with violent actions of lone actors and marginalized extremist groups; now, radical and violent ideologies have become mainstream and infiltrated the highest levels of government in countries around the world [3].
This past year, the COVID-19 pandemic has impacted and interacted with VR in troubling ways. The pandemic has accentuated structural violence, including systemic racism and socio-economic disparities [4], which exacerbate social polarization and support for VR [5,6]. Extremist groups have, and continue to, capitalize on the pandemic to spread disinformation that scapegoat marginalized communities and endorse acts of violence [7]. Political leaders around the world, including in the United States, exploit COVID-19 related fear and anger in the populace to consolidate power and promote anti-democratic agendas [3].
One strategy used by extremists to sow dissent is to endorse conspiracy theories about the origins of the virus and authorities’ handling of the pandemic [8,9]. COVID-19 conspiracy theory beliefs include ideas such as: (1) contrary to information provided by governments, the virus is of unnatural origin; (2) the spread of the virus is encouraged by pharmaceutical companies for financial gain, and; (3) the virus is a vehicle of the Chinese government [10]. Endorsement of conspiracy theories such as these fuel distrust in government and public health authorities and are associated with intentions to follow public health guidelines (e.g., anti-vaccine beliefs) [10,11]. Furthermore, COVID-19 conspiracist ideation is positively associated with the justification, willingness and intent to be violent, findings that are aligned with previous research linking general endorsement of conspiracy theories and violent action [12].
COVID-19 conspiracist ideation is associated with greater anxiety and feelings of powerlessness [13], both indicators of psychological distress, and the impact of the pandemic on psychological distress is becoming well established [14]. Anxious and depressive symptoms are associated with fear of the virus and with the collateral damages stemming from the confinement, and in particular from income loss and social isolation. The pandemic has exacerbated pre-existing social inequalities and mounting evidence indicates that COVID-related psychological distress disproportionately affects minority groups and vulnerable communities [15]. Conspiracy beliefs may provide a mechanism to empower individuals by allowing them to adopt narratives that explain and reduce the current uncertainties and distress associated with powerlessness. As such, conspiracy beliefs about the pandemic may provide individuals with an opportunity to feel both more in control in times of high uncertainty and more connected to a community of individuals who share the same emotional experience and beliefs [12,16].
Throughout history, conspiracy theories have been a part of pandemics and epidemics. As far back as the medieval black plague, conspiracy theories proliferated to provide meaning and assign responsibility of the perceived threat to outgroup individuals, either marginal or strangers [17,18]. Thus, narratives designating an out-group enemy were widespread before the COVID-19 health emergency, and are now associated with an exacerbation of social inequalities and an absence of future perspectives [19]. In the past year, high levels of uncertainty and distress brought about by the pandemic have fueled an upsurge in social polarization.
The association between psychological distress and support for VR has been repeatedly corroborated [20,21]; in contrast, to date no research has explored the potential moderation of the association between COVID-19 conspiracist ideation and support for VR by psychological distress. Studying the relationships between conspiracy theories, attitudes legitimizing VR, and psychological distress is crucial in developing public health communication and prevention programs which take into account the mental health component of these social dynamics. Results from such studies could contribute to the identification of tools to mitigate the conflicts and violence associated with the present context. We propose a theoretical model (Figure 1) which contextualizes the relationship between support for VR, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress.
This study aims to investigate associations between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, psychological distress and sympathy for VR in a large sample of young adults living in four Canadian urban settings (i.e., Calgary, Edmonton, Montreal and Toronto). We hypothesised that (a) support for VR is positively associated with endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, and (b) there is an interaction effect of psychological distress and endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories on support for VR.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Description of the Sample

In total, 6003 individuals aged 18–35 completed an on-line survey, of which 54.8% were women (see Table 1 for a detailed description of the sample).
The online survey targeted young adults in large cities in Quebec, Ontario and Alberta. We anticipated a sample size of 2000 participants in each province for a total of 6000 participants. Data collection took place between 16 October 2020 and 17 November 2020. The total response rate for the survey was 19% and 19%, 18%, 19% and 22% in Calgary, Edmonton, Montreal and Toronto, respectively. Inclusion criteria for participants were: aged between 18 and 35 years, and residents of Montreal, Toronto, Calgary or Edmonton. Exclusion criteria were: cognitive deficit or other disability that would prevent an individual from providing informed consent, and not speaking English or French (the languages in which the survey was administered). Participants are all registered in the AskingCanadians pool, a Delvinia Technology Inc. online data collection firm with access to more than one million Canadian professionals and consumers who are nationally representative by region and monitored against Statistics Canada. The firm emailed potential participants an introductory message with a hyperlink to the survey. Participants received a gift card valued at 2.50$ according to how much time they dedicated to the survey. Ethics approval was obtained by X (BLINDED FOR REVIEW) before initiating the study, and all participants provided an electronic informed consent.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Endorsement of Conspiracy Theories

Endorsement of COVID-19-related conspiracy theories was assessed with questions asking participants to rate, on a Likert scale from 1 = do not agree to 5 = agree completely, their level of agreement with four statements adapted from Freeman et al. [22]: “The government is misleading the public about the cause of the Coronavirus”, “The spread of the Coronavirus is a deliberate attempt by a group of powerful people to gain control”, “Coronavirus is a bioweapon developed by China to destroy the West”, and “The mainstream media is deliberately feeding us misinformation about the Coronavirus and lockdown”. The Cronbach alpha for the total score was 0.88.

2.2.2. Attitudes toward Violent Radicalization

The Sympathies for Radicalization scale (SyfoR) [23] consists of questions related to nine acts of protest ranging from nonviolent (e.g., take part in non-violent political protests) to progressively more extreme acts (e.g., use of suicide bombs to fight against injustices). Subjects are asked to rate their attitude towards these acts on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = completely condemn to 7 = completely sympathize) with a higher score meaning greater support for VR. A total score (range 8–56) of sympathy for radicalization was used in this study (excluding the non-violent protest item). The SyfoR has been adapted to Canadian contexts [24]. Cronbach’s alpha in this study was 0.97.
The Radicalism Intention Scale (RIS) is a subscale of the validated Activism and Radicalism Intention Scales (ARIS). A previous validation with ethnically diverse populations yielded adequate internal consistency and discriminant validity [25]. The RIS assesses an individual’s willingness to support illegal and violent behaviour in the name of one’s in-group or organisation. It is composed of four items rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree to 7 = completely agree) with a higher total score indicating more support for VR. The total score (range 4–28) was used for sensitivity analyses in this study and Cronbach’s alpha was 0.95.

2.2.3. Psychological Distress

The Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 (HSCL-25) is a self-report questionnaire aimed at screening for levels of anxiety and depression. Items are rated on a Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (extremely), and a total score is obtained by computing the mean of all items. The clinical cut-off is set at 1.75 (score range from 1 to 4). This means that an individual with a score of 1.75 or more can be considered as having high psychological distress. The HSCL-25′s psychometric qualities and transcultural validity have been well established among different cultural groups [26,27]. Cronbach’s alpha in this study was 0.98.

2.2.4. Sociodemographic Variables

Participants self-reported age, city of residence (Montreal, Calgary, Edmonton, Toronto), gender (woman, man or gender-diverse), and immigrant generation (first-, second- and third and above-generation immigrant). The financial problems variable was collected using the question “Presently in your household, are you experiencing difficulties related to lack of money?” (Not at all, some, a moderate amount, a lot), and educational level was collected with the question “What is the highest grade you completed?” (high school or less, technical degree or some college/university, university degree and above).

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Missing data were imputed with multiple imputation chained equations, R-package mice, using 5 imputed datasets [28]. For Table 2, p-values for differences in mean scores between categories were calculated using ANOVA. Effect size is reported as η2. Multivariate regressions and moderation analysis were adjusted for self-reported gender, age, city of residence, reported level of financial problems, educational level and immigrant generation. First, the association between VR and endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories was established using linear regression adjusted for the listed covariates. Second, moderation was identified using an interaction term for endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and the continuous mean score for psychological distress in the regression of sympathy for VR. Finally, the association between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and sympathy for VR was estimated in strata below and above the clinical cut-off for HSCL-25.

3. Results

At the descriptive level, mean total SyfoR and endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories scores were higher for self-reported men compared with women, and lower in first- and second-generation immigrants compared with individuals from families residing in Canada for three or more generations. A majority of respondents (69.1%) reported no or little financial problems, but those who did also had higher levels of sympathy for VR, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, and psychological distress. Regarding educational level, sympathy for VR, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, and psychological distress were highest for individuals with a technical degree or some college/university. Scores on sympathy for VR, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, and psychological distress were all higher in younger individuals (age 18–25 compared to 26–35). Among the four cities, participants in Montreal reported the highest unadjusted scores on the SyfoR, endorsement of conspiracy theories and psychological distress. Almost half of the total sample reported psychological distress mean scores above the clinical cut-off 1.75. In Montreal, this proportion was 56.1%.
In multivariate regression, endorsement of conspiracy theories was significantly associated with SyfoR scores (β = 0.88, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.80–0.96). (Table 3) First generation immigrants had a significantly lower SyfoR score than third or more generation, while there was no significant difference for second generation immigrants. Higher education was significantly associated with higher SyfoR scores. Different to the descriptive analyses, SyfoR scores were highest for individuals with university-level education. However, moderate financial problems were significantly associated with higher SyfoR scores, indicating a non-linear association between socio-economic status and support for VR in the current context.

Moderation Analyses

A significant interaction (moderation) was seen between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress continuous mean scores (β = 0.49, 95% CI 0.40–0.47). (Table 4) The magnitude of the association between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and SyfoR was greater in individuals reporting high psychological distress (β = 1.36, 95% CI 1.26–1.46) compared to those reporting low psychological distress (β = 0.47, 95% CI 0.35–0.59). The moderation effect of psychological distress on the association between sympathy for VR and endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories is also illustrated in Figure 2.
Sensitivity analyses using the RIS as outcome showed results in the same direction as for SyfoR (Supplemental Tables S1 and S2).

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to provide evidence of a worrisome phenomenon: the association between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and support for VR during the present health emergency among a large sample of young adults in four different urban settings in Canada. Further, the study shows moderation of this association by psychological distress.
Prior research suggests that endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories is problematic from a public health perspective, because it may hinder following public health guidelines during the pandemic [8, 9, 10]. Our results highlight another concerning public health issue related to the endorsement of conspiracy theories, that of increased support for VR [2]. During the pandemic, research has reported instances of support for, and engagement in, VR fueled by COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Most notably, the association of the virus with China has resulted in an increase in hate crimes and violence against individuals who identify as Asian [29]. There are also concerns that a believed association between COVID-19 and 5G technology motivated arson attacks against telecommunication infrastructure [12]. Nonetheless, to our knowledge, to date no multi-site empirical study has explored and demonstrated a relationship between psychological distress, COVID-19 conspiracy theories and legitimation of violence. The issue is urgent, as the global spread of COVID-19 conspiracy theories indicates a risk of a concomitant increase in levels of both sympathy for and acts of VR. An upsurge in levels of sympathy for VR may result in an increase in discrimination, hate crimes and incidents, as well as more deadly attacks from either lone actors or organized groups, particularly militias, encouraged by this shift in population attitudes. Our finding that the association of conspiracy theories and support for VR was moderated by psychological distress supports the hypothesis that symptoms of anxiety and depression interact with conspiracy beliefs, which are in turn associated with higher support for VR, highlighting the crucial role of supporting mental health among young people during the pandemic.
Aligned with previous North American studies of VR, support for VR scores were lower among women, older participants, and first-generation immigrants. Of importance, the same socio-demographic factors emerged as protective in association with endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories, further highlighting commonalities between the two phenomena. Thus, prevention and intervention programs to reduce both conspiracy beliefs and support for VR should support young adult men and gender minorities, and target majority groups rather than immigrant groups.
In this study, the non-linear association between socio-economic status and support for VR is noteworthy. Despite the importance of social inequities as a driver of VR, the literature has traditionally reported a weak association between poverty and this outcome [30]. During the pandemic, many have experienced significant financial difficulties, yet our results associate moderate but not severe financial difficulties with VR. This suggests that, while in a situation where survival is at stake, it may sometimes be difficult to engage in resistance or dissent; however, this also adds to the documentation of the pandemic-related upsurge in bitterness associated with the exacerbation of socio-economic inequities.

5. Limitations

There are several limitations to this study. First, this is a cross-sectional study; thus we cannot make causal claims and, similarly to previous research, have not been able to ascertain the temporality of psychological distress and conspiracist ideation [13]. Nonetheless, our results are consistent for both SyfoR and RIS, where the association between sympathy for VR and COVID-19 conspiracist ideation is significantly stronger in the stratum with psychological distress mean score above the clinical cut-off. Second, the overall response rate in this study was 19%. As such, there is considerable risk of selection bias that could over- or underestimate the relationship between COVID conspiracy theories and VR. For instance, if people who endorse conspiracy theories and support for VR were less likely to complete the survey, we may be underestimating the relationship between these variables. On-line surveys also present limitations as compared to face-to-face interviews, such as misunderstanding of study questions. In addition, online surveys often show an overrepresentation of individuals with higher education. However, an online survey is advantageous for research specific to conspiracy theories and VR in that it: (1) guarantees anonymity and increases the likelihood that subjects will provide accurate, sensitive information and (2) is attractive to the 18–35 age group and cultural communities who may distrust institutions and authorities [31,32]. Several studies have examined the validity and test–retest reliability of online self-administered survey instruments and found that overall psychometric qualities were satisfactory and comparable to traditional versions. Third, participants in this study were all living in four of the major Canadian urban settings, thus limiting the generalizability of results to young people living in rural areas within North America. Finally, in order to establish whether different types of conspiracy theories are associated with support for VR, more fine-grained measures regarding conspiracy theories are needed.

6. Conclusions

Given our findings, we argue that public health communication strategies regarding COVID-19, which thus far have focused predominantly on cognitive dimensions of disinformation (i.e., deconstructing conspiracy Table 1 theories), should integrate, consider, and target the underlying emotional components associated with the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs. Direct confrontation of conspiracy beliefs is at best ineffective, and at worst harmful [13]. Alternative communication strategies are urgently needed to mitigate social polarization and its violent consequences. They may build on democratic health communication approaches which have been shown efficient during the pandemic [33]. In addition, an increased policy effort to improve feelings of safety and mental wellbeing in young populations during the COVID-19 pandemic is warranted. This may be done through a public health approach targeting social determinants of youth’s wellbeing through their community environments at school or at work. Such efforts should, for instance, aim to mitigate pandemic-related financial uncertainties, decrease the social isolation stemming from social distancing and confinement measures and legitimate the collective discontent associated with the growing inequities revealed by the pandemic. Furthermore, an effort in terms of digital literacy and an increase in visibility and availability of psychological support in the community, i.e., an increased presence of health care services, may be helpful. With the end of the pandemic still not in sight, phenomena of conspiracy theories and VR, which began prior to the COVID-19 virus, should be a priority and be monitored to maintain a proactive prevention agenda rather than a reactive crisis resolution stance, which unfortunately too often prevails in the field of interpersonal violence.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/article/10.3390/ijerph18157846/s1, Table S1: Multivariate regressions of RIS, Table S2: Moderation of the association between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and RIS by psychological distress.

Author Contributions

All authors have approved the submitted version and agrees to be personally accountable for the author’s own contributions and for ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work. Conceptualization, A.L. and C.R.; Methodology, A.L., D.M., C.R. and R.L.F.; Formal Analysis, A.L.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, A.L.; Writing—Review & Editing, A.L., D.M., R.L.F. and Z.L.; Supervision, C.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by FQRSC (Fonds Québécois de Recherche en Santé et Culture) grant # 2017-SE-196373, and RAPS (Équipe de Recherche et Action sur les Polarisations Sociales) grant # 180645.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of McGill University Faculty of Medicine (protocol code A09-B69-20A, approval granted 29 September 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available from the authors upon request.

Acknowledgments

The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Schmid, A.P. Radicalisation, De-radicalisation, Counter-radicalisation: A Conceptual Discussion and Literature Review. ICCT Res. Pap. 2013, 97, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Wynia, M.K.; Eisenman, D.; Hanfling, D. Ideologically Motivated Violence: A Public Health Approach to Prevention. Am. J. Public Health 2017, 107, 1244–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. The Fascist Threat. Available online: https://ajph.aphapublications.org/doi/full/10.2105/AJPH.2021.306169 (accessed on 2 February 2021).
  4. Krieger, N. Enough: COVID-19, Structural Racism, Police Brutality, Plutocracy, Climate Change—and Time for Health Justice, Democratic Governance, and an Equitable, Sustainable Future. Am. J. Public Health. 2020, 110, 1620–1623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ozer, S. Globalization and radicalization: A cross-national study of local embeddedness and reactions to cultural globalization in regard to violent extremism. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2020, 76, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Marone, F. Hate in the time of coronavirus: Exploring the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on violent extremism and terrorism in the West. Secur. J. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Macklin, G. Coronavirus and the Far Right: Seizing the Moment? Instituto per gli Studi di Politica Internazionale: Milano, Italy, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  8. Marinthe, G.; Brown, G.; Delouvée, S.; Jolley, D. Looking out for myself: Exploring the relationship between conspiracy mentality, perceived personal risk, and COVID-19 prevention measures. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2020, 25, 957–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Romer, D.; Jamieson, K.H. Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the U.S. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020, 263, 113356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Earnshaw, V.A.; Eaton, L.; Kalichman, S.C.; Brousseau, N.M.; Hill, E.C.; Fox, A.B. COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support. Transl. Behav. Med. 2020, 10, 850–856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Rozbroj, T.; Lyons, A.; Lucke, J. Psychosocial and demographic characteristics relating to vaccine attitudes in Australia. Patient Educ. Couns. 2019, 102, 172–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Jolley, D.; Paterson, J.L. Pylons ablaze: Examining the role of 5G COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2020, 59, 628–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Antecedents and consequeneces of COVID-19 conspiracy theories: A rapid review of the evidence. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/u8yah (accessed on 2 February 2021).
  14. Cénat, J.M.; Blais-Rochette, C.; Kokou-Kpolou, C.K.; Noorishad, P.-G.; Mukunzi, J.N.; McIntee, S.-E.; Dalexis, R.D.; Goulet, M.-A.; Labelle, P.R. Prevalence of symptoms of depression, anxiety, insomnia, posttraumatic stress disorder, and psychological distress among populations affected by the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2021, 295, 113599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cénat, J.M.; Dalexis, R.D.; Kokou-Kpolou, C.K.; Mukunzi, J.N.; Rousseau, C. Social inequalities and collateral damages of the COVID-19 pandemic: When basic needs challenge mental health care. Int. J. Public Health 2020, 65, 717–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Luna, S. Affective Atmospheres of Terror on the Mexico–U.S. Border: Rumors of Violence in Reynosa’s Prostitution Zone. Cult. Anthropol. 2018, 33, 58–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Uscinski, J.E.; Enders, A.M.; Klofstad, C.A.; Seelig, M.I.; Funchion, J.R.; Everett, C.; Wuchty, S.; Premaratne, K.; Murthi, M.N. Why do people believe COVID-19 conspiracy theories? Harv. Kennedy Sch. MisInf. Rev. 2020, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. van Prooijen, J.-W.; Douglas, K.M. Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis situations. Mem. Stud. 2017, 10, 323–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Miconi, D.; Oulhote, Y.; Hassan, G.; Rousseau, C. Sympathy for violent radicalization among college students in Quebec (Canada): The protective role of a positive future orientation. Psychol. Violence 2020, 10, 344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Rousseau, C.; Hassan, G.; Miconi, D.; Lecompte, V.; Mekki-Berrada, A.; El Hage, H.; Oulhote, Y. From social adversity to sympathy for violent radicalization: The role of depression, religiosity and social support. Arch. Public Health 2019, 77, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Misiak, B.; Samochowiec, J.; Bhui, K.; Schouler-Ocak, M.; Demunter, H.; Kuey, L.; Raballo, A.; Gorwood, P.; Frydecka, D.; Dom, G. A systematic review on the relationship between mental health, radicalization and mass violence. Eur. Psychiatry 2019, 56, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Freeman, D.; Waite, F.; Rosebrock, L.; Petit, A.; Causier, C.; East, A.; Jenner, L.; Teale, A.-L.; Carr, L.; Mulhall, S.; et al. Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs, mistrust, and compliance with government guidelines in England. Psychol. Med. 2020, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Bhui, K.; Warfa, N.; Jones, E. Is violent radicalisation associated with poverty, migration, poor self-reported health and common mental disorders? PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e90718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Frounfelker, R.L.; Frissen, T.; Miconi, D.; Lawson, J.; Brennan, R.T.; d’Haenens, L.; Rousseau, C. Transnational evaluation of the Sympathy for Violent Radicalization Scale: Mesuring population attitudes towards violent radicalization among young adults in two countries. Transcult. Psychiatry 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Moskalenko, S.; McCauley, C. Measuring political mobilization: The distinction between activism and radicalism. Terror. Political Violence 2009, 21, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Mollica, R.F.; Caspi-Yavin, Y.; Bollini, P.; Truong, T.; Tor, S.; Lavelle, J. The Harvard trauma questionnaire: Validating a cross-cultural instrument for measuring torture, trauma, and post-traumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1992, 180, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Moum, T. Mode of administration and interviewer effects in self-reported symptoms of anxiety and depression. Soc. Indic. Res. 1998, 45, 279–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. van Buuren, S.; Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. J. Stat. Softw. 2011, 45, 1–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Tessler, H.; Choi, M.; Kao, G. The anxiety of being Asian American: Hate crimes and negative biases during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. J. Crim. Justice 2020, 45, 636–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Emmelkamp, J.; Asscher, J.J.; Wissink, I.B.; Stams, G.J.J.M. Risk factors for (violent) radicalization in juveniles: A multilevel meta-analysis. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2020, 55, 101489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. van Gelder, M.M.; Bretveld, R.W.; Roeleveld, N. Web-based questionnaires: The future in epidemiology? Am. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 172, 1292–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Wright, K.B. Researching Internet-based populations: Advantages and disadvantages of online survey research, online questionnaire authoring software packages, and web survey services. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 2005, 10, JCMC1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Tworek, H.; Beacock, I.; Ojo, E. Democratic Health Communications during Covid-19: A RAPID Response; UBC Centre for the Study of Democratic Institutions: Vancouver, BC, Canada, 2020. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Radicalization leading to violence, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress in relational context.
Figure 1. Radicalization leading to violence, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress in relational context.
Ijerph 18 07846 g001
Figure 2. Psychological distress as moderator of the association between sympathy for violent radicalization and endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories.
Figure 2. Psychological distress as moderator of the association between sympathy for violent radicalization and endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories.
Ijerph 18 07846 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participants.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participants.
Variablen (%)
Self-reported gender
Woman3292 (54.8%)
Man2646 (44.1%)
Gender-diverse30 (0.5%)
Missing35 (0.6%)
Psychological distress
≤1.752441 (40.7%)
>1.752974 (49.5%)
Missing588 (9.8%)
City
Montreal2000 (33.3%)
Calgary1002 (16.7%)
Edmonton1000 (16.7%)
Toronto2001 (33.3%)
Financial problems
Not at all1963 (32.70%)
A little2184 (36.4%)
Moderate896 (14.9%)
A lot769 (12.8%)
Missing191 (3.2%)
Education
High school or less1267 (21.1%)
Apprenticeship, technical institute, trade or vocational school, college, CEGEP or other non-university certificate or diploma,1741 (29.0%)
University certificate, diploma or degree2892 (48.2%)
Missing103 (1.7%)
Immigration status
First generation1454 (24.2%)
Second generation1577 (26.3%)
Third generation or more2872 (47.8%)
Missing100 (1.7%)
mean (SD) min, max, % missing
Age26.72 (4.53) 18.00, 35.00, 0.0%
Psychological distress2.00 (.79) 1.00, 4.00, 9.8%
Endorsement of conspiracy theories8.78 (4.87) 4.00, 20.00, 7.7%
Sympathy for violent radicalisation (SyfoR)23.72 (13.86) 8.00, 56.00, 7.9%
Radicalism Intention Scale (RIS)13.87 (7.40) 4.00, 28.00, 9.3%
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of study variables: SyfoR, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of study variables: SyfoR, endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress.
SyfoR,
Total Score
Endorsement of
COVID-19 Conspiracy
Theories, Total Score
Psychological Distress,
Mean Score
VariableMean (SD)p-Valueη2Mean (SD)p-Valueη2Mean (SD)p-Valueη2
Self-reported gender <0.00010.04 <0.00010.02 0.010.002
Woman21.29 (12.42) 8.24 (4.49) 2.00 (0.71)
Man26.62 (14.96) 9.45 (5.23) 1.99 (0.87)
Gender-diverse30.42 (10.19) 6.21 (3.31) 2.43 (0.74)
Age <0.00010.01 <0.00010.004 <0.00010.02
18–2525.81 (13.84) 9.17 (4.92) 2.15 (0.82)
26–3522.41 (13.72) 8.53 (4.83) 1.91 (0.75)
City of residence <0.00010.01 <0.00010.03 <0.00010.03
Calgary23.66 (12.80) 8.32 (4.57) 1.90 (0.73)
Edmonton24.77 (13.62) 8.90 (4.96) 2.05 (0.82)
Montreal25.11 (15.91) 9.90 (5.31) 2.16 (0.87)
Toronto21.77 (11.86) 7.80 (4.23) 1.86 (0.68)
Financial problems <0.00010.10 <0.00010.13 <0.00010.25
Not at all19.51 (11.14) 7.16 (3.89) 1.61 (0.58)
A little22.67 (12.56) 8.26 (4.32) 1.95 (0.66)
Moderate28.33 (14.64) 10.38 (5.05) 2.37 (0.78)
A lot32.46 (17.20) 12.47 (5.86) 2.78 (0.86)
Education <0.00010.02 <0.00010.06 <0.00010.05
High school or less23.89 (12.26) 8.95 (4.56) 2.08 (0.76)
Apprenticeship, technical institute, trade or vocational school, college, CEGEP or other non-university certificate or diploma26.80 (15.68) 10.46 (5.26) 2.23 (0.88)
University certificate, diploma or degree21.78 (13.02) 7.69 (4.47) 1.84 (0.70)
Immigrant status <0.00010.02 <0.00010.01 <0.00010.04
First generation21.03 (12.48) 8.44 (4.30) 1.80 (0.66)
Second generation23.21 (12.13) 8.16 (4.48) 1.91 (0.72)
Third generation or more25.36 (15.11) 9.27 (5.28) 2.15 (0.85)
Table 3. Multivariate regressions of support for violent radicalization (SyfoR) on endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress, with and without a conspiracy theory * psychological distress interaction term.
Table 3. Multivariate regressions of support for violent radicalization (SyfoR) on endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychological distress, with and without a conspiracy theory * psychological distress interaction term.
β95% CIp-Valueβ95% CIp-Value
Intercept6.684.458.91<0.000117.614.4520.74<0.0001
Endorsement of COVID-19 consp. theories0.880.800.96<0.0001−0.21−0.420.010.06
Psychological distress (mean score)6.325.776.86<0.00011.100.122.080.03
Self-reported gender (ref = woman)
Man4.383.745.02<0.00013.662.984.35<0.0001
Gender-diverse7.453.0611.840.017.983.6312.330.0004
Age−0.29−0.36−0.22<0.0001−0.29−0.36−0.22<0.0001
City (ref = Montreal)
Calgary2.181.273.09<0.00012.551.633.47<0.0001
Edmonton1.760.882.650.00012.031.172.90<0.0001
Toronto1.390.652.140.00031.700.962.44<0.0001
Financial problems (ref = Not at all)
A little0.10−0.660.860.790.42−0.311.150.26
Moderate0.93−0.051.900.061.210.242.170.01
A lot0.39−0.821.610.52−0.33−1.560.900.59
Education (ref = High school or less)
Apprenticeship, Tech. school or vocational school, college, CEGEP or other non-university cert. or diploma1.550.712.390.00031.020.201.840.02
University cert., diploma or degree2.181.382.98<0.00011.420.622.220.0005
Immigration (ref = 3rd generation or more)
1st generation−1.54−2.33−0.750.0002−1.23−2.00−0.450.002
2nd generation−0.04−0.750.660.910.13−0.560.820.71
Interaction Endorsement of COVID-19 consp. theories * Psychol. distress 0.490.400.57<0.0001
Note. CI: confidence interval.
Table 4. Moderation of the association between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and SyfoR by psychological distress.
Table 4. Moderation of the association between endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracy theories and SyfoR by psychological distress.
OutcomeModerator (Level)Estimate95% CIp-Value
SyfoRPsychological distress (≤1.75)0.470.35–0.59<0.0001
Psychological distress (>1.75)1.361.26–1.46<0.0001
Note. CI: confidence interval.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Levinsson, A.; Miconi, D.; Li, Z.; Frounfelker, R.L.; Rousseau, C. Conspiracy Theories, Psychological Distress, and Sympathy for Violent Radicalization in Young Adults during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7846. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18157846

AMA Style

Levinsson A, Miconi D, Li Z, Frounfelker RL, Rousseau C. Conspiracy Theories, Psychological Distress, and Sympathy for Violent Radicalization in Young Adults during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(15):7846. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18157846

Chicago/Turabian Style

Levinsson, Anna, Diana Miconi, Zhiyin Li, Rochelle L. Frounfelker, and Cécile Rousseau. 2021. "Conspiracy Theories, Psychological Distress, and Sympathy for Violent Radicalization in Young Adults during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 15: 7846. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph18157846

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop