Next Article in Journal
Microbiome as Mediator of Diet on Colorectal Cancer Risk: The Role of Vitamin D, Markers of Inflammation and Adipokines
Previous Article in Journal
The Immunopathogenesis of Alzheimer’s Disease Is Related to the Composition of Gut Microbiota
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Most Effective Combination of Nutraceuticals for Improved Memory and Cognitive Performance in the House Cricket, Acheta domesticus

by
Samskruthi Madireddy
1,* and
Sahithi Madireddy
2
1
Independent Researcher, 1353 Tanaka Drive, San Jose, CA 95131, USA
2
Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 23 December 2020 / Revised: 22 January 2021 / Accepted: 22 January 2021 / Published: 25 January 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Nutrition and Metabolism)

Abstract

:
Background: Dietary intake of multivitamins, zinc, polyphenols, omega fatty acids, and probiotics have all shown benefits in learning, spatial memory, and cognitive function. It is important to determine the most effective combination of antioxidants and/or probiotics because regular ingestion of all nutraceuticals may not be practical. This study examined various combinations of nutrients to determine which may best enhance spatial memory and cognitive performance in the house cricket (Acheta domesticus (L.)). Methods: Based on the 31 possible combinations of multivitamins, zinc, polyphenols, omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs), and probiotics, 128 house crickets were divided into one control group and 31 experimental groups with four house crickets in each group. Over eight weeks, crickets were fed their respective nutrients, and an Alternation Test and Recognition Memory Test were conducted every week using a Y-maze to test spatial working memory. Results: The highest-scoring diets shared by both tests were the combination of multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 fatty acids (VitZncPuf; Alternation: slope = 0.07226, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.07001), the combination of probiotics, polyphenols, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs (ProPolVitZncPuf; Alternation: slope = 0.07182, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.07001), the combination of probiotics, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs (ProVitZncPuf; Alternation: slope = 0.06999, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.07001), and the combination of polyphenols, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs (PolVitZncPuf; Alternation: slope = 0.06873, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.06956). Conclusion: All of the nutrient combinations demonstrated a benefit over the control diet, but the most significant improvement compared to the control was found in the VitZncPuf, ProVitZncPuf, PolVitZncPuf, and ProPolVitZncPuf. Since this study found no significant difference between the performance and improvement of subjects within these four groups, the combination of multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 fatty acids (VitZncPuf) was concluded to be the most effective option for improving memory and cognitive performance.

1. Introduction

The healthcare community increasingly acknowledges the role of social determinants of health in driving health disparities. Food insecurity is one such factor that contributes to nutritional deficits, affecting both physical and mental health [1,2]. In 2016, about 11.5% of adults and 17.5% of children in the United States lived in households facing food insecurity [3]. This food insecurity carries a risk of many health issues common in the United States [4]. Access to nutritious food is important because evidence suggests that diet can play a significant role in cognition via the gut–brain axis, which is the bidirectional communication between the gut and the brain [5]. Moreover, certain foods are known to contain nutrients that can slow down cognitive decline or improve cognitive performance.
Individually, multivitamins, zinc, polyphenols, omega-3 fatty acids, and probiotics are shown to support brain function either through the gut–brain axis or through antioxidant functions [6,7,8,9,10]. However, the effect of various combinations of these nutrients on cognition and spatial memory remains unexplored. Knowing the most effective combination of antioxidants and/or probiotics is important, as regular ingestion of all nutraceuticals may not be practical. Finding the combination of nutrients that best enhances cognitive performance is crucial to developing strategies for improved learning and cognition in humans. This study examined the combinations of multivitamins, zinc, polyphenols, omega-3 fatty acids, and probiotics that best enhanced memory and cognitive performance in the house cricket.

2. Effect of Nutraceuticals

2.1. Polyphenols

Polyphenol intake has consistently shown benefits in various aspects of memory and learning [6,11]. Dietary polyphenols have been linked to greater cognitive evolution, as well as improvements in language and verbal memory [12,13]. These polyphenols are generally secondary plant metabolites with antioxidative properties. The antioxidant effects of polyphenols are important because the imbalance between antioxidants and reactive oxygen species (ROS) leads to oxidative stress [14,15,16]. The brain is particularly vulnerable to oxidative stress because of its high metabolic demand [17,18]. Because ROS are highly reactive, they frequently damage macromolecules, which can lead to mitochondrial dysfunction and ultimately neurodegeneration [19,20]. However, polyphenols have well-documented antioxidant and anti-inflammatory effects [21,22,23]. Polyphenols can protect against oxidative damage by scavenging free radicals and deactivating metals used in ROS generation [24,25,26]. They can also activate antioxidant enzymes, decrease peroxide levels, and repair membranes that have suffered oxidative damage [27].

2.2. Probiotics

In addition to polyphenols, probiotics can also improve cognitive function and spatial memory [9,28]. Probiotics refer to bacteria that support health by changing the composition of the gut microbiome [29]. This, in turn, influences brain health via the gut–brain axis [5,30]. Probiotics and gut microbes can affect brain physiology through their influence on cytokine levels [31]. Changes in the gut microbiota are communicated to the brain through the vagus nerve and through levels of dietary tryptophan, a serotonin precursor [32,33]. Dietary changes are one established method of altering gut microbiota populations [34]. Probiotics are capable of regulating the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis [35], which is involved in the stress response, and changing levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which plays an important role in learning and memory [36].

2.3. Multivitamins

Similarly, multivitamins, especially vitamins A, C, and E, have been shown to benefit cognition and spatial memory as well [37,38,39]. Vitamins have been linked to better cognitive health, particularly in free recall memory [40,41]. Vitamins A, C, B group, and E can act as antioxidants by scavenging free radicals and preventing oxidative stress [42,43]. Vitamin C, in particular, is highly concentrated in the brain, where it promotes neuronal, vascular, and neurotransmitter function [44]. Decreased vitamin B12, B6, and folate have also been linked to cognitive decline associated with aging [45,46,47]. Moreover, maternal B12 levels have been linked to the cognitive function of their offspring [48]. Taking B vitamins was shown to be beneficial for the cognitive function of people without dementia as well [49]. Vitamin D is a steroid hormone with many functions that take effect after binding a receptor in the nucleus [50,51]. Vitamin D levels have also been linked to brain functioning [52]. This may be due to its role in neuroinflammation, which is involved in cognitive decline and neurodegeneration accompanying aging [53].

2.4. Omega-3 Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids

In addition, omega-3 PUFAs are important in neural function and they play a critical role as both energy substrates and cell membrane components [8,54,55]. They also protect against oxidative stress, inflammation, and apoptosis while mitigating the activity of neurotrophic factors [8]. Omega-3 PUFAs are essential in cellular function, as well as the development of cognition, learning, and memory [56,57]. Likewise, omega-3 fatty acids have been associated with increased relational memory, which is dependent on hippocampal brain activity [58]. Omega-3 fatty acids, including docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), have also been shown to improve synaptic plasticity, membrane fluidity, and neuronal metabolism [59,60,61,62]. PUFAs are involved in regulating glucose levels, feeding, neurotransmission, emotions, apoptosis, and neuroinflammation [63,64]. They also assist in behavior and cognitive development [65]. Fatty acids play an additional role in protecting against neuroinflammation and neuron death [66]. Therefore, neuronal function and integrity depend on adequate omega-3 PUFA levels. DHA, in particular, has been implied to play a role in preserving the health of aging neurons [67]. These effects may be through DHA’s ability to change the expression of genes that regulate neurogenesis and neuron function.

2.5. Zinc

Another nutrient, zinc, is crucial for memory formation and learning [68]. Zinc is necessary in forming synapses and in mediating structural plasticity; this activity potentially allows zinc to modulate the function of the hippocampus in memory [69]. Zinc is critical for cognitive development, since it is involved in neuronal migration and it regulates neurogenesis and differentiation [70,71]. In addition to its functions within glia and neurons, zinc also affects neurotransmission. Zinc levels in the brain are largely protected from zinc deficiencies in the diet since homeostasis is maintained by the blood–brain barrier and the blood–cerebrospinal fluid barrier [72]. Zinc deficiencies can affect attention, behavior, and motor development [73,74]. Animal studies have shown that psychological stress reduces serum zinc levels, implying that zinc deficiencies and gut inflammation are linked to stress [75]. Zinc is also essential in the general development and function of the central nervous system (CNS) [68]. Long-term administration of zinc sulfate in rats enhanced learning, spatial memory, and exploratory activity [10]. In addition to improving spatial working memory, zinc supplements in rats were also found to improve recognition memory [76]. Zinc might also affect memory formation through its ability to regulate glutamate signaling [76]. Additionally, zinc ions are highly concentrated in the hippocampus and, thus, play a key role in modulating spatial learning and memory [77,78].

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Animals

Insects make suitable model organisms because they are less expensive, easier to maintain in bulk, and possess simpler nervous systems than vertebrates; however, they still share fundamental neurobiology and behavior with vertebrates [79]. Studies in insects have previously yielded significant insights about the fundamental processes behind learning and memory [80]. Moreover, studies using crickets have shown that mushroom bodies responsible for memory and olfactory learning in their brains continue neurogenesis into adulthood in response to sensory input [81,82,83]. This adult neurogenesis may be related to mechanisms of learning and memory in invertebrates and possibly vertebrates [84]. Crickets are useful as a model organism because they can retain an olfactory memory throughout their entire lives and modify it readily in response to experience [85]. Crickets are also a good model for nutritional effects. For example, one study used crickets to identify how protein and carbohydrate levels influenced weight, muscle mass, and fat reserves [86]. Crickets have been widely used within behavioral tests [87,88,89]. For example, one study used crickets with a plus-shaped maze to examine predator-induced stress and found that the crickets showed consistent behavioral responses in their tests [90]. Another study used the Y-maze with crickets to assess the role of thorax temperature during mate choice [91].
A total of 128 1-week-old house crickets were used in this study. Crickets were housed in Y-mazes made with 12″ × 2″ × 2″ (L × H × W) inexpensive rectangular plastic tubes from Cleartec Packaging, Inc. (Park Hills, MO, USA) at angles of 120° relative to each other in a Y shape, leaving a triangular space (center zone) between the three tubes (Figure 1). Y-mazes were used because they are suitable for behavioral tests that do not induce significant stress to the crickets compared to other memory tests with apparatuses such as water mazes.
Small holes were drilled into the sides of the Y-mazes for aeration. Throughout the experiment, one house cricket was placed in the bottom arm of each Y-maze under a 6:18 light/dark schedule with a constant temperature of 75 °F. Crickets had constant access to food and water (in a gel form to protect crickets from drowning).

3.2. Nutrient Treatments

Multivitamins, zinc, polyphenols, omega-3 fatty acids, and probiotics were used in this study. All of these nutrients have been demonstrated to have a positive impact on memory and cognition when consumed independently.

3.2.1. Multivitamins

Crickets were given one serving every two weeks, each serving including vitamin A (1200 μg), vitamin B1 (2.5 mg), vitamin B2 (2.5 mg), vitamin B3 (20 mg), vitamin B6 (3 mg), vitamin C (100 mg), vitamin D3 (10 μg), vitamin E (20 mg), and vitamin K1 (80 μg).

3.2.2. Zinc

Crickets were given one serving every two weeks, each serving including zinc sulphate (ZnSO4) (220 mg).

3.2.3. Polyphenols

Crickets were given one serving every two weeks, each serving (2.37 g) including turmeric extract (Curcuma longa), bitter orange, grape extract, organic decaf tea (leaf extract), olive extract, noni, pomegranate extract, hawthorn berry powder, apple extract, alfalfa, quercetin dihydrate, Aronia, acai (Euterpe oleracea), blueberry extract, and celery extract.

3.2.4. Omega-3 Fatty Acids

Crickets were given one serving every two weeks, each serving including Omega-3 Phospholipid Peptide Complex (292 mg).

3.2.5. Probiotics

Crickets were given one serving every two weeks, each serving including 50 billion colony forming units (CFUs) with 11 live bacterial strains: Lactobacillus rhamnosus, Lactobacillus acidophilus, Lactobacillus casei, Lactobacillus salivarius, Lactobacillus plantarum, Lactobacillus paracasei, Bifidobacterium longum, Bifidobacterium bifidum, Bifidobacterium lactis, Bifidobacterium breve, and Streptococcus thermophilus.

3.3. Groups

Based on the 31 possible combinations of multivitamins (Vit), zinc (Znc), polyphenols (Pol), omega-3 PUFAs (Puf), and probiotics (Pro), the 128 house crickets were divided into one control group and 31 experimental groups with four house crickets in each group (Table 1). Of the 128 house crickets, 124 (31 groups) were fed with various combinations of nutrients demonstrated to have a positive effect on cognitive performance, while the remaining four house crickets were fed with a normal diet (control group). Sufficient quantities of food and water were available for all house crickets.

3.4. Spatial Memory Testing

Spatial memory refers to the ability to memorize and recall locations and spaces, which is useful during navigation. With its simple, three-armed design, the Y-maze has traditionally been effective in evaluating spatial memory. In this experiment, Y-mazes were used to conduct weekly Alternation Tests and Recognition Memory Tests to assess spatial working memory.

3.4.1. Habituation and Practice Sessions

During the one week of habituation, crickets were kept in the home arm of the Y-maze with the other arms blocked. This allowed crickets to become more comfortable in their environments. Within the home arm, crickets had access to water and normal food. At the end of the habituation period, the food and water were removed from the home arm and crickets were starved for one day and night in order to motivate them to collect the food from other arms of the Y-maze during the practice phase. After habituation was completed, crickets underwent practice sessions to ensure that they could collect food rewards from arms of the maze before testing and supplementation. During the week, crickets had two practice sessions with three trials, in which crickets collected a food reward from an open goal arm (no entry was provided to the alternate arm). The open goal arm was pseudo-randomly varied between trials in each practice session to prevent a preference for either one.
Immediately after completion of the one-week practice period, crickets were kept in the home arm of the Y-maze with the other arms blocked and fed according to their prescribed diet. Testing occurred twice a week, alternating between two tests that assess spatial memory using the Y-maze: the Alternation Test and the Recognition Memory Test. Testing continued throughout the feeding process to examine differences in the development of spatial memory over time with the nutrients provided, starting from a baseline established during the first week. The night before testing, crickets were slightly food-deprived so that they were motivated to explore the arms of the Y-maze.

3.4.2. Alternation Test

During the Alternation Test, crickets were allowed to explore all three arms of the Y-maze. Alternations were counted when the cricket explored a different arm each successive time in a set of three arm visits, such as arm 1 → arm 2 → arm 3. If the cricket explored an arm twice in the set of three arm visits, such as arm 1 → arm 2 → arm 1, this was not counted as an alternation (Figure 2). The Alternation Test is dependent on the fact that crickets habitually explore their least-recently visited location due to natural curiosity, which relies on their working memory of where they have previously visited. Each test consisted of six sets of three arm visits (18 arm visits total).

3.4.3. Recognition Memory Test

During the Recognition Memory Test, crickets were given access to only one of arms 2 or 3 (for example, arm 2) of the Y-maze, where the food was placed (Figure 3). This arm was alternated among the six trials during each testing to avoid creating a bias for one arm. After obtaining the food, crickets were returned to arm 1 (the home arm), where they were sequestered for two minutes. During this time, food was placed in the unvisited arm (arm 3 in this example). After the two minutes were over, all arms were opened, and the crickets were able to freely explore the arm they have not visited, in which they would find the food. Thus, this test evaluated spatial memory by demonstrating whether the crickets had a recollection of which arm they had previously visited.

3.5. Potential Bias

During the practice sessions, a possible “path bias” was observed affecting the crickets’ movement through the Y-maze. Crickets tended to travel along one edge of an arm, forming a path along that edge to one of the other two arms (Figure 4). For example, a cricket moving along the left side of the home arm would often follow that edge into the left arm (arm 2). An apparatus was constructed to overcome this “path bias”. Rectangular blocks of sponge (1.5″ × 2″ × 0.625″ L × H × W) were placed at either side of the arm just before the center zone, creating a narrow path (1.5″ × 2″ × 0.75″ L × H × W). Thus, even when the crickets followed one edge within their arm, they ultimately had to move to the center of the arm as they neared the center zone. This minimized path bias so that once each cricket reached the center zone, it had to explicitly choose which arm to explore.

3.6. Statistical Analysis

All data were analyzed using the Prism 8 data analysis program (GraphPad Software Inc., San Diego, CA, USA).

4. Results

In both the Recognition Test (Figure 5) and the Alternation Test (Figure 6), all groups exhibited improvement in performance over time. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA showed that in the final results there was a significant main effect of diet on performance in both tests, as measured by the number of correct arm choices or alternations (p < 0.0001). Tukey tests were run along with the ANOVA to further examine differences among the means of individual combinations at the final trial. In the Alternation Test, the mean final performance of the Control varied significantly from the groups VitZncPuf, ProVitZncPuf, and ProPolVitZncPuf (p = 0.0194). There was no significant difference among these three groups. Similarly, in the Recognition Memory Test, the mean final performance of the Control varied significantly from the groups VitZncPuf, ProVitZncPuf, PolVitZncPuf, and ProPolVitZncPuf (p = 0.0194). There was no significant difference among these four groups.
Further, the improvement of each group (final score-baseline score) was computed, and an ordinary one-way ANOVA was used to evaluate variance among these values for both the Recognition Test (Figure 7) and the Alternation Test (Figure 8). While the Control’s improvement differed significantly from many experimental groups in both tests, the most significant differences in the Alternation Test were between the Control and the groups VitZncPuf, ProVitZncPuf, and ProPolVitZncPuf (p < 0.0001). The most significant differences in the Recognition Memory Test were between the Control and the groups VitZncPuf, ProPolZnc, ProVitZncPuf, PolVitZncPuf, and ProPolVitZncPuf (p < 0.0001). In both tests, there was no significant difference among improvement scores within these winning groups.
Figure 9 shows the mean number of correct arm choices of each group in the initial and final Recognition Memory Test since starting their specific diets. Figure 10 similarly shows the mean number of successful alternations of each group in the initial and final Alternation Test since starting their specific diets.
Regression analysis was conducted to determine the relative magnitude of improvement over time, represented by the slope produced. Figure 11 and Figure 12 show the lines generated through regression analysis for each group in the Recognition Memory Test and the Alternation Test, respectively. In the Recognition Memory Test, the slopes were also significantly nonzero (p = 0.0005 for control group, p < 0.0001 for experimental groups) and significantly different from one another (p < 0.0001). In the Alternation Test, the slopes were significantly nonzero (p = 0.0025 for control group, p < 0.0001 for experimental groups) and significantly different from one another (p < 0.0001).
Table 2 shows the ranking of diets in order of their slope for both tests. Similar to the findings of the previous ANOVAs, the highest scoring diets shared by both tables were VitZncPuf (Alternation: slope = 0.07226, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.07001), ProPolVitZncPuf (Alternation: slope = 0.07182, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.07001), ProVitZncPuf (Alternation: slope = 0.06999, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.07001), and PolVitZncPuf (Alternation: slope = 0.06873, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.06956). The Control ranked the lowest in both tests (Alternation: slope = 0.02205, Recognition Memory: slope = 0.02590). Overall, regression analysis showed that the slopes were significantly nonzero and statistically different.

5. Discussion

This study tested the hypothesis that the combination of multivitamins, zinc, polyphenols, omega-3 fatty acids, and probiotics would best enhance spatial memory and cognitive performance. The results revealed that crickets consistently demonstrated more improvement in memory tests when fed nutrient-rich diets compared to crickets fed a normal diet, suggesting that these nutrients may indeed play a role in improving memory. While all of the nutrient combinations showed a benefit over the normal diet, the most significant improvement compared to the control was found in the VitZncPuf group (fed multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs), ProVitZncPuf group (fed probiotics, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs), PolVitZncPuf group (fed polyphenols, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs), and ProPolVitZncPuf group (fed probiotics, polyphenols, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs). These four groups were also ranked highest in improvement based on linear regression analysis, with VitZncPuf group ranking only slightly higher than the ProVitZncPuf group, PolVitZncPuf group, and ProPolVitZncPuf group. However, as the post hoc Tukey test found no significant differences in the performance of subjects within these four groups (VitZncPuf, ProVitZncPuf, PolVitZncPuf, and ProPolVitZncPuf), the combination of multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs may be the most efficient option for improving memory, producing the greatest results with the least number of distinct nutrients. These results did not entirely support the experimental hypothesis, because although ProPolVitZncPuf was effective, its effects were not significantly higher than that of VitZncPuf, making multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs a potential winning combination.
Though previous studies did not focus on how combinations of nutrients interact to affect cognition, these results are consistent with findings regarding individual nutrients from this study’s winning combination (multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs). The current findings are supported by reports that prenatal and postnatal zinc supplementation in rats enhanced spatial learning, cognition, and locomotion [10]. Zinc deficiency has also been implicated in cognitive impairment, with improved learning and memory achieved following zinc supplementation [77,92]. Zinc is known to be concentrated in the CNS, particularly in the hippocampus, where it is involved in synaptic transmission [10]. These findings are further supported by another study of rats that developed zinc deficiencies following 145 days of a low-zinc diet, after which they received zinc-enriched yeast for 55 days. Although the zinc-deficient rats had impaired learning and memory, these effects were mitigated through administration of zinc-enriched yeast [93]. However, it remains unknown whether these findings can be observed in humans as well, as a study of 602 children found that administration of 30 mg of zinc for six months had little effect on improving cognition [94].
This study’s results are also consistent with findings that vitamin intake can benefit cognition in people without dementia [49]. A previous study also found significantly improved cognition in aged mice that received vitamin E and C for 60 days [95]. Another study observed cognitive improvements in 48 adults aged 65 who received multivitamins (vitamin B6, B12, and folic acid) for 12 weeks [96]. A study of 114 people with hyperhomocysteinemia also found that supplements of vitamin B6, B12, and folic acid improved cognitive function [97]. Another study using 32 house crickets examined the effects of combinations of polyphenols, probiotics, and multivitamins on spatial memory and cognitive performance and found that the combination of probiotics and multivitamins led to the most improvement [98]. Present results are also consistent with evidence that omega-3 fatty acids improve spatial memory [99,100,101]. Moreover, omega-3 fatty acids are known to regulate expression of several genes associated with apoptosis and oxidative stress [102]. Low dietary omega-3 PUFAs and low plasma DHA have also been found to lead to behavioral deficits and low omega-3 PUFA levels in the brain [103].
The subjects in this study were limited to house crickets as crickets have been established as the best insect model to investigate learning and memory [104]. While data on the time that crickets took to travel through the Y-maze may be useful in analyzing memory, this study did not include these measurements, due to the crickets’ inconsistent pauses during their exploration of the arms of the Y-maze. There were no outstanding risks to the subjects, as the crickets were adequately fed with necessary nutrients. No physical stress was applied to the subjects throughout the study. Quencher was used as the water source to maintain cleanliness and protect the crickets from drowning in water when they were young. The National Institutes of Health (NIH) Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (8th Edition) was followed during testing.
Future investigations may replicate this experiment with mice using a Morris water maze. Other nutrients, such as iron or fiber, may also be tested, which could reveal an even more effective combination of dietary supplements to improve cognitive performance. Further investigations may also examine combinations of individual vitamins within the multivitamin cocktail used in this study. In addition, future research may directly study the effect of nutrition on human cognitive performance, particularly during childhood learning. This may be examined through natural studies measuring nutrition and performance and evaluating correlations between these factors and income level. Such studies could illustrate the effect of socioeconomic disparities on nutrition and learning, ultimately pointing to changes that can be made in mass nutrition.

6. Implications

These findings suggest potential ways of efficiently using nutrition in dietary strategies to support learning and cognition in humans. This research has implications for food insecurity, as many people lack access to nutritious foods that support learning and cognitive performance. This may be crucial in the context of childhood education, during which barriers to food access could affect learning and thus affect academic outcomes. This study may direct further research developing more detailed nutritional plans for supporting cognition, as well as informing larger-scope changes to be made in food access and mass nutrition to better support childhood learning.

7. Conclusions

Maintaining a nutritious diet necessary for health can be challenging for those facing poverty or food insecurity due to limited resources, stress, and competing priorities. Knowing how food can affect cognition informs strategies to protect and improve neuronal function through modifying diet and mass nutrition. This study investigated the best combination of nutrients for enhanced cognitive performance and memory in the house cricket. All of the nutrient combinations tested demonstrated a benefit over the control diet, but the most significant improvement compared to the control was found in the combination of multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 fatty acids (VitZncPuf), the combination of probiotics, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs (ProVitZncPuf), the combination of polyphenols, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs (PolVitZncPuf), and the combination of probiotics, polyphenols, multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 PUFAs (ProPolVitZncPuf). Since this study found no significant difference between the performance and improvement of subjects within these four groups, the combination of multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 fatty acids (VitZncPuf) was concluded to be the most effective option for improving memory and cognitive performance. While the group fed multivitamins, zinc, polyphenols, omega-3 fatty acids, and probiotics produced improvement, its improvement was not significantly higher than that of the group fed multivitamins, zinc, and omega-3 fatty acids. To strengthen or corroborate the findings of this study, future investigations may replicate it with mice using either a Y-Maze or a Morris water maze apparatus.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.M. (Samskruthi Madireddy) and S.M. (Sahithi Madireddy); methodology, S.M. (Samskruthi Madireddy) and S.M. (Sahithi Madireddy); data curation, S.M. (Samskruthi Madireddy); investigation, S.M. (Samskruthi Madireddy); validation, S.M. (Samskruthi Madireddy); visualization, S.M. (Sahithi Madireddy); writing—original draft preparation, S.M. (Samskruthi Madireddy) and S.M. (Sahithi Madireddy); writing—review and editing, S.M. (Sahithi Madireddy). Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gundersen, C.; Ziliak, J.P. Food insecurity and health outcomes. Health Aff. 2015, 34, 1830–1839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Shankar, P.; Chung, R.; Frank, D.A. Association of food insecurity with children’s behavioral, emotional, and academic outcomes: A systematic review. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2017, 38, 135–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Coleman-Jensen, A.; Rabbitt, M.P.; Gregory, C.A.; Singh, A. Household Food Security in the United States in 2016; Economic Research Report–237; Economic Research Service, U.S. Department of Agriculture: Washington, DC, USA, 2017. Available online: https://www.ers.usda.gov/webdocs/publications/84973/err-237.pdf (accessed on 25 January 2021).
  4. Gregory, C.A.; Coleman-Jensen, A. Food insecurity, Chronic Disease, and Health among Working-Age Adults; Economic Research Report–235; Economic Research Service, U.S. Department of Agriculture: Washington, DC, USA, 2017. Available online: https://www.ers.usda.gov/webdocs/publications/84467/err-235.pdf (accessed on 25 January 2021).
  5. Madireddy, S.; Madireddy, S. The role of diet in maintaining strong brain health by taking the advantage of the gut-brain axis. J. Food Nutr. Res. 2019, 7, 41–50. [Google Scholar]
  6. Figueira, I.; Menezes, R.; Macedo, D.; Costa, I.; dos Santos, C.N. Polyphenols beyond barriers: A glimpse into the brain. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2017, 15, 562–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Grima, N.A.; Pase, M.P.; Macpherson, H.; Pipingas, A. The effects of multivitamins on cognitive performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2012, 29, 561–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Lange, K.W.; Nakamura, Y.; Chen, N.; Guo, J.; Kanaya, S.; Lange, K.M.; Li, S. Diet and medical foods in Parkinson’s disease. Food Sci. Hum. Wellness 2019, 8, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Papalini, S.; Michels, F.; Kohn, N.; Wegman, J.; van Hemert, S.; Roelofs, K.; Arias-Vasquez, A.; Aarts, E. Stress matters: Randomized controlled trial on the effect of probiotics on neurocognition. Neurobiol. Stress 2019, 10, 100141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Piechal, A.; Blecharz-Klin, K.; Pyrzanowska, J.; Widy-Tyszkiewicz, E. Influence of long-term zinc administration on spatial learning and exploratory activity in rats. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 2016, 172, 408–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Rosli, H.; Shahar, S.; Din, N.C.; Haron, H.; Rajab, N.F. Prevalence of poor mental health and cognitive status among middle-aged adults and its predictors in relation to polyphenols intake. Malays. J. Med. Sci. 2019, 26, 72–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Letenneur, L.; Proust-Lima, C.; Le Gouge, A.; Dartigues, J.F.; Barberger-Gateau, P. Flavonoid intake and cognitive decline over a 10-year period. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 165, 1364–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Vauzour, D. Dietary polyphenols as modulators of brain functions: Biological actions and molecular mechanisms underpinning their beneficial effects. Oxidative Med. Cell. Longev. 2012, 2012, 914273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Madireddy, S.; Madireddy, S. Protection from the pathogenesis of neurodegenerative disorders, including Alzheimer’s disease, Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis, Huntington’s disease, and Parkinson’s diseases, through the mitigation of reactive oxygen species. J. Neurosci. Neurol. Disord. 2019, 3, 148–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Newsholme, P.; Cruzat, V.F.; Keane, K.N.; Carlessi, R.; de Bittencourt, P.I.H. Molecular mechanisms of ROS production and oxidative stress in diabetes. Biochem. J. 2016, 473, 4527–4550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Pizzino, G.; Irrera, N.; Cucinotta, M.; Pallio, G.; Mannino, F.; Arcoraci, V.; Squadrito, F.; Altavilla, D.; Bitto, A. Oxidative stress: Harms and benefits for human health. Oxidative Med. Cell. Longev. 2017, 2017, 8416763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hassan, W.; Silva, C.E.B.; Mohammadzai, I.U.; da Rocha, J.B.T.; Landeira-Fernandez, J. Association of oxidative stress to the genesis of anxiety: Implications for possible therapeutic interventions. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2014, 12, 120–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Taysi, S.; Tascan, A.S.; Ugur, M.G.; Demir, M. Radicals, oxidative/nitrosative stress and preeclampsia. Mini Rev. Med. Chem. 2019, 19, 178–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Akbar, M.; Essa, M.M.; Daradkeh, G.; Abdelmegeed, M.A.; Choi, Y.; Mahmood, L.; Song, B.J. Mitochondrial dysfunction and cell death in neurodegenerative diseases through nitroxidative stress. Brain Res. 2016, 1637, 34–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Madireddy, S.; Madireddy, S. Regulation of reactive oxygen species-mediated damage in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Joseph, J.A.; Shukitt-Hale, B.; Casadesus, G. Reversing the deleterious effects of aging on neuronal communication and behavior: Beneficial properties of fruit polyphenolic compounds. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2005, 81, 313S–316S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lange, K.W. Red wine, resveratrol, and Alzheimer’s disease. Mov. Nutr. Health Dis. 2018, 2, 31–38. [Google Scholar]
  23. Lange, K.W.; Li, S. Resveratrol, pterostilbene and dementia. BioFactors 2018, 44, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Hamaguchi, T.; Ono, K.; Murase, A.; Yamada, M. Phenolic compounds prevent Alzheimer’s pathology through different effects on the Amyloid-β aggregation pathway. Am. J. Pathol. 2009, 175, 2557–2565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Ono, K.; Hamaguchi, T.; Naiki, H.; Yamada, M. Anti-amyloidogenic effects of antioxidants: Implications for the prevention and therapeutics of Alzheimer’s disease. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 2006, 1762, 575–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Ramassamy, C. Emerging role of polyphenolic compounds in the treatment of neurodegenerative diseases: A review of their intracellular targets. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2006, 545, 51–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Thapa, A.; Carroll, N.J. Dietary modulation of oxidative stress in Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2017, 18, 1583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Romo-Araiza, A.; Ibarra, A. Prebiotics and probiotics as potential therapy for cognitive impairment. Med. Hypotheses 2019, 134, 109410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hemarajata, P.; Versalovic, J. Effects of probiotics on gut microbiota: Mechanisms of intestinal immunomodulation and neuromodulation. Ther. Adv. Gastroenterol. 2013, 6, 39–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Sánchez, B.; Delgado, S.; Blanco-Míguez, A.; Lourenço, A.; Gueimonde, M.; Margolles, A. Probiotics, gut microbiota, and their influence on host health and disease. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2017, 61, 1600240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Cryan, J.F.; Dinan, T.G. Mind-altering microorganisms: The impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behavior. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 13, 701–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Breit, S.; Kupferberg, A.; Rogler, G.; Hasler, G. Vagus nerve as modulator of the brain-gut axis in psychiatric and inflammatory disorders. Front. Psychiatry 2018, 9, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Miki, T.; Eguchi, M.; Kurotani, K.; Kochi, T.; Kuwahara, K.; Ito, R.; Kimura, Y.; Tsuruoka, H.; Akter, S.; Kashino, I.; et al. Dietary fiber intake and depressive symptoms in Japanese employees: The furukawa nutrition and health study. Nutrition 2016, 32, 584–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Ercolini, D.; Fogliano, V. Food design to feed the human gut microbiota. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2018, 66, 3754–3758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Hadizadeh, M.; Hamidi, G.; Salami, M. Probiotic supplementation improves the cognitive function and the anxiety-like behaviors in the stressed rats. Iran. J. Basic Med. Sci. 2019, 22, 506–514. [Google Scholar]
  36. Romo-Araiza, A.; Gutiérrez-Salmeán, G.; Galván, E.J.; Hernández-Frausto, M.; Herrera-López, G.; Romo-Parra, H.; García-Contreras, V.; Fernández-Presas, A.M.; Jasso-Chávez, R.; Borlongan, C.V.; et al. Probiotics and, prebiotics as a therapeutic strategy to improve memory in a model of middle-aged rats. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2018, 10, 416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Bahrami, A.; Bahrami-Taghanaki, H.; Khorasanchi, Z.; Tayefi, M.; Ferns, G.A.; Sadeghnia, H.R.; Ghayour-Mobarhan, M. The association between neuropsychological function with serum vitamins A, D, and E and hs-CRP concentrations. J. Mol. Neurosci. 2019, 68, 243–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gordstein, F.; O’Brien, J.; Kang, J.H.; Dushkes, R.; Cook, N.R.; Okereke, O.; Manson, J.E.; Glynn, R.J.; Buring, J.E.; Gaziano, M.; et al. Long-term multivitamin supplementation and cognitive function in men: A randomized trial. Ann. Intern. Med. 2013, 159, 806–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rutjes, A.W.; Denton, D.A.; Di Nisio, M.; Chong, L.Y.; Abraham, R.P.; Al-Assaf, A.S.; Anderson, J.L.; Malik, M.A.; Vernooij, R.W.; Martínez, G.; et al. Vitamin and mineral supplementation for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in mid and late life. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2018, 12, CD011906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Manders, M.; de Groot, L.C.P.G.M.; van Staveren, W.A.; Wouters-Wesseling, W.; Mulders, A.J.M.J.; Schols, W.J.M.G.A.; Hoefnagels, W.H.L. Effectiveness of nutritional supplements on cognitive functioning in elderly persons: A systematic review. J. Gerontol. Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2004, 59, 1041–1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. McCleery, J.; Abraham, R.P.; Denton, D.A.; Rutjes, A.W.; Chong, L.Y.; Al-Assaf, A.S.; Griffith, D.J.; Rafeeq, S.; Yaman, H.; Malik, M.A.; et al. Vitamin and mineral supplementation for preventing dementia or delaying cognitive decline in people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2018, 11, CD011905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Franceschelli, S.; Pesce, M.; Ferrone, A.; De Lutiis, M.A.; Patruno, A.; Grilli, A.; Felaco, M.; Speranza, L. Astaxanthin treatment confers protection against oxidative stress in U937 cells stimulated with lipopolysaccharide reducing O2-production. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e88359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Olivera-Pueyo, J.; Carmelo Pelegrín-Valero, C. Dietary supplements for cognitive impairment. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 2017, 45, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  44. May, J. Vitamin C transport and its role in the central nervous system. Subcell. Biochem. 2012, 56, 85–103. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Almeida, O.P.; Ford, A.H.; Hirani, V.; Singh, V.; van Bockxmeer, F.M.; McCaul, K. B vitamins to enhance treatment response to antidepressants in middle-aged and older adults: Results from the B-VITAGE randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 2014, 205, 450–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Clarke, R.; Bennett, D.; Parish, S.; Lewington, S.; Skeaff, M.; Eussen, S.J.P.M.; Lewerin, C.; Stott, D.J.; Armitage, J.; Hankey, G.J.; et al. Effects of homocysteine lowering with B vitamins on cognitive aging: Meta-analysis of 11 trials with cognitive data on 22,000 individuals. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 100, 657–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Smith, A.D.; Refsum, H. Homocysteine, B vitamins, and cognitive impairment. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2016, 36, 211–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Venkatramanan, S.; Armata, I.E.; Strupp, B.J.; Finkelstein, J.L. Vitamin B-12 and cognition in children. Adv. Nutr. 2016, 7, 879–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Suh, S.W.; Kim, H.S.; Han, J.H.; Bae, J.B.; Oh, D.J.; Han, J.W.; Kim, K.W. Efficacy of vitamins on cognitive function of non-demented people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Maddock, J.; Zhou, A.; Cavadino, A.; Kuźma, E.; Bao, Y.; Smart, M.; Saum, K.; Schöttker, B.; Engmann, J.; Kjærgaard, M.; et al. Vitamin D and cognitive function: A mendelian randomisation study. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 13230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Mokry, L.E.; Ross, S.; Morris, J.A.; Manousaki, D.; Forgetta, V.; Richards, J.B. Genetically decreased vitamin D and risk of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2016, 87, 2567–2574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Pettersen, J.A. Does high dose vitamin D supplementation enhance cognition? A randomized trial in healthy adults. Exp. Gerontol. 2017, 90, 90–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Farghali, M.; Ruga, S.; Morsanuto, V.; Uberti, F. Can Brain Health Be Supported by Vitamin D-Based Supplements? A Critical Review. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Alex, A.; Abbott, K.A.; McEvoy, M.; Schofield, P.W.; Garg, M.L. Long-chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and cognitive decline in non-demented adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr. Rev. 2020, 78, 563–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Madireddy, S.; Madireddy, S. The role of diet in maintaining strong brain health by taking the advantage of the gut-brain axis. In Advances in Food Science, 1st ed.; Hien, P.P., Ed.; Vide Leaf: Hyderabad, India, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  56. Birberg-Thornberg, U.; Karlsson, T.; Gustafsson, P.A.; Duche, K. Nutrition and theory of mind—The role of polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA) in the development of theory of mind. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2006, 75, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Kuszewski, J.C.; Wong, R.H.X.; Howe, P.R.C. Effects of long-chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids on endothelial vasodilator function and cognition—Are they interrelated? Nutrients 2017, 9, 487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Baym, C.L.; Khan, N.A.; Monti, J.M.; Raine, L.B.; Drollette, E.S.; Moore, R.D.; Scudder, M.R.; Kramer, A.F.; Hillman, C.H.; Cohen, N.J. Dietary lipids are differentially associated with hippocampal-dependent relational memory in prepubescent children. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 99, 1026–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Agrawal, R.; Gomez-Pinilla, F. ‘Metabolic syndrome’ in the brain: Deficiency in omega-3 fatty acid exacerbates dysfunctions in insulin receptor signalling and cognition. J. Physiol. 2012, 590, 2485–2499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Bhatia, H.S.; Agrawal, R.; Sharma, S.; Huo, Y.-X.; Ying, Z.; Gomez-Pinilla, F. Omega-3 fatty acid deficiency during brain maturation reduces neuronal and behavioral plasticity in adulthood. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e28451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Gómez-Pinilla, F. Brain foods: The effects of nutrients on brain function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 568–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Innis, S.M. Dietary (n23) fatty acids and brain development. J. Nutr. 2007, 137, 855–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Ciappolino, V.; Mazzocchi, A.; Enrico, P.; Syrén, M.L.; Delvecchio, G.; Agostoni, C.; Brambilla, P. N-3 polyunsatured fatty acids in menopausal transition: A systematic review of depressive and cognitive disorders with accompanying vasomotor symptoms. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2018, 19, 1849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Mudd, A.T.; Fil, J.E.; Knight, L.C.; Dilger, R.N. Dietary iron repletion following early-life dietary iron deficiency does not correct regional volumetric or diffusion tensor changes in the developing pig brain. Front. Neurol. 2018, 8, 735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Gow, R.V.; Hibbeln, J.R. Omega-3 fatty acid and nutrient deficits in adverse neurodevelopment and childhood behaviors. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. 2014, 23, 555–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Hussain, G.; Schmitt, F.; Loeffler, J.P.; Gonzalez de Aguilar, J.L. Fatting the brain: A brief of recent research. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Sidhu, V.K.; Huang, B.X.; Desai, A.; Kevala, K.; Kim, H.Y. Role of DHA in aging-related changes in mouse brain synaptic plasma membrane proteome. Neurobiol. Aging 2016, 41, 73–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Qi, Z.; Liu, K.J. The interaction of zinc and the blood-brain barrier under physiological and ischemic conditions. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 2019, 364, 114–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Grabrucker, A.M.; Knight, M.J.; Proepper, C.; Bockmann, J.; Joubert, M.; Rowan, M.; Nienhaus, G.U.; Garner, C.C.; Bowie, J.U.; Kreutz, M.R.; et al. Concerted action of zinc and ProSAP/Shank in synaptogenesis and synapse maturation. EMBO J. 2011, 30, 569–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Jung, A.; Spira, D.; Steinhagen-Thiessen, E.; Demuth, I.; Norman, K. Zinc deficiency is associated with depressive symptoms-results from the Berlin aging study II. J. Gerontol. Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2017, 72, 1149–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Vela, G.; Stark, P.; Socha, M.; Sauer, A.K.; Hagmeyer, S.; Grabrucker, A.M. Zinc in gut-brain interaction in autism and neurological disorders. Neural Plast. 2015, 972791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Takeda, A.; Nakamura, M.; Fujii, H.; Tamano, H. Synaptic Zn2+ homeostasis and its significance. Metallomics 2013, 5, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. De Moura, J.E.; de Moura, E.N.; Alves, C.X.; Vale, S.H.; Dantas, M.M.; Silva, A.D.; Almeida, M.G.; Leite, L.D.; Brandão-Neto, J. Oral zinc supplementation may improve cognitive function in schoolchildren. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 2013, 155, 23–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Takeda, A.; Tamano, H. Significance of the degree of synaptic Zn2+ signaling in cognition. Biometals 2016, 29, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Tao, L.; Zheng, Y.; Shen, Z.; Li, Y.; Tian, X.; Dou, X.; Qian, J.; Shen, H. Psychological stress-induced lower serum zinc and zinc redistribution in rats. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 2013, 155, 65–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Sandusky-Beltran, L.A.; Manchester, B.L.; McNay, E.C. Supplementation with zinc in rats enhances memory and reverses an age-dependent increase in plasma copper. Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 333, 179–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Yang, Y.; Jing, X.-P.; Zhang, S.-P.; Gu, R.-X.; Tang, F.-X.; Wang, X.-L.; Xiong, Y.; Qiu, M.; Sun, X.-Y.; Ke, D.; et al. High dose zinc supplementation induces hippocampal zinc deficiency and memory impairment with inhibition of BDNF signaling. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e55384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Tamano, H.; Koike, Y.; Nakada, H.; Shakushi, Y.; Takeda, A. Significance of synaptic Zn2+ signaling in zincergic and non-zincergic synapses in the hippocampus in cognition. J. Trace Elem. Med. Biol. 2016, 38, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Burns, J.G.; Foucaud, J.; Mery, F. Costs of memory: Lessons from ‘mini’ brains. Proc. R. Soc. B 2011, 278, 923–929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Mallory, H.S.; Howard, A.F.; Weiss, M.R. Timing of Environmental Enrichment Affects Memory in the House Cricket, Acheta domesticus. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0152245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Ming, G.; Song, H. Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian brain: Significant answers and significant questions. Neuron 2011, 70, 687–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Malaterre, J.; Strambi, C.; Chiang, A.; Aouane, A.; Strambi, A.; Cayre, M. Development of cricket mushroom bodies. J. Comp. Neurol. 2002, 452, 215–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Scotto-Lomassese, S.; Strambi, C.; Aouane, A.; Strambi, A.; Cayre, M. Sensory inputs stimulate progenitor cell proliferation in an adult insect brain. Curr. Biol. 2002, 12, 1001–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Cayre, M.; Malaterre, J.; Scotto-Lomassese, S.; Strambi, C.; Strambi, A. The common properties of neurogenesis in the adult brain: From invertebrates to vertebrates. Comp. Biochem. Phys. B 2002, 132, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Matsumoto, Y.; Mizunami, M. Lifetime olfactory memory in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2002, 188, 295–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Córdoba-Aguilar, A.; Nava-Sánchez, A.; González-Tokman, D.M.; Munguía-Steyer, R.; Gutiérrez-Cabrera, A.E. Immune priming, fat reserves, muscle mass and body weight of the house cricket is affected by diet composition. Neotrop. Entomol. 2016, 45, 404–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Rapkin, J.; Jensen, K.; Lane, S.M.; House, C.M.; Sakaluk, S.K. Macronutrient intake regulates sexual conflict in decorated crickets. J. Evol. Biol. 2016, 29, 395–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Erregger, B.; Kovac, H.; Stabentheiner, A.; Hartbauer, M.; Römer, H.; Schmidt, A.K.D. Cranking up the heat: Relationships between energetically costly song features and the increase in thorax temperature in male crickets and katydids. J. Exp. Biol. 2017, 220, 2635–2644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  89. Gabel, E.; Gray, D.A.; Hennig, R.M. How females of chirping and trilling field crickets integrate the ‘what’ and ‘where’ of male acoustic signals during decision making. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 2016, 202, 823–837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Adamo, S.A.; Kovalko, I.; Mosher, B. The behavioural effects of predator-induced stress responses in the cricket (Gryllus texensis): The upside of the stress response. J. Exp. Biol. 2013, 216, 4608–4614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Erregger, B.; Hennig, R.M.; Römer, H. The ‘hot male’ hypothesis: Do female crickets prefer males with increased body temperature in mate choice scenarios? Anim. Behav. 2018, 138, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Boroujeni, S.T.; Naghdi, N.; Shahbazi, M.; Bagherzadeh, F.; Kazemnejad, A.; Javadian, M. The effect of severe zinc deficiency and zinc supplement on spatial learning and memory. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 2009, 130, 48–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Zhang, S.Q.; Zhang, H.B.; Cheng, Q.; Zhu, Y.M.; Xia, C.H.; Zhu, Y.H.; Zhang, Y. Zinc-enriched yeast improves learning and memory impairments in zinc-deficient rats. Biol. Trace Elem. Res. 2019, 189, 180–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Rico, J.A.; Kordas, K.; López, P.; Rosado, J.L.; Vargas, G.G.; Ronquillo, D.; Stoltzfus, R.J. Efficacy of iron and/or zinc supplementation on cognitive performance of lead-exposed Mexican schoolchildren: A randomized, placebo-Controlled Trial. Pediatrics 2006, 117, e518–e527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Arzi, A.; Hemmati, A.A.; Razian, A. Effect of vitamins C and E on cognitive function in mouse. Pharmacol. Res. 2004, 49, 249–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Lee, H.K.; Kim, S.Y.; Sok, S.R. Effects of multivitamin supplements on cognitive function, serum homocysteine level, and depression of Korean older adults with mild cognitive impairment in care facilities. J. Nurs. Scholarsh. 2016, 48, 223–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Cheng, D.; Kong, H.; Pang, W.; Yang, H.; Lu, H.; Huang, C.; Jiang, Y. B vitamin supplementation improves cognitive function in the middle aged and elderly with hyperhomocysteinemia. Nutr. Neurosci. 2016, 19, 461–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Madireddy, S. A winning combination among polyphenols, probiotics, vitamins for improved memory and cognitive performance. Int. J. Med. Res. Health Sci. 2020, 9, 27–31. [Google Scholar]
  99. Privitera, G.J.; Zavala, A.R.; Federico, S.; Sotak, K.L. High fat diet intake during pre and periadolescence impairs learning of a conditioned place preference in adulthood. Behav. Brain Funct. 2011, 7, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Hajjar, T.; Meng, G.Y.; Rajion, M.A.; Vidyadaran, S.; Othman, F.; Farjam, A.S.; Li, T.A.; Ebrahimi, M. Omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid improves spatial learning and hippocampal peroxisome proliferator activated receptors (PPARα and PPARγ) gene expression in rats. BMC Neurosci. 2012, 13, 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Hooijmans, C.R.; Kiliaan, A.J. Fatty acids, lipid metabolism and Alzheimer pathology. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2008, 585, 176–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Lapillonne, A.; Clarke, S.D.; Heird, W.C. Polyunsaturated fatty acids and gene expression. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 2004, 7, 151–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Su, H.M. Mechanisms of n-3 fatty acid-mediated development and maintenance of learning memory performance. J. Nutr. Biochem. 2010, 21, 364–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Mizunami, M.; Matsumoto, Y.; Watanabe, H.; Nishino, H. Chapter 41: Olfactory and visual learning in cockroaches and crickets. In Invertebrate Learning and Memory; Menzel, R., Benjamin, P.R., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2013; pp. 547–558. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Y-maze.
Figure 1. Y-maze.
Nutrients 13 00362 g001
Figure 2. Alternation Test.
Figure 2. Alternation Test.
Nutrients 13 00362 g002
Figure 3. Recognition Memory Test.
Figure 3. Recognition Memory Test.
Nutrients 13 00362 g003
Figure 4. Path bias and solution to overcome path bias.
Figure 4. Path bias and solution to overcome path bias.
Nutrients 13 00362 g004
Figure 5. The effect of diet on performance in Recognition Memory Tests over time. Graphed is the mean performance of each group during each testing period with the standard error of the mean (SEM). All groups experienced improvement in performance over time.
Figure 5. The effect of diet on performance in Recognition Memory Tests over time. Graphed is the mean performance of each group during each testing period with the standard error of the mean (SEM). All groups experienced improvement in performance over time.
Nutrients 13 00362 g005
Figure 6. The effect of diet on performance in Alternation Tests over time. Graphed is the mean performance of each group during each testing period with the SEM. All groups experienced significant improvement in performance over time.
Figure 6. The effect of diet on performance in Alternation Tests over time. Graphed is the mean performance of each group during each testing period with the SEM. All groups experienced significant improvement in performance over time.
Nutrients 13 00362 g006
Figure 7. Improvement in performance in Recognition Memory Tests over time. Graphed is the mean improvement of each group with the SEM, calculated by final #-initial#.
Figure 7. Improvement in performance in Recognition Memory Tests over time. Graphed is the mean improvement of each group with the SEM, calculated by final #-initial#.
Nutrients 13 00362 g007
Figure 8. Improvement in performance in Alternation Tests over time. Graphed is the mean improvement of each group with the SEM, calculated by final #-initial#.
Figure 8. Improvement in performance in Alternation Tests over time. Graphed is the mean improvement of each group with the SEM, calculated by final #-initial#.
Nutrients 13 00362 g008
Figure 9. Baseline and final measures of performance in Recognition Memory Tests. Graphed are the mean number of correct arm/leg choices of each group at 0 days and 48 days since starting the specific diets. All groups exhibited improvements in performance.
Figure 9. Baseline and final measures of performance in Recognition Memory Tests. Graphed are the mean number of correct arm/leg choices of each group at 0 days and 48 days since starting the specific diets. All groups exhibited improvements in performance.
Nutrients 13 00362 g009
Figure 10. Baseline and final measures of performance in Alternation Tests. Graphed are the mean number of successful alternations of each group at 0 days and 48 days since starting the specific diets. All groups exhibited improvements in performance.
Figure 10. Baseline and final measures of performance in Alternation Tests. Graphed are the mean number of successful alternations of each group at 0 days and 48 days since starting the specific diets. All groups exhibited improvements in performance.
Nutrients 13 00362 g010
Figure 11. Recognition Memory Test: Regression. Graphed are the mean values for each group’s performance during each testing period with the SEM, as well as the line generated through regression analysis.
Figure 11. Recognition Memory Test: Regression. Graphed are the mean values for each group’s performance during each testing period with the SEM, as well as the line generated through regression analysis.
Nutrients 13 00362 g011
Figure 12. Alternation Test: Regression. Graphed are the mean values for each group’s performance during each testing period with the SEM, as well as the line generated through regression analysis.
Figure 12. Alternation Test: Regression. Graphed are the mean values for each group’s performance during each testing period with the SEM, as well as the line generated through regression analysis.
Nutrients 13 00362 g012
Table 1. Groups.
Table 1. Groups.
GroupGroup LabelDiet
Group 1ControlNormal diet
Group 2ProProbiotics
Group 3PolPolyphenols
Group 4VitMultivitamins
Group 5ZncZinc
Group 6PufOmega-3 PUFAs
Group 7ProPolProbiotics and Polyphenols
Group 8PolVitPolyphenols and Multivitamins
Group 9VitZncMultivitamins and Zinc
Group 10ZncPufZinc and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 11PufProOmega-3 PUFAs and Probiotics
Group 12ProVitProbiotics and Multivitamins
Group 13ProZncProbiotics and Zinc
Group 14PolZncPolyphenols and Zinc
Group 15PolPufPolyphenols and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 16VitPufMultivitamins and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 17ProPolVitProbiotics, Polyphenols, and Multivitamins
Group 18PolVitZncPolyphenols, Multivitamins, and Zinc
Group 19VitZncPufMultivitamins, Zinc, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 20ZncPufProZinc, Omega-3 PUFAs, Probiotics
Group 21ProPolZncProbiotics, Polyphenols, and Zinc
Group 22ProPolPufProbiotics, Polyphenols, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 23ProVitZncProbiotics, Multivitamins, and Zinc
Group 24ProVitPufProbiotics, Multivitamins, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 25PolVitPufPolyphenols, Multivitamins, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 26PolZncPufPolyphenols, Zinc, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 27ProPolVitZncProbiotics, Polyphenols, Multivitamins, and Zinc
Group 28ProPolVitPufProbiotics, Polyphenols, Multivitamins, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 29ProPolZncPufProbiotics, Polyphenols, Zinc, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 30ProVitZncPufProbiotics, Multivitamins, Zinc, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 31PolVitZncPufPolyphenols, Multivitamins, Zinc, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Group 32ProPolVitZncPufProbiotics, Polyphenols, Multivitamins, Zinc, and Omega-3 PUFAs
Table 2. The ranking of diets in order of their slope for the (a) Alternation Test and (b) Recognition Memory Test.
Table 2. The ranking of diets in order of their slope for the (a) Alternation Test and (b) Recognition Memory Test.
(a)(b)
RankingCombinationRankingCombination
1VitZncPuf1VitZncPuf
ProVitZncPuf
ProPolVitZncPuf
2ProPolVitZncPuf2PolVitZncPuf
3ProVitZncPuf3ProPolZncPuf
4PolVitZncPuf4ProPolZnc
5PolZncPuf5PolZncPuf
6ProPolZncPuf6ProPolVitPuf
7PolVitZnc7ProPolVitZnc
8ProPolZnc8ZncPufPro
9ProVitZnc9PolVitZnc
10ProPolVitZnc10ProVitZnc
11ZncPufPro11ProVitPuf
12ProPolVitPuf12PolVitPuf
13ProVitPuf13ProPolPuf
14PolVitPuf14VitPuf
15VitPuf15ProPolVit
16ProZnc16VitZnc
17ProPolVit17PolZnc
18PolZnc18ZncPuf
19ProPolPuf19PolPuf
20VitZnc20ProZnc
21PufPro21Puf
22ZncPuf22PufPro
23ProVit23PolVit
24Znc24Znc
25PolVit25ProVit
26PolPuf26ProPol
27Vit27Vit
28Puf28Pol
29Pol29Pro
30ProPol30Control
31Pro
32Control
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Madireddy, S.; Madireddy, S. Most Effective Combination of Nutraceuticals for Improved Memory and Cognitive Performance in the House Cricket, Acheta domesticus. Nutrients 2021, 13, 362. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/nu13020362

AMA Style

Madireddy S, Madireddy S. Most Effective Combination of Nutraceuticals for Improved Memory and Cognitive Performance in the House Cricket, Acheta domesticus. Nutrients. 2021; 13(2):362. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/nu13020362

Chicago/Turabian Style

Madireddy, Samskruthi, and Sahithi Madireddy. 2021. "Most Effective Combination of Nutraceuticals for Improved Memory and Cognitive Performance in the House Cricket, Acheta domesticus" Nutrients 13, no. 2: 362. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/nu13020362

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop